We spend about one third of our lives asleep (Siegel, 2005). Sufficient and restorative sleep is considered a cornerstone of both mental and physical health (Siegel, 2005). Yet, sleep problems such as difficulty falling and remaining asleep are highly prevalent in adult populations (Buysse et al., 1989; Ohayon, 2002; Léger et al., 2000).

Research has consistently shown that disturbed sleep can have detrimental effects on physical health, psycho-social well-being and psychological functioning (Roth, 2007). For example, disturbed sleep has been associated with heart disease (Kanagala et al., 2003) and an increased risk of developing neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease (Burke et al., 2018). Disturbed sleep can also negatively affect cognitive functioning (Diekelmann, 2014), as well as academic performance (Curcio et al., 2006) and quality of life more generally (Léger et al., 2001; Ohayon et al., 1997).

Good sleep quality, on the other hand, has positive effects on daily emotional functioning and reduced stress reactivity (Blaxton et al., 2017). Thus, it is important to determine potential modifiable risk factors of poor sleep quality so that negative health outcomes may be prevented.

Particularly young adults and students seem to be at risk of developing sleep problems (Buboltz Jr et al., 2009). Longitudinal research has shown that the transition from adolescence to adulthood is marked by several social and biological changes that may affect sleep quality. Young adults typically do not continue to execute the sleep habits acquired during childhood or adolescence (Park et al., 2019). The new lifestyle challenges young adults are faced with can lead to the development of sleep difficulties in this group. Interventions aimed at increasing sleep quality in this group would therefore need to consider these particular challenges.

The chronotype is an important factor to take into account when assessing behavioral predictors of sleep quality (Burke et al., 2015). Chronotype refers to a person’s preferred activity and sleep schedules and are governed by internal circadian rhythm timings (Kerkhof, 1985). A preference for late activity and late sleep onset is described as an evening chronotype. Throughout the lifespan circadian timings change from an early preference in childhood to a late preference during adolescence, peaking around the age of 20. Hereafter, another transition toward the earlier chronotype occurs, which has been described as a marker for the end of adolescence (Roenneberg et al., 2004). The evening chronotype has been associated with poorer sleep quality (Vitale et al., 2015). Furthermore, evening types have an increased risk of experiencing depressive symptoms (Kivelä et al., 2018), and this relationship was mediated by sleep quality in a student sample (Van den Berg et al., 2018).

In terms of psychological health, young adulthood in particular represents a vulnerable time for the onset of psychiatric disorders (Kessler et al., 2005). Research suggests complex bidirectional relations between poor sleep and mood disorders (Ohayon & Roth, 2003). Sleep disturbances can be a clinically valuable predictive sign in depression, as they tend to precede the onset of a depressive episode or relapse (Alvaro et al., 2013; Baglioni et al., 2011; Ford & Kamerow, 1989; Ohayon & Roth, 2003). However, some research also suggests a reverse relationship in that depressive symptoms predict worse sleep quality (Kennedy et al., 1991). In a student sample, poor sleep quality was associated with higher self-reported negative mood, and anxiety and stress were the strongest predictors of subjective sleep quality (Lund et al., 2010). This line of work indicates that mood and cognitive reactions to stress are likely to play an important role in the complex psychological interactions that affect sleep quality. Young adulthood is a period in which cognitive regulation skills develop. Imaging studies comparing children, adolescents and young adults show linear relationships between age and the activation of prefrontal cortex regions required for complex emotion regulation involving cognitive control (McRae et al., 2012). One recent development in sleep research is to assess the relationships between cognitive emotion regulation skills, psychological health and sleep parameters (Watts & Norbury, 2017).

An important modifiable lifestyle risk factor for students is the increased access to and use of stimulants (e.g., caffeine) and sedatives (e.g., alcohol, cannabis), which may influence sleep parameters (Lund et al., 2010). In particular, caffeine is the most commonly used psychoactive substance worldwide (Clark & Landolt, 2017). A recent systematic review of epidemiologic studies and clinical trials concluded that caffeine intake negatively impacts subjective and objective sleep quality (Clark & Landolt, 2017). Specifically, evening caffeine intake equivalent of a double espresso was shown to induce a circadian phase delay of 40 min (Burke et al., 2015).

Only few studies have comprehensively assessed the behavioral, psychological and cognitive predictors of sleep quality in students. This is important as young adulthood is a sensitive time for experiencing poor sleep quality (Lund et al., 2010), and is characterized by several developmental transitions: changes in chronotype, increased vulnerability for affective disorders, biological developments in cognitive regulation capabilities as well as more access to substances. We aim to examine how the combination of these factors affects subjective sleep quality in this phase. Individually some of these variables have been studied previously, however, not in a combined model, which we expect to represent the transition from adolescence to adulthood more accurately. Further novel elements of this study are that we considered cognitive emotion regulation as a potential risk factor, as well as non-problematic levels of substance use. To our knowledge no other studies have assessed the combination of these factors before. Specifically, we hypothesized that a later chronotype, more depressive symptoms, increased use of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies, and more substance use (caffeine, alcohol, smoking, cannabis) would be associated with lower subjective sleep quality.

Method

Participants and Procedure

A total of 1114 Dutch-speaking University students (77.8% female; 22.2% male) from Leiden, The Hague, Delft, Rotterdam and Amsterdam participated in this cross-sectional study. Inclusion criteria were: being enrolled as a student (full-time or part-time), being ≥18 years of age and being fluent in Dutch. Participants were between 17 and 56 years old, with an average age of 21.1 (±2.9) years. Data were collected in March of 2016, 2017 and 2018. Each year data collection ended as daylight saving time began, so that difficulties with adjusting to daylight saving time would not influence the data. Students were recruited with flyers distributed throughout University buildings and through the social networks of the students who collected the data. Participants were provided a link through which they could read information about the study, sign an online informed consent form and complete questionnaires. Participation was rewarded with a lottery to win small monetary amounts ≤ €50. The study protocol and informed consent were approved by the Institutional Review Board, and all subjects gave informed consent.

Measures

The socio-demographic variables age, gender and education level were recorded.

Sleep Quality

Subjective sleep quality was assessed with a Dutch translation of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysse et al., 1989). The PSQI is a 19-item questionnaire assessing seven components of sleep quality (subjective sleep quality; sleep latency; sleep duration; habitual sleep efficiency; sleep disturbances; sleep medication use; and daytime functioning) over the last month. The combined scores range from 0 (no difficulty) to 21 (severe difficulties) and higher scores indicate worse sleep quality. A total PSQI score of >5 indicates clinically significant sleep quality problems (Buysse et al., 1989). Based on the seven PSQI components it has an internal consistency score of Cronbach’s ɑ = 0.66.

Chronotype

We assessed chronotype using the Morningness/Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ) (Horne & Östberg, 1976). It is a widely used instrument consisting of 19 items assessing preferred daily activity times and preferred sleep and wake times. Total scores range from 18 to 86, where low scores (<42) indicate eveningness and higher scores (>58) indicate morningness. The MEQ has a high internal consistency in our sample with Cronbach’s ɑ = 0.87.

Depressive Symptoms

The Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology- Self-Rated (QIDS-SR; Rush et al., 2003) was used to assess depressive symptoms. This 16-item self-report instrument measures the severity and frequency of depressive symptoms based on the nine DSM-IV depression domains (sad mood; concentration; self-criticism; suicidal ideation; interest; energy/fatigue; sleep disturbances; changes in appetite/weight; psychomotor agitation/retardation) over the last week. Total scores range between 0 (no depressive symptoms) to 27 (severe depressive symptoms). In our sample the QIDS-SR has a Cronbach’s ɑ = 0.78.

Cognitive Emotion Regulation

Emotion regulation was assessed using the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ; Garnefski & Kraaij, 2007). It consists of 36 questions measuring nine subscales, namely ‘self-blame’, ‘acceptance’, ‘rumination’, ‘positive refocusing’, ‘refocus on planning’, ‘positive reappraisal’, ‘putting into perspective’, ‘catastrophizing’, and ‘blaming others’. Respondents indicate on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always) how often they use each cognitive style in response to a negative event. Scores for each subscale range between 4 and 20 and higher scores indicate more frequent use of the corresponding emotion regulation strategy. In our sample, the CERQ has high internal consistency with Cronbach’s ɑ levels ranging between 0.76 and 0.83 for all subscales.

Substance Use

We assessed average weekly alcohol and caffeine consumption, smoking cigarettes and cannabis use over the past month with a 7-item questionnaire. The amount of caffeine intake (in mg) was calculated based on the average number of caffeinated beverages (coffee, tea, and caffeinated soft drinks) per week, using guidelines from the Dutch Nutrition Centre (Voedingscentrum, 2019). Alcohol use was measured as average number of alcoholic beverages per week. Smoking and cannabis use were dichotomized (yes/no).

Data Analyses

To assess the possible predictors of subjective sleep quality (PSQI total score) we performed a multiple regression analysis where sum scores of the continuous variables (MEQ, QIDS-SR, all CERQ subscales, caffeine consumption, alcohol consumption), the categorical variables (smoking, cannabis use), and gender and age were entered in one step. The sleep disturbance component (4 items) in the QIDS-SR was excluded from the sum score because it represents a component of the PSQI outcome measure and might artificially inflate the regression coefficient for depressive symptoms.

Regression Model Validation

In order to produce a more robust analysis, we used the bootstrap method on the whole sample to calculate the regression coefficients (significance level at p = .05). Additionally, to assess overfitting, we employed a validation procedure on a hold-out sample. We estimated the regression model on a training sample (random selection of 80% of the sample) and then tested the model in the hold-out sample (remaining 20% of the sample). Based on the model estimated on the training sample, predicted scores (on the outcome variable) were calculated for the hold-out sample and compared to the observed values. The model was considered to be robust (good predictive value), if the standard errors of the estimate remained constant in the training sample and the hold-out sample.

Missing Data

Initially 1393 participants started filling in the questionnaire. We removed cases who were not students (N = 55) and cases who only filled in the demographic data or only filled in the first questionnaire (MEQ) in the set (N = 73) and those who missed all components of the outcome variable PSQI (N = 35). There were also participants (N = 116) who did not fill in any items of the last questionnaire in the set (CERQ), who were removed. This rendered the final analysis data set of N = 1114 with no missing values. For participants who missed only one item in the sleep efficiency component (bed times, get up times, habitual sleep duration) of the PSQI (N = 4), we assumed the best score for this component (a sleep efficiency of >85%). Similarly we assumed the best item score (0) for participants who missed one of the two day-time functioning component items of the PSQI (N = 3).

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Table 1 summarizes the descriptive statistics of all variables. The mean PSQI score for the sample was M = 5.6 (±2.8), indicating clinically relevant poor sleep quality, and 42.8% of participants scored above this threshold.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for all variables (N = 1114)

We conducted a multiple linear regression analysis with bootstrap (requesting bias corrected confidence intervals) entering all variables in one step to predict the total PSQI score (Table 2). Among the predictors there were no correlations that exceeded a value of .7, indicating that they are not multicollinear. A complete correlation matrix can be found as online supplementary material. The model was significant, F(17, 1114) = 25.77, p = .001. The amount of explained variance was 28.6% (R2 = 0.286), which indicates a large effect size (Cohen’s f2 = 0.4). More depressive symptoms (higher scores on QIDS-SR) were associated with poorer subjective sleep quality (higher PSQI scores), B = .31, p = .001, 95% CI = .27, .36. An earlier chronotype was associated with better sleep quality, B = −.05, p = .001, 95% CI = −.06, −.03. Higher scores on the ‘putting into perspective’ CERQ subscale were associated with better subjective sleep quality, B = −.06, p = .02, 95% CI = −.11, −.01. Smoking cigarettes was associated with poorer subjective sleep quality, B = .53, p = .02, 95% CI = .10, .98.

Table 2 Regression 1 - Bootstrapped coefficients with PSQI total score as outcome

Validation

A multiple regression was run on a random 80% training sample (N = 881). The model was significant, F(17, 863) = 19.78, p = .001. The amount of explained variance for the model was also 28.0% (R2 = 0.28) rendering a large effect size (Cohen’s f2 = 0.39). Similar to the bootstrapped coefficients in whole sample, the predictors that contributed to the model in the training sample were: MEQ (B = −.05, p = .001, 95% CI = −.07, −.03), QIDS (B = .31, p = .001, 95% CI = .27, .36), and smoking cigarettes (B = .76, p = .001, 95% CI = .32, 1.20). Notably, also alcohol consumption (B = −.03, p = .03, 95% CI = −.06, −.003) was a significant predictor of subjective sleep quality, whereas ‘putting into perspective’ was not (B = −.03, p = .24, 95% CI = −.09, .02). This model was then used to calculate predicted PSQI scores for the hold-out sample (N = 233). The correlation between predicted and observed values in the training sample was r = .530, while in the hold-out sample it was r = .531, indicating that the model predicts PSQI scores almost equally well in the training and the test sample. We further calculated the standard error of the estimate for the hold-out sample (SE = 2.3) to compare it to the one obtained from the training sample (SE = 2.4). Since both values are close to each other we conclude that the results are stable in an unselected group.

Discussion

This large cross-sectional study aimed to assess how several important lifestyle and psychological factors might contribute to deteriorating sleep quality in young adulthood. Our analysis showed that chronotype (more eveningness), depressive symptoms, less use of the cognitive emotion regulation strategy ‘putting into perspective’, and smoking were significant predictors of poor subjective sleep quality, controlling for age and gender. Together these variables explained 29% of the variance in subjective sleep quality. Caffeine consumption, cannabis use and the other eight cognitive emotion regulation styles were not significantly associated with subjective sleep quality. In the validation procedure alcohol consumption was also significantly associated with better subjective sleep quality, whereas ‘putting into perspective’ was not.

Chronotype

A later chronotype was the second largest predictor of subjective sleep quality. Our finding is in line with existing literature on chronotypes and sleep quality. Nearly 28% of the sample were evening types; given that the peak in eveningness across the lifespan occurs around the age of 20 (Roenneberg et al., 2004), and the average age in our sample was 21, our sample represents a fair estimate of evening types in this population. We suspect that a large proportion of the sample is yet to transition toward an earlier chronotype and is currently more vulnerable to sleep quality problems. Evening types were shown to have reduced sleep quality during the week as compared to morning and intermediate types (Vitale et al., 2015; Wittmann et al., 2006). This may be explained by evening types having to adhere to social and professional schedules that are misaligned with their chronotype and only being able to achieve comparable levels of sleep quality as earlier types during the weekend (Vitale et al., 2015). It is possible that the same explanation holds for our student sample, in that evening types also adhere to their study and social schedule during the week, which may be misaligned to their circadian rhythms.

Depressive Symptoms

The strongest predictor of poorer subjective sleep quality (adjusted for the other variables) was depressive symptoms. Our finding supports existing research on the relationship between depression and poor sleep quality (Kennedy et al., 1991; Ohayon & Roth, 2003). Other epidemiological studies conclude that somatic or emotional distress presents the best predictor of insomnia in adults (Ford & Kamerow, 1989). In a sample closer to our own, a large study of college students found that emotional distress was the largest predictor of poor sleep quality (Lund et al., 2010). In open-ended questions, a large proportion of students in this study named thought patterns typical for depressive symptoms (such as worries about the future and school) as the reason for their difficulties with sleep (Lund et al., 2010).

Since our study was cross-sectional, we cannot establish temporal precedence and the term ‘predictor’ in this context is strictly statistical. Nevertheless, the strong association between depressive symptoms and sleep are further supported by research showing that 90% of patients with depression present with sleep disturbances (Franzen & Buysse, 2008) and that untreated sleep problems increase the likelihood of relapse in depression (Ford & Kamerow, 1989; Franzen & Buysse, 2008; Ohayon & Roth, 2003). One possible explanation is that depression and sleep parameters depend on shared regulatory neurotransmitter pathways (Wulff et al., 2010), which may support the idea that treating depressive symptoms and sleep problems should go hand in hand (Cunningham & Shapiro, 2018).

Cognitive Emotion Regulation

Based on theoretical models of emotion regulation (Baglioni et al., 2010), we expected that disturbances in emotion regulation typically seen in the context of heightened cognitive arousal, such as rumination and worry, would be related to sleep quality. Particularly, rumination has been shown to increase pre-sleep arousal and predict reduced sleep quality in young adults with depressive symptoms (Pillai et al., 2014) and healthy students (Takano et al., 2012). Longitudinal research in students further showed that problems with emotion regulation relate bi-directionally to sleep problems (Tavernier & Willoughby, 2015).

Eight of nine emotion regulation styles assessed in this study did not predict sleep quality significantly. Only less use of ‘putting into perspective’ was associated with poorer sleep quality. However, this variable was not significant in the validation procedure. A potential explanation could be that in order to observe the effects of daily emotion regulation, sleep quality needs to be assessed the same night. Perhaps momentary measurements that are sensitive to fluctuations in stress-responsiveness, cognitive arousal and same-night sleep quality could provide a useful approach to studying cognitive-emotional predictors of sleep quality in the future (see: Gilbert et al., 2017 for an example in diurnal cortisol functioning). Another distinction that we did not make and could help explain our finding is that effective emotion regulation may depend on coping style flexibility. For instance one emotion regulation style may be adaptive in response to a particular stressor, but not in response to another (Kahn et al., 2013). Another possible explanation for the results may be that emotion regulation plays a more important role for a sub-group of the sample, such as those individuals with elevated symptoms of depression or those who have already developed the cognitive capabilities to use complex emotion regulation strategies (McRae et al., 2012). Because we controlled for depressive symptoms in the regression analysis, emotion regulation styles may not have emerged as independent predictors of sleep quality.

Substance Use

In our study, caffeine was not a significant predictor of sleep quality. This is somewhat surprising as caffeine is a well-studied substance known to affect wakefulness and sleep (Clark & Landolt, 2017). In our sample caffeine consumption was low in general, which may help to explain this result. Another explanation could be that people moderate their caffeine intake according to their own levels of caffeine sensitivity (Rétey et al., 2007).

Alcohol was not a significant predictor in our main analysis. Yet, it was a significant predictor of better sleep quality in our validation procedure. Although this result requires replication before firm conclusions can be drawn, the finding is in accordance with other mixed results about the effects of alcohol in different dosages on health and sleep. A review on the effects of alcohol on objective sleep parameters in healthy subjects concluded that, at all dosages, alcohol reduced time to sleep onset and seemed to produce a more consolidated (less waking after sleep onset) first half of sleep. This, however, was usually followed by increased interruptions in the second half (Ebrahim et al., 2013).

In line with the literature on smoking, it was a significant predictor of poorer sleep quality in our main analysis. This finding is supported by research showing that smokers tend to report more sleep-related psychosomatic symptoms, feel less rested after sleep (Wittmann et al., 2010), have more difficulties initiating and maintaining sleep, and show more daytime sleepiness (Wetter & Young, 1994). Nicotine acts on several (nicotinergic acetylcholine, glutaminergic, dopaminergic and serotinergic) neurotransmitter pathways in the brain and thereby affects states of wakefulness and sleepiness as well as sleep architecture (Jaehne et al., 2009). Thus nicotine’s effects on sleep quality, specifically its effect on delayed sleep onset latency and sleep architecture can likely be attributed to its stimulant effects on the brain, while frequent sleep disturbances may be explained by the experience of nightly withdrawal symptoms (Wittmann et al., 2010).

Cannabis use was not a significant predictor of subjective sleep quality. A recent critical review of randomized controlled trials and open label studies concluded that many studies show some evidence that cannabis use is associated with greater subjective sleep quality, shorter sleep latency, longer sleep duration and fewer episodes of wakefulness during the night (Kuhathasan et al., 2019). However, most studies lacked placebo controls and did not use objective measures of sleep quality (Kuhathasan et al., 2019). Our study did not find such associations, but only a small proportion of our sample reported consuming cannabis so a potential lack of power may also explain our result.

Strengths of the study are that we had a large sample (N = 1114) and thus high overall power. In addition, the survey was anonymous which likely reduced socially desirable responses. We also sought to assess a more comprehensive model of changing lifestyle and psychological factors relevant in young adulthood than many studies previously. Additionally, we used a bootstrap and a validation method to assess how robust our findings are.

There are several limitations to point out about our study. First, even though the dataset is large, it is cross-sectional, thus we cannot assert whether the independent variables also act as predictors in a temporal sense. Second, we aimed to identify factors that are relevant and undergoing changes in college students’ lives, yet our list is not exhaustive. There may be other important factors we did not consider, such as academic stress or levels of physical activity, which future studies on the topic could consider.

Third, because we used posters and flyers and the social network of the students who carried out the data collection to recruit participants, we cannot exclude the possibility of self-selection bias and reduced generalizability resulting from our recruitment strategy. Forth, since women were strongly over-represented in our sample, we caution readers that our results may not generalize to the wider student population. Fifth, for practical reasons we measured sleep quality with a subjective (self-report) measure, so recall bias could have occurred. Another limitation is that only self-report measures were used in this study, which might have contributed to inflated correlations among variables.

Because recent research highlights that risk factors for poor sleep quality in young adulthood likely to differ from risk factors in adolescence or adulthood (Park et al., 2019), our results may help shed some light on to the unique challenges that young adults face regarding sleep during their student life. Furthermore, our results may help guide higher education institutions and healthcare professionals working with students to increase sleep quality, academic performance (Curcio et al., 2006) and reduce distress during psychologically vulnerable times (Baglioni et al., 2011). Additionally, our results stress that students might benefit from more flexible schedules and opportunities to study at times that suit their chronobiology best (for example by making lecture materials available online). This is particularly important for evening chronotypes, who, due to insufficient sleep and social jetlag, might not be able to reach their optimal cognitive and academic performance (Wittmann et al., 2006).

By simultaneously assessing a set of developmentally relevant psychological and lifestyle factors, this large scale study found that more eveningness, more depressive symptoms, less use of the cognitive emotion regulation style ‘putting into perspective’, and smoking cigarettes significantly predicted lower sleep quality in students. ‘Putting into perspective’ and alcohol were only significant predictors in some analyses and thus these findings require replication. Further research with momentary measurements and longitudinal designs will be needed to better disentangle the complex relationships between behavior, psychopathology and sleep quality in this age group.