Introduction

The rapid diffusion and adoption of smartphones, tablets and smartwatches, have greatly facilitated people’s access to goods and services found on the web. This ease of access, however, has translated into increased hours of connectivity such that the amount of time people spend on their smartphones has become a growing concern, prompting several scholars to explore whether their excessive use could lead to addictive behaviours (Duke & Montag, 2017; Kim et al., 2016; Lo Coco et al., 2020; Servidio, 2019). The addictive use of the Internet is often denominated by such terms as: social media addiction, Facebook addiction, and problematic smartphone addiction (Choi et al., 2014; Marino, Gini, Vieno, & Spada, 2018), among others. While Internet addiction and problematic smartphone addiction are two different terms, they are closely related and refer to the overuse of online communication technologies characterised by constant interaction with others (Wegmann & Brand, 2016).

Problematic Smartphone Use (PSU) is a recent construct which is described as the excessive use of the smartphone, accompanied by addiction-like behaviour such as withdrawal (when unable to use one’s phone) and tolerance (increased use to obtain the same level of satisfaction) (Elhai, Levine, & Hall, 2019; Rozgonjuk & Elhai, 2019). Indeed, PSU can involve deficits in social relationships, work and/or school performance, and can even lead to family disputes caused by excessive smartphone use because of too many hours spent on social media interaction (Billieux, Maurage, Lopez-Fernandez, Kuss, & Griffiths, 2015). Additionally, results from several previous studies found an association between PSU and poor mental health condition including depression and anxiety (Coyne, Stockdale, & Summers, 2019; Kara, Baytemir, & Inceman-Kara, 2019), sleep deficits (Zhang & Wu, 2020), low self-esteem (Lannoy et al., 2020), low self-control (Servidio, 2019), and lower motivation in doing physical activities (Pereira, Bevilacqua, Coimbra, & Andrade, 2020). We should underline, however, that PSU is not actually included in the official diagnosis of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) or the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) and does not seem to pose the same risks that drug and alcohol use disorders do. However, Montag, Wegmann, Sariyska, Demetrovics, and Brand (2019) indicate that PSU falls into the realm of mobile Internet use disorders. Thus, based on preliminary evidence, we believe that studying PSU may be useful in complementing what we know of the effects of technological addictive disorders and allows us to better specify which variables potentially drive PSU.

A relevant theory that helps us understand the addictive use of Internet applications relating to gambling, gaming, and pornography use, among others, is the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognitive-Execution model (I-PACE) (Brand et al., 2019; Brand, Young, Laier, Wölfling, & Potenza, 2016). Since the I-PACE conceptualization has further been extended to include research on PSU disorders (Oberst, Wegmann, Stodt, Brand, & Chamarro, 2017; Rozgonjuk & Elhai, 2019; Servidio, 2019), its theoretical framework was deemed appropriate for this study. The framework emphasizes the interaction among a person’s core characteristics and the cognitive, affective, and executive processes as it attempts to explain the mechanisms underlying the development and maintenance of a specific Internet-use disorder such as PSU. It also illustrates the different levels of the addiction process in disorders that include, but are not limited to Internet-gaming. It poses that the development of problematic and addictive behaviour occurs when there is an interaction between an individual’s predisposing variables (e.g., depression, anxiety, and personal factors) and certain aspects related to specific situations (Brand et al., 2016; Brand et al., 2019). The result of the interaction is an experience of gratification and compensation associated with specific Internet-related behaviour (e.g., paying attention to specific stimuli, or feeling the desire to play online). On the other hand, the affective and cognitive response variables are conceptualized as influencing the choice of Internet technology, which can result in adaptive or dysfunctional behaviour.

A person, for example, could use his or her own smartphone to alleviate the burden of real-life problems, to avoid loneliness, or to experience pleasure and positive emotions from the simple fact that one is online (Brand et al., 2016). These expectancies can influence not only the individual’s behaviour, but also the decision to use or not use a specific Internet and/or smartphone application (Wegmann & Brand, 2016). Thus, certain motives and predisposing factors are reinforced by the effects of the gratification experienced and the escape from negative emotions. Consequently, this may lead to excessive use of the preferred Internet and/or smartphone application, resulting in reduced control and stabilization of the person’s core characteristics.

Thus, within the conceptualization of I-PACE, this study focuses on the following variables: first, maximization as a personality trait that represents the general tendency to strive for the best possible option, and leads to delayed decisions (Schwartz et al., 2002); second, a state variable such as fear of missing out (FoMO), which is the apprehension of missing out on rewarding experiences, leading to a corresponding need to stay persistently connected with one’s social network (Przybylski, Murayama, DeHaan, & Gladwell, 2013); and third, self-esteem, which is another personality variable potentially involved in the risk of PSU (Lannoy et al., 2020). The proposed relationships are analysed through correlational analysis and the Structural Equation Modelling (SEM) to test the mediating effects. Finally, because the role of individual characteristics (e.g., age and gender) in the risk of PSU is often inconsistent (Chen et al., 2017), all these variables were included as covariates.

Maximization as a Possible Predictor of PSU

Maximization may be one among several characteristics of an individual that is linked to smartphone overuse. It has been conceptualized as a personality trait which describes individual differences in the general tendency of striving to make the best choice (for reviews see, Misuraca & Fasolo, 2018; Cheek & Schwartz, 2016). According to Schwartz et al. (2002), maximization is an inclination to look for the best alternative possible through systematic and exhaustive searches. The construct of maximization has been widely applied in exploring decision-making strategies regarding consumer behaviour experiences.

Findings based on the construct of maximization have revealed that people who display tendencies to maximize have less life-satisfaction, happiness, optimism, and self-esteem (Newman, Schug, Yuki, Yamada, & Nezlek, 2018); they are also depressed, anxious, and impulsive (Purvis, Howell, & Iyer, 2011; Schwartz et al., 2002). It has also been shown that maximizers tend to report maladaptive decision-making strategies, avoid making decisions (Schwartz et al., 2002), exhibit less behavioural coping strategies and show greater tendency to depend on others for their decisions (Parker, de Bruin, & Fischhoff, 2007). Another set of studies has revealed that maximizers are more prone to social comparison (Polman, 2010) and are unhappy when they discover that others selected better choices compared to theirs, even if their own choice was better than expected (Huang & Zeelenberg, 2012).

Although maximization has not been extensively investigated in the field of PSU, there is a recent study that has linked maximization to problematic social network use. Specifically, this study found that tendencies to maximize and to procrastinate lead to problematic social networks use, especially in cases when there is a high incidence of specific fears and intentions to discover, or to avoid missing out on a potentially “better” alternative for social experiences (Müller, Wegmann, Stolze, & Brand, 2020). Moreover, another study shows that specific online addictions such as PSU, social media addiction, and Internet gaming disorder share the same features with generalized Internet addiction (Chen et al., 2020). It has also been suggested that PSU is associated with social media addiction because persons tend to use their smartphone as easy and portable access to social media applications (Kuss et al., 2018; Sha, Sariyska, Riedl, Lachmann, & Montag, 2019).

People with the tendency to maximize and who receive social media notifications on their smartphone and/or smartwatch about social events could be more susceptible to experiencing negative feelings because of the need to maximize the many choices and decisions they would have to make to avoid missing out on new social experiences. According to the I-PACE model (Brand et al., 2016; Brand et al., 2019), this subjective stress response to situational factors may influence the decision to use Internet-related technologies, such as the smartphone, to cope with the associated cognitions and affects. Therefore, people who think that they can solve specific problems by going online could be led to an uncontrolled and impulsive use of online communication applications. Indeed, similar to those with Internet-addictive behaviour, maximizers tend to delay decision-making but exhibit impulsivity in making such decisions in favour of options that offer immediate gratification (Müller et al., 2020). The negative impact of this impulsivity on PSU has been documented since previous studies have provided evidence of impulsivity as a risk factor for PSU (Kim et al., 2016; Servidio, 2019). In fact, Billieux et al. (2015) proposed a theoretical framework in which PSU is driven by impulsivity leading to excessive smartphone use.

No previous studies have examined the link between maximization and PSU, even if the study by Müller et al. (2020) has investigated the relationship between maximization and problematic social network use. Overall, considering the existing studies, we can make a reasonable hypothesis that tendencies towards maximization could be related to PSU (H1). This hypothesis is in line with the assumption taken by the I-PACE model, which illustrates how personality factors and social situations could lead to high expectancies, and in this case, smartphone-use could represent a dysfunctional coping strategy of escaping from negative feelings and of experiencing pleasure by going online to maximize personal needs.

The Mediating Role of FoMo and Self-Esteem

Given the scarcity of studies examining the potential influence of maximization on PSU, little is known regarding the mediating mechanisms underlying this relationship. Thus, the current study was interested in exploring whether fear of missing out (FoMO; Przybylski et al., 2013) and self-esteem could mediate the relationship between maximization and PSU.

In recent years, FoMO has received great attention since most of the existing researches focus on the question of whether FoMO is connected to an increased use of social media (e.g., Alt & Boniel-Nissim, 2018; Milyavskaya, Saffran, Hope, & Koestner, 2018). The proliferation of social media applications and the related rise in smartphone use have heightened people’s awareness of possibly missing out on potentially exciting social experiences. In this vein, an assumption could be made that the frequent reception of notifications on smartphones and/or smartwatches, reminding people of potentially more rewarding alternatives, could increase feelings of FoMO (Milyavskaya et al., 2018; Müller et al., 2020). Furthermore, it is likewise well known that people who score high in FoMO are likely to overuse their smartphones to satisfy the desire of staying connected (Rozgonjuk, Sindermann, Elhai, & Montag, 2020).

Similar to maximization, a high score in FoMO has been found to show higher levels of regret indicating that FoMO plays an important role in the context of PSU and social media addiction (Oberst et al., 2017; Wegmann, Oberst, Stodt, & Brand, 2017). Moreover, current studies show that FoMO is associated to alcohol consumption (Riordan, Flett, Cody, Conner, & Scarf, 2019), and lower levels of life satisfaction (Casale, Rugai, & Fioravanti, 2018). Additionally, other researchers have demonstrated that FoMO is associated with PSU and other online addictive behaviours, such as excessive social media use (Elhai et al., 2018).

Recently, Milyavskaya et al. (2018) drew further similarities between FoMO and maximization. Specifically, starting from the assumption that having too many choices can lead to choice paralysis, “FoMO can be thought to arise from an abundance of choices among activities or experiences, particularly those of a social nature, coupled with an uncertainty over the ‘best’ choice and anticipatory regret over the options not selected” (Milyavskaya et al., 2018, p. 725). Thus, people who score higher in maximization might have increased feelings of FoMO. Accordingly, this study hypothesizes that tendencies towards maximization predict the severity of the experience of FoMO (H2).

Additionally, empirical studies have examined the mediating role of FoMO in the relationship between psychological variables and PSU. For example, Wang et al. (2019a) found that procrastination and FoMO partially mediated the relationship between sensation seeking and smartphone addiction. Another study indicated that envy was positively related to PSU, with FoMO mediating this relationship (Wang et al., 2019b). Moreover, another study shows that people who score high in FoMO are more prone to check the notifications on their smartphone, as well as to update their social media profile and status because they want to be part of the online community, which behaviour may result in developing the risk of PSU (Long, Wang, Liu, & Lei, 2019). Consequently, they may develop an addictive PSU behaviour due to an almost compulsive use of such communication tools.

Following these lines of research, the present study hypothesizes that FoMO could mediate the relationship between maximization and PSU (H3). The reception of frequent reminders of alternatives, and the thought of possibly missing out on social experiences which are perhaps more gratifying, could be positively associated with PSU (Milyavskaya et al., 2018; Müller et al., 2020). In the I-PACE model, FoMO is a cognitive or affective response driven by predisposing variables such as personality traits, and which in turn contribute to PSU (Brand et al., 2016).

With regard to self-esteem, although prior scholars have investigated the relationships between personality traits and PSU, few studies have explored its role in PSU (Lannoy et al., 2020). According to Rosenberg (1965), self-esteem is one’s positive or negative attitude toward oneself and one’s evaluation of one’s own thoughts and feelings overall in relation to oneself. People with high self-esteem tend to use technologies in a balanced manner, tend to positively cope with stressful situations, and develop a positive attitude towards life (Servidio, Gentile, & Boca, 2018). On the other hand, people with low self-esteem try to find shelter in the Internet network, which allows them to control the self-aspects they want to make public. Similar to what is commonly observed with other forms of addictive behaviour, low self-esteem is associated with Internet addiction and with the compulsive use of social media applications (Andreassen, Pallesen, & Griffiths, 2017).

A cross-sectional study showed that dysfunctional attitudes and self-esteem were found to be mediators between anxious attachment styles and smartphone addiction (Yuchang, Cuicui, Junxiu, & Junyi, 2017). Another study found that self-esteem mediated the link between adolescent student-student relationship and PSU (Wang et al., 2017). Moreover, the results of a recent meta-analysis identified that self-esteem, among other psychopathological variables, was inconsistently related to PSU, with small to medium effects (Elhai, Dvorak, Levine, & Hall, 2017). Overall, these results suggest exploring the role of self-esteem by including new constructs.

As regards the relationship between maximization and self-esteem, one study shows that people with high maximization tendencies, compared to satisfiers, are more sensitive to experiencing low self-esteem (Misuraca & Fasolo, 2018). The behaviour of maximizers is characterized by a high need for scrupulousness and order that reduces self-esteem and optimism and increases high levels of regret, because they sacrifice limited resources in an attempt to discover the best options (Newman et al., 2018). In relation to smartphone use, people tend to use their own smartphone to search for the best alternative, and in turn reinforce the decision to remain connected online, thus increasing the risk of developing addictive behaviours.

To date, however, few studies have examined the mediating role of self-esteem in the relation between personality traits and PSU. Based on the literature reviewed above, this study hypothesizes that maximization is negatively associated with self-esteem, which in turn is negatively associated with PSU (H4). In other words, self-esteem mediates the relationship between maximization and PSU (H5).

The Present Study

Taking all the previous discussions together, it is assumed that an individual with the tendency to maximize would most likely have the propensity for PSU. This association between maximization and PSU could be mediated by specific variables related to opinions and feelings of potentially missing out on significant events that occur online (e.g., FoMO), and by a miximizer’s low levels of self-esteem, which in turn could increase PSU. Thus, the tendency to avoid missing out on potentially better social experiences may reinforce a dysfunctional and uncontrolled use of the smartphone, resulting in higher symptoms of PSU. These cyclic relationships are consistent with the assumption taken by the I-PACE model, given that previous gratification experiences in the use of Internet-based applications tend to change the subjective reward expectations that are associated with the specific behaviour.

Finally, even though much is known about the association between personality traits and PSU, no previous studies have investigated the effects of other personality traits, like maximization, on this relationship, and how FoMO and self-esteem may affect the link between maximization and PSU. Investigating these relationships may give new insights by providing additional knowledge to PSU studies, as maximization could be a predisposing variable that leads to PSU. Additionally, including FoMO and self-esteem into this relationship may explain their mechanisms in the association.

Method

Participants

A sample of 277 undergraduate students was recruited from an Italian public university during the 2019–2020 academic year. The sample consisted of 68 males (24.5%) and 207 females (74.7%). Two students did not report their gender (0.7%). The participants’ ages ranged from 19 to 38 (M = 23.46, SD = 3.56). Most participants attended psychological and educational courses (64.6%); the rest were enrolled in various courses such as economics (4%), healthcare (5.8%) and a scientific one (13.7%). The remaining students (11.9%) did not indicate their degree courses.

Measures

Maximization Scale

The Maximization scale (Schwartz et al., 2002) was used to measure the individual maximizing tendency. The scale was translated from English to Italian according to the recommendations of the International Test Commission (2017). It contains 13 items (e.g., I often find it difficult to shop for a gift for a friend) and the participants reported the extent to which each statement described them, using a seven-point Likert scale ranging (from 1 = “strongly disagree” to 7 = “strongly agree”) with higher scores indicating higher maximization tendencies. The scale revealed a satisfactory internal reliability, α = .75 (M = 3.78, SD = .97). Finally, confirmatory factor analysis showed that all the items loaded on the scale, indicating that the construct validity of the instrument was good, χ2S-B (60, = 270) = 85. 82, p = .018, CFI = .941, TLI = .923, RMSEA = .040, 90% CI [.02, .06], SRMR = .050.

Fear of Missing out Scale

The Italian version of the Fear of Missing Out (FoMO) scale was used to measure the adolescents’ disposition towards fear of missing out (Casale & Fioravanti, 2020; Przybylski et al., 2013). The scale comprises 10 items (e.g., I fear others have more rewarding experiences than me), measured on a five-point Likert type (from 1 = “not at all true of me” to 5 = “extremely true of me”). For the present study, the reliability of the scale was, α = .70.

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale

The Italian version of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem scale (Prezza, Trombaccia, & Armento, 1997) was used to measure the subjective feelings of self-value and self-acceptance. Participants rated their agreement with 10 statements (e.g., “I feel that I have a number of good qualities”) on a four-point Likert type (from 1 = “strongly agree” to 4 = “strongly disagree”). Higher scores indicate higher self-esteem. The internal consistency in the sample of this present study, α = .86, was good.

Problematic Smartphone Use Scale

The short Italian version of the 10 items-Problematic Smartphone Use scale for adolescents and young adults was used (PSU; De Pasquale, Sciacca, & Hichy, 2017; Kwon, Kim, Cho, & Yang, 2013). Participants gave their answer on a six-point Likert scale (from 1 = “strongly disagree” to 6 = “strongly agree”) with higher scores indicating higher PSU. A sample item is “I have used my Smartphone longer than I had intended”. The PSU scale showed good reliability and validity for the assessment of PSU and the Italian version has shown good psychometric properties as well (see also, Servidio, 2019). The scale reliability in the current sample was, α = .82.

Procedure

A cross-section web-based survey was conducted, which provided a battery of assessment aimed at investigating individual behaviour towards digital technologies. Additionally, the participants were asked to provide demographic information about their gender, age, and degree course. A snowball procedure was applied in recruiting the participants. Specifically, it consisted of having a participant ask colleagues or fellow university students to complete the survey, and those new participants, in turn, recruited other university students they knew and so on. The information about the study was emailed with the link of the survey to undergraduate students. Furthermore, the link of the survey was posted on a Facebook wall. Only measures relevant to the current research are reported.

All participants were informed of the study’s objectives and were guaranteed strict confidentiality in their answers to the questionnaire. Those who agreed to participate in this study signed an online informed consent form, and were invited to complete an anonymous questionnaire. All participants volunteered for the study and none of them received any kind of remuneration. Moreover, they were also given the opportunity to withdraw the data from the study at any stage. Completing the online survey took approximately 25 min.

Statistical Analyses

Before performing the data analyses, all the variables were screened for linearity, homoscedasticity, and homogeneity of variance. Additionally, univariate normality (skewness and kurtosis), multivariate outliers, and cases with missing values were checked. The scatterplot of standardised predicted values versus standardised residuals, showed that the data met the assumptions of linearity and the residuals were approximately normally distributed. This suggests that the data does not violate the assumption of homoscedasticity. The Levene’s F test revealed that the homogeneity of variance assumption was met with, p = .166. Following the general recommendations for skewness and kurtosis, our variables were normally distributed (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2014); the highest skewness value was, .73 (FoMO), and for kurtosis was, −.41 (Maximization). By computing the Mahalanobis distance with, p < .001 for the chi-square (χ2) distribution value (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2014), no multivariate outliers among the cases were identified. No cases had missing data. Descriptive statistics and Pearson’s correlation analyses were computed. The internal reliability was obtained by computing the alpha of Cronbach (α). SPSS 25 package was used to run the preliminary statistical procedures.

A partial and a full mediating model were separately tested to verify whether the proposed mediators, self-esteem, and FoMO fully explained the relationships between maximization and PSU. Age and gender were also controlled. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and structural equation modelling (SEM) were performed using Mplus 7.04 (Muthén & Muthén, 2014). As Hu and Bentler (1999) recommended, multiple indices were used to evaluate the model fit (adopted cut-offs in brackets): the chi-square (χ2) test value with the associated p value (p > .05), comparative fit index (CFI ≥ .95), Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI ≥ .95), root-mean-squared error of approximation (RMSEA ≤ .06), and its 90% confidence interval, and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR < .08). Both confirmatory and SEM were estimated with the maximum-likelihood parameter with standard errors and a mean-adjusted chi-square test statistic that were robust to non-normality (MLM) as Maydeu-Olivares (2017) suggested. The MLM chi-square test statistic is also referred to as the Satorra-Bentler (S-B) chi-square.

With regard to the SEM, item parcelling was applied (Little, Cunningham, Shahar, & Widaman, 2002) to define each construct of the model on a latent level. The item parcelling was built by applying a balanced method, which aims to combine high and low inter-correlations values (Little, Rhemtulla, Gibson, & Schoemann, 2013). Finally, the partial mediating model and the full mediating model were compared to determine the mediating role of self-esteem and FoMO in the relationship between maximization and PSU. The models were evaluated by using a chi-square (χ2) difference test, which provides a statistical test of whether the constraints are justified (Kline, 2016). To establish significant differences between models, at least two out of three criteria had to be satisfied: Δχ2 significant at, p < .05, ΔCFI ≤ .005, and ΔRMSEA ≤ .010 (Chen, 2007).

Ethics

All the research materials and procedures were designed and conducted according to the ethical standards laid out by the Italian Psychological Association (AIP) and the Declaration of Helsinki.

Results

Descriptive and Correlations

Means, standard deviations, and bivariate correlations of the measurements of maximization, self-esteem, FoMO, PSU, age, and gender are shown in Table 1. The mean of maximization score indicates tendencies towards maximizers rather than satisfiers (who are inclined to attempt to find a tree that works for them rather than the best one) in the current sample. Finally, all the correlations between the constructs are significant and satisfy the conditions for performing the SEM analysis.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and correlations between the measures of maximization, self-esteem, FoMO, PSU, age, and gender

Structural Equation Modelling and Mediation Analysis

Figure 1 illustrates the findings from the SEM used to analyse the hypotheses of the present study and includes the standardized estimates. The hypothesized model provided a good fit to the data, χ2S-B (59, N = 275) = 90.22, p = .005, CFI = .966, TLI = .955, RMSEA = .044, 90% CI [.02, .06], SRMR = .061. Age and gender were predictors of PSU. Finally, PSU accounted for 35% of the variance.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Results of the structural equation model, including factor loadings and direct effects and significance levels. Note. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001

Table 2 shows the results of the mediation analysis and the indirect effects for PSU and its predictor and mediators’ variables.

Table 2 Mediation and indirect effects with standardized estimate of fear of missing out, and self-esteem for the relationship between maximization and problematic smartphone use

The results of the analysis indicated that the relationship between maximization and PSU was partially mediated by self-esteem and FoMO. As for the indirect effects, both self-esteem and FoMO indirectly affected the association between maximization and PSU.

Next, a full mediated model and a partially mediated one were tested. Although the full mediated model provided sufficient fit to the data, χ2S-B (60, N = 275) = 113.96, p = .000, CFI = .941, TLI = .924, RMSEA = .060, 90% CI [.04, .07], SRMR = .072, after comparing the two models, the results showed that the full mediated model fits the data worse, Δχ2(1) = 26.27, p < .001, ΔCFI = −.025, ΔRMSEA = .013. Therefore, the less restricted model fits the data better than the smaller model in which some parameters are fixed. Thus, the partial mediating model was selected as the final one.

Discussion

The aim of the current study was to investigate the predictive role of maximization on PSU, and the mediating effects of FoMO and self-esteem in this relationship.

A direct, positive, and significant association between maximization and PSU was found, supporting H1. The empirical results of this study confirm that maximization inclinations are associated with PSU. These results fit with the theoretical conceptualization of the I-PACE model (Brand et al., 2019), whose focus is to explain how specific Internet-use disorders, such as PSU, are developed and maintained. Individuals with the tendency to maximize in finding the “better” option increase their risk of developing PSU. Given that impulsiveness is a personality characteristic of maximizers, we can interpret this result in the context of studies demonstrating that impulsivity increases the risk of PSU (Kim et al., 2016; Servidio, 2019), as well as in the framework of the I-PACE model, which associates impulsivity with unspecified Internet-use disorder (Brand et al., 2019). The results of the SEM indicate that the association between maximization and PSU was partially mediated by FoMO and self-esteem, revealing the predictive role of maximization in the development of PSU. Thus, this study provides new insights for future investigations aimed at identifying and reducing the risk of problematic smartphone use.

Consistent with a previous study (Müller et al., 2020), the assumption that an association could exist between maximization and FoMO (H2) was supported by the present results. This association might indicate that the tendency to maximize decisions increases the likelihood of FoMO, which then intensifies dysfunctional online interaction and specifically the risk of PSU. This positive association underlines the idea that by having several options on hand, people could change decisions and use their smartphone to search for better alternatives, and in turn increase the risk of developing PSU. People who experience FoMO tend to frequently check their social media smartphone applications, which could result in PSU (Casale et al., 2018). Similarly, because a smartphone allows access to different kind of applications, FoMO could be accentuated when the person considers multiple socially relevant events (Milyavskaya et al., 2018). According to the I-PACE model, people could also use their smartphone applications to cope with negative emotions arising from the feeling caused by FoMO and this mechanism characterises the development of addictive behaviour (Brand et al., 2019). In this view, PSU could be interpreted as a cognitive-emotional regulation strategy aimed at handling the stress associated with failing to fulfil personal needs. Taken together, the study’s findings provide a new perspective in exploring the cause of FoMO and how it is related to PSU.

The hypothesized mediating role of FoMO (H3) in the prediction of PSU was partially supported by the results of the SEM. While this study offered empirical support for the hypothesized mediating link, the result of the partial mediation provides a clue that other mediators may have been overlooked in the proposed model. Further studies should consider examining the effect of social comparison (Festinger, 1954), which often leads people to think that they are worse off than others in certain aspects, thus increasing the experience of negative feelings. Indeed, maximizers tend to adopt strategies of seeking out and comparing alternatives because they experience a tendency towards perfectionism and this could lead to increased use of the Internet and its related technologies such as the smartphone in searching for better alternatives.

This study also confirmed that maximization was negatively related to self-esteem, which in turn was negatively related to PSU, supporting H4, that is, self-esteem partially mediates the association between maximization and PSU (H5). Moreover, the indirect effects were marginally significant. By contrast, an increase in maximization is associated with lower levels of self-esteem and life-satisfaction (Schwartz et al., 2002) and reduced self-esteem increases the risk of PSU. Maximizing is linked to social comparison which leads to more counterfactual thinking and regret and subsequently leads to greater dissatisfaction and lower personal well-being (Newman et al., 2018). As suggested by Billieux et al. (2015), individuals with low self-esteem have distorted cognitions and difficulty in emotion regulation. Thus, these people feel the need to use a smartphone to obtain comfort in affective relationships. Self-esteem is a key component of any self-improvement program that could help people to resist addictive behaviours such as PSU. The results of the current study are consistent with previous findings which suggest that individuals with low self-esteem have the propensity of becoming Internet addicts (Andreassen et al., 2017; Servidio et al., 2018). In addition, this result is consistent with another study showing that individuals with low self-esteem have difficulty undertaking social interactions and receive less social support, and consequently feel unpleasant emotions such as loneliness (Yuchang et al., 2017). According to the I-PACE model, loneliness and self-esteem are two important factors in addictive behaviour (Brand et al., 2019), thus self-esteem could work as a protective factor against PSU. Therefore, removing irrational beliefs and improving self-esteem could reduce PSU tendencies.

Limitations and Future Research

This study has some limitations. First, due to the cross-sectional nature of the study design, we cannot make any causal inferences about the observed findings. For example, the causality between maximization and PSU remains unclear. It could be plausible that smartphone use may increase the desire to search for the best alternative. According to Milyavskaya et al. (2018), for instance, we can assume circular or bi-directional association in a manner that maximization, as well as FoMO and self-esteem, affect PSU but that smartphone overuse, in turn, can influence these variables, increasing the problematic or addictive behaviour in terms of stabilizing and intensifying mechanisms (cf., Brand et al., 2019; Müller et al., 2020). Because of the cross-sectional nature of this study, future research could perhaps explore these findings using a longitudinal design.

Second, the scale for measuring maximization has not yet been validated. Based on the results of the present study, future investigations could use the proposed instrument to validate and test its psychometric properties. A third limitation was related to the sample, which was skewed towards women, who usually use the smartphone and social media applications more than men (Chen et al., 2017). Finally, the present study used only self-report questionnaires that have well-known limitations such as desirability biases.

Conclusions and Practical Implications

The results of the current study suggest that maximization tendency is a significant predictor of PSU, especially if reinforced by a belief of missing out on an opportunity for a rewarding social experience, as indicated by the partially mediating role of FoMO and self-esteem. The present study contributes to prior literature on the relationship between personality traits constructs, in this case, maximization, and PSU by considering the mediating role of FoMO and self-esteem. Furthermore, this is the first study that, to our knowledge, explores the link between maximization and PSU, indicating that maximization can be a potential PSU-driving variable. These findings have specific social and behavioural implications on the relevance that FoMO and maximization have in relation to the dysfunctional use of smartphones.

From a practical point of view, the current outcomes suggest that health professionals who are trying to reduce the prevalence of PSU should focus on increasing self-esteem and decreasing FoMO to prevent the problematic behaviour connected with the overuse of smartphone applications and related Internet technologies. Finally, the study provided empirical support for the I-PACE model that the interaction of several risk factors may lead to different kinds of problematic online behaviour.