Skip to main content
Log in

Social exclusion increases the visual working memory capacity of social stimuli

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

While social exclusion has previously been found to weaken the visual working memory capacity (WMC) of nonsocial stimuli such as colored squares, this study examines the influence of social exclusion on the visual WMC of social stimuli. In Experiment 1 and 2, a Cyber-ball game was used to manipulate social exclusion. Biology motion (BM) in Experiment 1, while in Experiment 2 human faces were used as social stimuli. In Experiment 1, 44 participants were asked to remember 1–5 biology motions. The results demonstrated that socially excluded participants had a larger WMC in BM than the socially included group. In Experiment 2, 54 participants were asked to remember 1–4 human faces showing three different emotions (angry, happy, neutral). The results demonstrated that socially excluded participants had a larger WMC than the control group in all three types of emotional face. The results of experiment 1 and 2 suggest that social exclusion increases the capacity of social stimuli in visual WMC.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baddeley. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory.pdf.

  • Barrett, Tugade, & Engle. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind.pdf. Psychological Bulletin, 130(4), 553–573.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, & Leary. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 817–827.

  • Baumeister, R. F., Twenge, J. M., & Nuss, C. K. (2002). Effects of social exclusion on cognitive processes: Anticipated aloneness reduces intelligent thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(4), 817–827.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beeney, J. E., Franklin, R. G., Jr., Levy, K. N., & Adams, R. B., Jr. (2011). I feel your pain: Emotional closeness modulates neural responses to empathically experienced rejection. Social Neuroscience, 6(4), 369–376. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2011.557245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benenson, J. F., Markovits, H., Hultgren, B., Nguyen, T., Bullock, G., & Wrangham, R. (2013). Social exclusion: More important to human females than males. PLoS One, 8(2), e55851. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055851.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, M. J., Young, S. G., Brown, C. M., Sacco, D. F., & Claypool, H. M. (2008). Adaptive responses to social exclusion: Social rejection improves detection of real and fake smiles. Psychological Science, 19(10), 981–983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blake, R., & Shiffrar, M. (2007). Perception of human motion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 47–73. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Böckler, A., Hömke, P., & Sebanz, N. (2013). Invisible man : Exclusion from shared attention affects gaze behavior and self-reports.Pdf. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(2), 140–148. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550613488951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewin, & Beaton. (2002). Thought suppression, intelligence, and working memory capacity.pdf. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 40(8), 923–930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewin, & Smart. (2005). Working memory capacity and suppression of intrusive thoughts. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 36(1), 61–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buelow, M. T., Okdie, B. M., Brunell, A. B., & Trost, Z. (2015). Stuck in a moment and you cannot get out of it: The lingering effects of ostracism on cognition and satisfaction of basic needs. Personality and Individual Differences, 76, 39–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, S., Balogh, S., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2015). Implicit attention to negative social, in contrast to nonsocial, words in the Stroop task differs between individuals high and low in loneliness: Evidence from event-related brain microstates. Cortex, 70, 213–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calvomg, L. (2008). Facial expressions of emotion (KDEF):Identification under different display-duration conditions.pdf. Behavior reasearch methods.

  • Cowan. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity.pdf. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2000(24), 87–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curby, K. M & Gauthier, I. (2007). A visual short-term memory advantage for faces.pdf. Psychon Bull Rev.

  • DeWall, Maner, & Rouby. (2009). Social exclusion and early-stage interpersonal perception:Selective attention to signs of acceptance.pdf. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(4), 729–741.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Downey, G., Khouri, H., & Feldman, S. I. (1997). Early interpersonal trauma and later adjustment: The mediational role of rejection sensitivity Developmental perspectives on trauma: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 85–114). Rochester: University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duclos, S. E., Laird, J. D., Schneider, E., Sexter, M., & a. (1989). Emotion-specific effects of facial expressions and postures on emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(1), 100–108. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.1.100.

  • Durante, K. M., Griskevicius, V., Hill, S. E., Perilloux, C., & Li, N. P. (2011). Ovulation, female competition, and product choice: Hormonal influences on consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(6), 921–934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. Paper presented at the Nebraska symposium on motivation.

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, O. (2014). The mechanisms underlying the effects of social exclusion on working memory capacity.pdf.

  • Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view.

  • Frykholm, S. R. a. G. (1983). Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention.pdf. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.

  • Gao, Z., Bentin, S., & Shen, M. (2015). Rehearsing biological motion in working memory: An EEG study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(1), 198–209. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00687.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, W. L. (2000). Social exclusion and selective memory: How the need to belong influences memory for social events.pdf.

  • Gardner, W. L., Pickett, C. L., Jeffries, V., & Knowles, M. (2005). On the outside looking in: Loneliness and social monitoring. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(11), 1549–1560.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. K., Hutton, S. B., & Morgan, M. J. (2010). Sex differences in scanning faces: Does attention to the eyes explain female superiority in facial expression recognition? Cognition & Emotion, 24(4), 629–637. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930902906882.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawes, D. J., Zadro, L., Fink, E., Richardson, R., O'Moore, K., Griffiths, B., ... Williams, K. D. (2012). The effects of peer ostracism on children's cognitive processes. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(5), 599–613. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2011.638815.

  • Huang, L., Yang, T., & Li, Z. (2003). Applicability of the positive and negative affect scale in Chinese. Chinese Mental Health Journal.

  • Jackson, M. C., & Raymond, J. E. (2008). Familiarity enhances visual working memory for faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 34(3), 556–568. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.34.3.556.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, Wu, C.-Y., Linden, D. E. J., & Raymond, J. E. (2009). Enhanced visual short-term memory for angry faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 363–374. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013895.

  • Jackson, M. C., Linden, D. E., & Raymond, J. E. (2014). Angry expressions strengthen the encoding and maintenance of face identity representations in visual working memory. Cognition & Emotion, 28(2), 278–297. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.816655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson. (1973). Visual-perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis.pdf. Perception & Psychophysics.

  • Johnson, M. K., Mcmahon, R. P., Robinson, B. M., Harvey, A. N., Hahn, B., Leonard, C. J., et al. (2013). The relationship between working memory capacity and broad measures of cognitive ability in healthy adults and people with schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 27(2), 220–229.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  •  Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2000). Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: Limits on long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(2), 336–358.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leary. (1995). Self-esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis.pdf. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 518–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leary. (1999). Making sense of self-esteem.pdf. Current Directions in Psychological Science., 8, 32–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. R. G. (2004). The sociometer, self-esteem, and the regulation of interpersonal behavior..pdf. In R.F. Baumeister & K .Vohs (Eds),Handbook of self-regulation (2nd ed). New York.NY:Guiford Press.

  • Lee, E. Y., Cowan, N., Vogel, E. K., Rolan, T., Valle-Inclán, F., & Hackley, S. A. (2010). Visual working memory deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease are due to both reduced storage capacity and impaired ability to filter out irrelevant information. Brain, 133(9), 2677–2689.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Maner, Gailliot, M. T., & DeWall, C. N. (2007). Adaptive attentional attunement: Evidence for mating-related perceptual bias. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(1), 28–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.05.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nezlek, J. B., Kowalski, R. M., Leary, M. R., Blevins, T., & Holgate, S. (1997). Personality moderators of reactions to interpersonal rejection: Depression and trait self-esteem. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(12), 1235–1244. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672972312001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oberauer, K., Süß, H. M., Wilhelm, O., & Sander, N. (2007). Individual differences in working memory capacity and reasoning ability.

  • O'Luanaigh, C., O'Connell, H., Chin, A. V., Hamilton, F., Coen, R., Walsh, C., ... Lawlor, B. A. (2012). Loneliness and cognition in older people: The Dublin healthy ageing study. Aging & Mental Health, 16(3), 347–352.

  • Otten, M., & Jonas, K. J. (2013). Out of the group, out of control? The brain responds to social exclusion with changes in cognitive control. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(7), 789–794.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parkinson, B. (2005). Do facial movements express emotions or communicate motives? Personality & Social Psychology Review An Official Journal of the Society for Personality & Social Psychology Inc, 9(4), 278–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlova, M. A. (2012). Biological motion processing as a hallmark of social cognition. Cerebral Cortex, 22(5), 981–995. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pickett, C. L., Gardner, W. L., & Knowles, M. (2004). Getting a cue: The need to belong and enhanced sensitivity to social cues. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(9), 1095–1107. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203262085.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Morey, C. C., & Cowan, N. (2011). How to measure working memory capacity in the change detection paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(2), 324–330. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0055-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sacco, D. F., Wirth, J. H., Hugenberg, K., Chen, Z., & Williams, K. D. (2011). The world in black and white: Ostracism enhances the categorical perception of social information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(4), 836–842. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.03.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scolari, M., Vogel, E. K., & Awh, E. (2008). Perceptual expertise enhances the resolution but not the number of representations in working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(1), 215–222. https://doi.org/10.3758/pbr.15.1.215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shen, M., Gao, Z., Ding, X., Zhou, B., & Huang, X. (2014). Holding biological motion information in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1332–1345. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036839.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shilling, A. A., & Brown, C. M. (2016). Goal-driven resource redistribution: An adaptive response to social exclusion. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 10(3), 149–167. https://doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000062.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Themanson, J. R., Ball, A. B., Khatcherian, S. M., & Rosen, P. J. (2014). The effects of social exclusion on the ERN and the cognitive control of action monitoring. Psychophysiology, 51(3), 215–225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., ... Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168(3), 242–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006.

  • Tousignant, B., Eugène, F., Sirois, K., & Jackson, P. L. (2017). Difference in neural response to social exclusion observation and subsequent altruism between adolescents and adults. Neuropsychologia.

  • Troje, N. F. (2003). Reference frames for orientation anisotropies in face recognition and biological-motion perception. Perception, 32(2), 201–210. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Troje, W., & Lavrov. (2005). Person identification from biological motion: Effects of structural and kinematic cue.pdf. Perception & Psychophysics.

  • Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Stucke, T. S. (2001). If you can't join them, beat them: Effects of social exclusion on aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 1058–1069. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1058.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Beest, I., & Williams, K. D. (2006). When inclusion costs and ostracism pays, ostracism still hurts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(5), 918–928. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.918.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verfaillie, J. V. a. K. (2004). Perception of biological motion:A stimulus set of human point-light actions.pdf. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers.

  • Vogel, E. K., Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2001). Storage of features, conjunctions, and objects in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(1), 92–114. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.27.1.92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect:The PANAS scales.pdf. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, N. C., Fawcett, J. M., & Newman, A. J. (2014). Electrophysiological markers of biological motion and human form recognition. Neuroimage, 84, 854–867. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkowski, B. M., Robinson, M. D., & Friesen, C. K. (2009). Gaze-triggered orienting as a tool of the belongingness self-regulation system. Psychological Science, 20(4), 495–501. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02321.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. D. (2007). Ostracism. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 425–452. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085641.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. D., & Nida, S. A. (2011). Ostracism: Consequences and coping. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(2), 71–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K., & Choi, W. (2000) Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 748–762.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. N. (2007). Visual working memory for observed actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 136(4), 639–652. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, M., Li, Z., Diao, L., Fan, L., Zhang, L., Yuan, S., & Yang, D. (2017). Social exclusion impairs distractor suppression but not target enhancement in selective attention. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 121, 72–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, Q., Qi, L., Diao, F., et al. (2018). Social exclusion weakens storage capacity and attentional filtering ability in visual working memory. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(1), 92–101. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This work was supported by the grants from the fundamental research funds for the central universities (SWU1809352), and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant nos. NSFC71472156 and NSFC31600919), and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (grant no. SWU1509110).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Cody Ding or Dong Yang.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and / or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Du, X., Xu, M., Ding, C. et al. Social exclusion increases the visual working memory capacity of social stimuli. Curr Psychol 39, 1149–1160 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00274-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00274-1

Keywords

Navigation