Skip to main content
Log in

Consistency and contrast effects in moral evaluation of euthanasia

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Euthanasia is a highly controversial topic. One of the arguments against legalisation of euthanasia is that it would lead to an attitudinal slippery slope effect; that is, a shift in attitudes toward euthanasia even toward cases which were not legalised. The present study tested a possible mechanism which may lead to such shift in two experiments. Participants judged morality of euthanasia in two hypothetical scenarios describing patients requesting euthanasia. We found that participants who first evaluated a case of a non-terminally ill patient suffering from fatigue afterward considered euthanasia for a terminally ill patient suffering from pain more morally right than participants who evaluated euthanasia in the latter case first. Furthermore, we found that presenting the case of the patient suffering from fatigue before asking about attitudes toward legality of euthanasia led participants to oppose it more. The study suggests that public’s expressed attitudes toward legality of euthanasia might be easily influenced by a choice of illustrative examples. However, the change in attitudes predicted by the slippery slope effect was not observed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Active euthanasia is defined as “intentionally administering medications or other interventions to cause the patient’s death [...]” with voluntary active euthanasia specifically limited to “[...] at the patient’s explicit request and with full informed consent” (Emanuel 1994, p. 1891). Some other possible definitions of euthanasia also specify the patient’s condition (e.g., World Health Organization 2004, p. 25) and some of the cases of euthanasia within the scope of definition used by Emanuel would fall outside of the scope of the definition by WHO.

  2. Based on the slippery slope argument we predicted that participants evaluating the pain scenario first (pain-fatigue group) should evaluate the fatigue scenario as less wrong and that participants evaluating the fatigue scenario first (fatigue-pain group) should evaluate the pain scenario as more wrong. These two order effects combined should result in generally higher wrongness ratings for the fatigue-pain group, which is tested by the predictor order.

  3. Participation in both studies was anonymous. Therefore, it is possible that some of the participants took part in both studies. Using a recent estimate by Stewart et al. (2015) that an average laboratory samples out of about 7300 MTurk workers, approximately 33 participants took part in both our studies. A recent study (Chandler et al. 2015) found reduced effect sizes when the same experiment was given twice to the same participants. However, given that the possible nonnaïveté of participants is likely only to reduce the effects studied in the present study and that the expected number of participants who took part in both studies is relatively low, we do not consider participants’ nonnaïveté a significant problem.

References

  • Andrade, E. B., & Ariely, D. (2009). The enduring impact of transient emotions on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, J., Paolacci, G., Peer, E., Mueller, P., & Ratliff, K. A. (2015). Using nonnaive participants can reduce effect sizes. Psychological Science, 26, 1131–1139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Van Landeghem, P., Carpentier, N., & Deliens, L. (2012). Different trends in euthanasia acceptance across Europe. A study of 13 western and 10 central and eastern European countries, 1981–2008. The European Journal of Public Health, 23, 378–380.

  • Dierickx, S., Deliens, L., Cohen, J., & Chambaere, K. (2015). Comparison of the expression and granting of requests for euthanasia in Belgium in 2007 vs 2013. JAMA Internal Medicine, 175, 1703–1706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, C. (1993). Choosing Death. British Medical Journal, 306, 1075.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emanuel, E. J. (1994). Euthanasia: Historical, ethical, and empiric perspectives. Archives of Internal Medicine, 154, 1890–1901.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emanuel, E. J. (2002). Euthanasia and physician-assisted suicide: A review of the empirical data from the United States. Archives of Internal Medicine, 162, 142–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emanuel, E. J., Onwuteaka-Philipsen, B. D., Urwin, J. W., & Cohen, J. (2016). Attitudes and practices of euthanasia and physician-assisted suicide in the United States, Canada, and Europe. JAMA, 316, 79–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fedorikhin, A., & Cole, C. A. (2004). Mood effects on attitudes, perceived risk and choice: Moderators and mediators. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 2–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FeldmanHall, O., Mobbs, D., Evans, D., Hiscox, L., Navrady, L., & Dalgleish, T. (2012). What we say and what we do: The relationship between real and hypothetical moral choices. Cognition, 123, 434–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feltz, A. (2015). Everyday attitudes about euthanasia and the slippery slope argument. In M. Cholbi & J. Varelius (Eds.), New directions in the ethics of assisted suicide and euthanasia (pp. 217–237). Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Guadagno, R. E., & Cialdini, R. B. (2010). Preference for consistency and social influence: A review of current research findings. Social Influence, 5, 152–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, L., Johansson, P., Tärning, B., Sikstrӧm, S., & Deutgen, T. (2010). Magic at the marketplace: Choice blindness for the taste of jam and the smell of tea. Cognition, 117, 54–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsella, T. D., & Verhoef, M. J. (1993). Alberta euthanasia survey: 1. Physicians' opinions about the morality and legalization of active euthanasia. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 148, 1921–1926.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, B. H., & Caplan, A. L. (2015). Euthanasia in Belgium and the Netherlands: On a slippery slope? JAMA Internal Medicine, 175, 1640–1641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 272–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment: Mechanisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110, 472–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hara, R. E., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., & Sinnott-Armstrong, N. A. (2010). Wording effects in moral judgments. Judgment and Decision making, 5, 547–554.

    Google Scholar 

  • Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349, 943.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petrinovich, L., & O'Neill, P. (1996). Influence of wording and framing effects on moral intuitions. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17, 145–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radbruch, L., Leget, C., Bahr, P., Müller-Busch, C., Ellershaw, J., de Conno, F., & Vanden Berghe, P. (2016). Euthanasia and physician-assisted suicide: A white paper from the European Association for Palliative Care. Palliative Medicine, 30, 104–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raijmakers, N. J., van der Heide, A., Kouwenhoven, P. S., van Thiel, G. J., van Delden, J. J., & Rietjens, J. A. (2013). Assistance in dying for older people without a serious medical condition who have a wish to die: A national cross-sectional survey. Journal of Medical Ethics, 41, 145–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N. (2007). Cognitive aspects of survey methodology. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 277–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1991). Constructing reality and its alternatives: Assimilation and contrast effects in social judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgment (pp. 217–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 195–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwitzgebel, E., & Cushman, F. (2012). Expertise in moral reasoning? Order effects on moral judgment in professional philosophers and non-philosophers. Mind & Language, 27, 135–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. W. (2005). Evidence for the practical slippery slope in the debate on physician-assisted suicide and euthanasia. Medical Law Review, 13, 17–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, N., Ungemach, C., Harris, A. J., Bartels, D. M., Newell, B. R., Paolacci, G., & Chandler, J. (2015). The average laboratory samples a population of 7,300 Amazon mechanical Turk workers. Judgment and Decision making, 10, 479–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Schwarz, N. (1988). Priming and communication: Social determinants of information use in judgments of life satisfaction. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 429–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Volokh, E. (2003). The mechanisms of the slippery slope. Harvard Law Review, 116, 1026–1137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vrakking, A. M., van der Heide, A., Looman, C. W., van Delden, J. J., Onwuteaka-Philipsen, B. D., van der Maas, P. J., & van der Wal, G. (2005). Physicians' willingness to grant requests for assistance in dying for children: A study of hypothetical cases. The Journal of Pediatrics, 146, 611–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vranka, M. A., & Bahník, Š. (2016). Is the emotional dog blind to its choices? Experimental Psychology, 63(3), 180–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiegmann, A., Okan, Y., & Nagel, J. (2012). Order effects in moral judgment. Philosophical Psychology, 25(6), 813–836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., & Hodges, S. (1992). Attitudes as temporary constructions. In L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgements (pp. 37–65). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization (2004). A glossary of terms for community health care and services for older persons. Kobe: WHO.

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research has been supported by the Internal grant of the Faculty of Arts, Charles University, ref. no. FF-VG2016-79, co-awarded to Marek Vranka.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Štěpán Bahník.

Ethics declarations

The study was funded by the Internal grant of the Faculty of Arts, Charles University (ref no. FF-VG2016–79). All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Implicit informed consent was used in the study, which did not pose any risks to the participants.

Conflict of Interests

The authors declare no conflict of interest associated with publication of the manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bahník, Š., Vranka, M.A. Consistency and contrast effects in moral evaluation of euthanasia. Curr Psychol 40, 822–830 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-0012-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-0012-7

Keywords

Navigation