Skip to main content
Log in

Not Seeing is Believing: the Role of Invisibility in Human Lives

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Our life is full of invisibility that exerts power on our acts, relationships, and construction of the self. This paper discusses psychological processes in which invisibility plays an essential role, and constructs a typology of invisibility in society and in our lives. After a brief look at the crucial role of invisibility in prevailing theories of psychology, I first show how invisibility works in children’s meaning constructions, the process in which their selves become clear for observers (the presentational self, Komatsu 2010). The development of children’s meaning making is led by different types of invisibility concerning the children themselves. Second, I extend the discussion from the development of individuals to the role of socially regulated invisibility that controls our acts and relationships with others, introducing examples concerning religious belief in history. After these discussions, I present a hierarchical classification of invisibility from a simple spatial-temporal separation of concretely existing objects and ourselves to an abstract aspect of invisibility in which the object and its meaning are unclear.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Gestalt quality is defined as “a positive content of presentation bound up in consciousness with the presence of complexes of mutually separable (i.e., independently presentable) elements” (von Ehrenfels 1988, p. 93). The term presentational self comes from “die Vorstellung” in the German language that is used by von Ehrenfels (1988), signifying presentation (Komatsu 2010).

  2. Excerpts 2 and 3 are from Komatsu and Konno (2014) and were translated into English by the author. Small revisions were made from the original and translations in Komatsu (2015). All the names are pseudonyms. Words in brackets show additional information included for clarification and omitted subjects or other pronouns. Omission of the first person is common and natural in the Japanese language. The academic year in Japan begins in April and ends in March.

  3. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Phryne

  4. These regions are in the west end of Kyushu Island that is situated in the west end of four big islands constituting the Japanese Archipelago.

  5. The number of the believers during the prohibition of Christianity is not clear. In the beginning of Meiji era when the prohibition of Christianity ended, there were seven districts that kept the communities of crypt Christian (Miyazaki 2014).

  6. From her standpoint of iconography, Wakakuwa (2008) discussed that Maria-Kannon is not a mere substitution for the original figure of the Virgin Mary, but an integration of the indigenous religious tradition, worship for goddess that prevails in East Asia and in the Christian tradition. She also stresses that the name Maria-Kannon was given by researchers and the figures were simply called “Maruya (Maria)” by worshippers.

  7. Word origin of Orasho is said to be Oratio in Latin. Orasho is sometimes accompanied with melody (Miyazaki 2014).

References

  • Bird, A., & Reese, E. (2006). Emotional reminiscing and the development of an autobiographical self. Developmental Psychology, 42, 613–626.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., & Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development, 77, 1568–1588.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giesen, B. (2012). Iconic difference and seduction. In J. C. Alexander, D. Bartmański, & B. Giesen (Eds.), Iconic power: materiality and meaning in social life (pp. 203–218). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Haden, C. A., Haine, R. A., & Fivush, R. (1997). Developing narrative structure in parent-child reminiscing across the preschool years. Developmental Psychology, 33, 295–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hedegaard, M., & Fleer, M. (2008). Studying children: a cultural-historical approach. Maidenhead: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Komatsu, K. (2006). The construction of a young child’s self in mother-child conversations: a longitudinal case study focusing on ‘the self in relation to others’. Japanese Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 115–125 (in Japanese).

  • Komatsu, K. (2010). Emergence of young children’s presentational self in daily conversation and its semiotic foundation. Human Development, 53, 208–228.

  • Komatsu, K. (2012). Temporal reticence of the self: who can know my self? Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 46, 357–372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Komatsu, K. (2013). Why and how young children’s presentational self emerges in day-to-day conversation about the past? Focusing on children’s daily trip to Yochien, in Japan. In G. Marsico, K. Komatsu, & A. Iannaccone (Eds.), Crossing boundaries: Intercontextual dynamics between family and school (pp. 109–133). Charlotte: Information Age.

    Google Scholar 

  • Komatsu, K. (2015). Otherness is everywhere to bring about your self: an inquiry into the whimsical emergence of children’s selves. In J. Valsiner, G. Marsico, N. Chaudhary, T. Sato, and V. Dazzani (Eds.), Annals of theoretical psychology (Vol. 13) Psychology as the science of human being: The Yokohama manifesto (pp. 287–297). Springer international.

  • Komatsu, K. (2016). On the dialectic nature of human mind: the dynamic tension between sameness and non-sameness. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 50, 174–183.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Komatsu, K., & Konno, C. (2014). Children’s presentational self in their personal stories: an exploration of 3rd grade students’ writings from a semiotic perspective. Japanese Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 323–335 (in Japanese).

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyazaki, K. (2014). カクレキリシタンの実像:日本人のキリスト教理解と受容 Tokyo: Yoshikawa-Kobunkan. (in Japanese).

  • Nakazono, S. (2016). Hidden Christianity: followers and their beliefs. Journal of Historical Studies, 941, 43–51 (in Japanese).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: the emergence of the mediated mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ohashi, Y. (2014). 潜伏キリシタン: 江戸時代の禁教政策と民衆 Tokyo: Kodan-sha. (in Japanese)

  • Ohnuki-Tierney, E. (1994). The power of absence: zero signifiers and their transgressions. L'Homme, 34(2) (Whole No. 130), 59–76.

  • Okada, A. (2009). キリストの身体: 血と肉と愛の傷 Tokyo: Chuo Koron Shinsha. (in Japanese)

  • Valsiner, J. (2007). Culture in minds and societies: foundations of cultural psychology. New Delhi: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Ehrenfels, C. (1988). On ‘Gestalt qualities’. In B. Smith (Ed. and trans.), Foundations of gestalt theory (pp.82–117). Munich: Philosophia.

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (Rev.ed.). (A. Kozulin Trans.) Cambridge: MIT Press.

  • Wakakuwa, M. (2008). 聖母像の到来 Tokyo: Seido-sha. (in Japanese)

  • Watanabe, M. E. (2007). A comparison of language arts education in the three countries, Japan, the United States and France: socio historical analyses of “Reading and Writing”. Nihon-Kenkyu, 35, 573–619 (in Japanese).

  • Williams, D. (1996). Deformed discourse: the function of the monster in mediaeval thought and literature. Exeter: University of Exeter press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Koji Komatsu.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Author Koji Komatsu declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

Except for quotations from studies that were published in the past, this article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals performed by any of the authors.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Komatsu, K. Not Seeing is Believing: the Role of Invisibility in Human Lives. Integr. psych. behav. 51, 14–28 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-016-9364-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-016-9364-4

Keywords

Navigation