Skip to main content
Log in

From Mind to Context, from Accuracy to Meaning. Exploring the Grammar of Remembering as a Socially Situated Act

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper begins by addressing the so-called memory crisis, a crisis which, since the 90s, has problematized the traditional manner in which memory is studied and understood. Special attention is paid to the changing role attributed to accuracy and meaning when remembering the past. In light of this crisis, I comment on Smorti and Fioretti’s paper (2015), focusing on the point that they make regarding how autobiographical narratives affect and change autobiographical memories. Complementing that view, according to which memories are transformed when they are externalized through a communicative act by means of narratives, this paper focuses on a more narrative and situated approach to memory, shifting from mind to social settings, from accuracy to meaning. Building on that approach, I briefly discuss the notion of event as a narrative construction. Finally, drawing on Burke’s pentad model (1969), I put forward a framework for studying remembering as a situated activity. The pentad of elements are addressed as follows: 1) Agency, or the mediational means for the construction of past events; 2) Act, or remembering as a reconstructive activity; 3) Scene, or the social dynamics of remembering; 4) Agent, or subjective positionings when reconstructing the past; and 5) Purpose, or uses of the past in relation to the future.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Here it is worth bearing in mind that before considering his materials as relevant to the study of remembering, Bartlett analyzed them as exemplars for the study of perceiving, imagining and thinking. In this regard, Bartlett’s contribution is not only about what is remembered, but also about what is first perceived to be remembered later (I would like to thank Alberto Rosa for pointing this out in the revision of this paper).

  2. As indicated by Edwards and Middleton (1987), Bartlett was initially going to entitle his book Conventionalization, which shows how important systems of meaning and the cultural ways in which members of a certain group reconstruct the past were to that author.

  3. In this work Wagoner (2013) comprehensively explores the concept of schema’s origins, its place within Bartlett’s work and its successive reconstructions by others after him.

  4. This is a problem of which Smorti and Fioretti (2015) are well aware when they point out that:

    “[I]t is very difficult to study autobiographical memory […] without making use of language and of narrative in particular. But if we as researchers use narrative or language it is very difficult to demonstrate what we have described is memory (or internal memory) because we have observed memory ‘in’ a narrative” (p.8).

  5. As Brockmeier (2010) points out, the temporal status of experiences is crucial here. Different studies (e.g., Szpunar, Chan and McDermott, 2009, cited in Brockmeier 2010) show no verifiable distinction on a neurobiological level between what we perceive or experience in the present, what we anticipate in our imagination for the future, and what we remember from the past. According to Brockmeier, this distinction is attributed afterwards by us in an act of interpretation and temporal localization through which we temporalize and meaningfully link our experiences by means of narratives.

  6. Along these lines, Brown and Reavey (2015) have analyzed how the shared use of photographs and diagrams of the wrecked trains by different survivors of the 7/7 London terrorist attacks helped to co-construct the memories of that event.

  7. In relation to this, Pennington and Hastie’s (1990a, 1990b) studies within the field of forensic psychology focus on how juries evaluate both the reliability of witness testimonies (as truthful or deceiving) and the actions narrated (as excusable, punishable, etc.).

  8. In Brescó (2016a), this process is examined by using the notion of prolepsis, defined by Cole (1996) as the process by which an imagined final cause acquires pragmatic force for current action.

References

  • Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). The dialogic imagination. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauman, R. (1986). Story, performance, and event. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Beals, D. E. (1998). Reappropriating schema: conceptions of development from Bartlett and Bakhtin. Mind Culture and Activity, 5(1), 3–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Billig, M. (1987). Arguing and thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brescó, I. (2009). Form and content of historical accounts. studying the (re)construction of past events through different narratives and identitarian positionings. Journal of Psychology and Society, 2(2), 102–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brescó, I. (2016a). The end into the beginning. Prolepsis and the reconstruction of the collective past. Culture & Psychology, special issue on collective memory.

  • Brescó, I. (2016b). Conflict, memory and positioning. Studying the dialogical and multivoiced dimension of the Basque conflict. Peace & Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, special issue on memory and conflict.

  • Brescó, I., & Rosa, A. (2012). Memory, history and narrative. shifts of meaning when (re)constructing the past. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 8(2), 300–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brescó, I., & Wagoner, B. (2016). Context in the cultural psychology of remembering. In C. Stone & L. Bietti (Eds.), Contextualizing human memory (pp. 69–85). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brockmeier, J., & Carbaugh, D. (Eds.) (2001). Narrative and Identity: Studies in Autobiography, Self and Culture. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brockmeier, J., & Harré, R. (2001). Narrative. problems and promises of an alternative paradigm. In J. Brockmeier & D. Carbaugh (Eds.), Narrative and Identity: Studies in Autobiography, Self and Culture (pp. 39–58). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brockmeier, J. (2010). Afther the archive: remapping memory. Culture & Psychology, 16(1), 5–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S. D., & Reavey, P. (2015). Vital memory and affect. Living with a difficult past. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S.D. & Reavey, P. (2016). False Memories of Epistemic Consensus. Culture & Psychology.

  • Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J., & Feldman, C. (1996). Group narrative as a cultural context of autobiography. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering our past (pp. 291–317). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, K. (1969). A rhetoric of motives. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castro, J., & Rosa, A. (2007). Psychology within time. theorizing about the making of socio-cultural psychology. In J. Valsiner & A. Rosa (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of sociocultural psychology (pp. 62–80). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Danziger, K. (2008). Marking the mind. A history of memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • De Luca Picione, R. (2015). The idiographic approach in psychological research. The challenge of overcoming old distinctions without risking to homogenize. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science. doi:10.1007/s12124-015-9307-5.

  • Ebbinghaus, H. (1885/1998). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. London: Thoemmes Press.

  • Edwards, D., & Middleton, D. (1987). Conversation and remembering: Bartlett revisited. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 1(2), 77–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, D., & Potter, J. (1992). Discursive psychology. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, M. (2001). From substance to story. narrative, identity, and the reconstruction of the self. In J. Brockmeier & D. Carbaugh (Eds.), Narrative and Identity: Studies in Autobiography, Self and Culture (pp. 283–298). Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: fabrication and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35, 603–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Group, P. M. (1982). Popular memory: theory, politics and method. In R. Johnson, G. McLennan, B. Schwarz, & D. Sutton (Eds.), Making histories: Studies in history-writing and politics. London: Centre for Contemporary Cultural Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, E. B. (2001). The repeated reproduction of Bartlett’s remembering. History of Psychology, 4(4), 341–366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Margalit, A. (2002). The ethics of memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Middleton, D., & Brown, S. D. (2007). Issues of socio-cultural study of memory: making memory matter. In J. Valsiner & A. Rosa (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of socio-cultural psychology (pp. 661–677). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Middleton, D., & Crook, C. (1996). Bartlett and socially ordered consciousness: A discursive perspective. Comments on Rosa. Culture & Psychology, 2, 379–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Middleton, D., & Edwards, D. (1990). Conversational remembering: A social psychological approach. In D. Middleton & D. Edwards (Eds.), Collective remembering (pp. 23–45). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mink, L. O. (1978). Narrative form as a cognitive instrument. In R. H. Canary & H. Kozicki (Eds.), The writing of history. literary form and historical understanding (pp. 129–149). Madison, Wisconsin: The University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: a social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111(2), 486–511.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1990a). Practical implications of psychological research on juror and jury decision making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(1), 90–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1990b). Explaining the evidence: tests of the story model for juror decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(2), 189–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polkinghorne, D. E. (1988). Narrative knowing and the human sciences. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Randall, W. L. (2010). The narrative complexity of our past: in praise of Memory’s sins. Theory and Psychology, 20(2), 147–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, G. (2001). The history and narrative debate, 1960–2000. In G. Roberts (Ed.), The history and narrative reader (pp. 1–21). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosa, A. (1996). Bartlett’s psycho-anthropological project. Culture & Psychology, 2(4), 355–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosa, A., & González, F. (2014). Performing ethics at identity crossroads. In S. Salvatore, A. Gennaro, & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Yearbook of Idiographic Science: Multicentric Identities in Globalizing Worlds (pp. 279–308). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saito, A. (Ed.) (2000). Bartlett, Culture and Cognition. London, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. (2001). The seven sins of memory. Boston and New York: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals, and Understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smorti, A., & Fioretti, C. (2015). Why narrating changes memory: A contribution to an integrative model of memory and narrative processes. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science. doi:10.1007/s12124-015-9330-6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, C., & Bietti, L. (Eds.) (2016). Contextualizing human memory. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Straub, J. (2005). Telling stories, making history: toward a narrative psychology of the historical construction of meaning. In J. Straub (Ed.), Narration, identity, and historical consciousness (pp. 44–98). New York: Berghahn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szpunar, K. K., Chan, J. C. K., & McDermott, K. B. (2009). Contextual processing in episodic future thought. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 1539–1548.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J., & Rosa, A. (2007). Contemporary social-cultural research: uniting culture, society, and psychology. In J. Valsiner & A. Rosa (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of socio-cultural psychology (pp. 1–20). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1987). The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky. Volume 4: The history of the development of higher mental functions. New York: Plenum Press.

  • Wagoner, B. (2013). Bartlett’s concept of schema in reconstruction. Theory & Psychology, 23(5), 553–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagoner, B. (2016a). The constructive mind: Frederic Bartlett's psychology in reconstruction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagoner, B. (2016b). What makes memory constructive? A study in the serial reproduction of Bartlett’s experiments. Culture & Psychology.

  • Wagoner, B., & Gillespie, A. (2014). Sociocultural mediators of remembering: An extension of Bartlett’s method of repeated reproduction. British Journal of Social Psychology, 53, 622–639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (2006). Voices of collective remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (2007). Collective Memory. In J. Valsiner & A. Rosa (Eds.)., The Cambridge Handbook of Socio-Cultural Psychology (pp. 645–660). New York: Cambridge University Press.

  • Wertsch, J. V. (2016). National memory and where to find it. In B. Wagoner (Ed.), Handbook of Culture and Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, H. (1986). The content of the form. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this chapter was supported by the Niels Bohr Professorship Centre for Cultural Psychology at Aalborg University. The author also wishes to thank Prof. Alberto Rosa for his helpful comments on the earlier draft of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ignacio Brescó de Luna.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Brescó de Luna, I. From Mind to Context, from Accuracy to Meaning. Exploring the Grammar of Remembering as a Socially Situated Act. Integr. psych. behav. 50, 320–332 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-016-9345-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-016-9345-7

Keywords

Navigation