Skip to main content
Log in

Developing “Personality” Taxonomies: Metatheoretical and Methodological Rationales Underlying Selection Approaches, Methods of Data Generation and Reduction Principles

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Taxonomic “personality” models are widely used in research and applied fields. This article applies the Transdisciplinary Philosophy-of-Science Paradigm for Research on Individuals (TPS-Paradigm) to scrutinise the three methodological steps that are required for developing comprehensive “personality” taxonomies: 1) the approaches used to select the phenomena and events to be studied, 2) the methods used to generate data about the selected phenomena and events and 3) the reduction principles used to extract the “most important” individual-specific variations for constructing “personality” taxonomies. Analyses of some currently popular taxonomies reveal frequent mismatches between the researchers’ explicit and implicit metatheories about “personality” and the abilities of previous methodologies to capture the particular kinds of phenomena toward which they are targeted. Serious deficiencies that preclude scientific quantifications are identified in standardised questionnaires, psychology’s established standard method of investigation. These mismatches and deficiencies derive from the lack of an explicit formulation and critical reflection on the philosophical and metatheoretical assumptions being made by scientists and from the established practice of radically matching the methodological tools to researchers’ preconceived ideas and to pre-existing statistical theories rather than to the particular phenomena and individuals under study. These findings raise serious doubts about the ability of previous taxonomies to appropriately and comprehensively reflect the phenomena towards which they are targeted and the structures of individual-specificity occurring in them. The article elaborates and illustrates with empirical examples methodological principles that allow researchers to appropriately meet the metatheoretical requirements and that are suitable for comprehensively exploring individuals’ “personality”.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For the term experiencing, see the section on The Psyche in part I below.

  2. To appear in the Annals of Theoretical Psychology, vols. 12, 13.

  3. The term “personality” put in quotation marks in this trilogy indicates that its definitions vary and that different researchers use this term to refer to different kinds of phenomena (see Uher 2014a).

  4. The term “non-physical” is put in quotation marks in the TPS-Paradigm because the term denotes properties that are not simply contrasted against the physical but are complementary instead (see Uher 2014a).

  5. The term psychical as opposed to psychological is explained below (see part I, section on The Psyche).

  6. Digital data can be conceived as immaterial physical phenomena. But as they can be perceived and used only through the material phenomena to which they are systematically related and bound (e.g., computer screen and other hardware), this specification is irrelevant for the issues explored here.

  7. In the TPS-Paradigm, the terms morphology and physiology denote the organismal structures and functions, in and of themselves, rather than the scientific disciplines that explore these phenomena.

  8. The meaning of the term mediation in the TPS-Paradigm refers to the Latin mediare, to be in the middle, not to the meaning established in statistics (where it is differentiated from moderation).

  9. Culture is also often conceived of as “environment” or context, but as it denotes semiotically mediated systems of socially shared meaning (Geertz 1973; Weber 1904) the TPS-Paradigm conceives of cultures as semiotic representations that are explored accordingly.

  10. Previously called the “environmental situation” (Uher 2011a, b, 2013; Uher et al. 2013a).

  11. Previously labelled “manifest system approaches” (Uher 2013) and “bottom-up approaches (Uher 2008a).

  12. Translated original: “Approcher des concepts constitués de mots avec un outil (la langue) constitué de mots fait qu’on distingue mal l’outil de mesure du phénomène observé” (Lahlou 1998 p. 52).

  13. Translated original: “Übergreifende Rahmentheorie” (Laucken 1974, p. 24)

  14. Translated original: “Erklärungsrepertoire, Erklärungsinstrumentarium” (Laucken 1974, p. 23)

  15. Translated original: “La langue, principal outil de description et de raisonnement cognitif, ‘scientifique’, ne représente qu’une petite partie, ou plutôt une projection très partielle, de la vie mentale du sujet.” (Lahlou 1998, p. 135).

  16. Previously called the Behavioural Repertoire Approach (Uher 2008a, b; Uher and Asendorpf 2008; Uher et al. 2008) or the Behavioural Repertoire x Environmental Situations Approach (BRxES-Approach; Uher 2011a, b, 2013; Uher et al. 2013a, b).

References

  • Abric, J.-C. (1984). A theoretical and experimental approach to the study of social representations in a situation of interaction. In R. Farr & S. Moscovici (Eds.), Social representation (pp. 169–184). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abric, J.-C. (1993). L’étude expérimentale des représentations sociales. In D. Jodelet (Ed.), Les représentations sociales (3ème éd). Paris: Presses Universitaires Francaises.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams, D. K., & Zener, K. E. (1935). Translators’ preface. In K. Lewin (Ed.), A dynamic theory of personality. Selected papers. New York and London: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York, NY: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1942). The use of personal documents in psychological science (Bulletin 49). New York, NY: Social Science Research Council.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1965). Letters from Jenny. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace and World.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1966). Traits revisited. American Psychologist, 21, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W., & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait names: a psycholexial study. Psychological Monographs, 47, 1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W., & Vernon, P. E. (1933). Studies in expressive movement. New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Almagor, M., Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (1995). The big seven model: a cross-cultural replication and further exploration of the basic dimensions of natural language trait descriptors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 300–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Archer, J. (1992). Ethology and human development. Hemel Hemstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf, Barnes & Noble.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arro, G. (2013). Peeking into personality test answers: inter- and intraindividual variety in item interpretations. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 47, 56–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asendorpf, J. B. (1988). Individual response profiles in the behavioral assessment of personality. European Journal of Personality, 2, 155–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2005). A defence of the lexical approach to the study of personality structure. European Journal of Personality, 19, 5–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology: Concepts and methods for studying the environment of human behavior. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berg, B. L., & Lune, H. (2012). Qualitative research methods for the social sciences (8th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187–229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block, J. (2010). The five-factor framing of personality and beyond: some ruminations. Psychological Inquiry, 21, 2–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G. (1967). An ethological study of same aspects of social behaviour of children in nursery school. In D. Morris (Ed.), Primate ethology (pp. 347–368). London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G. (1972). Categories of child-child interaction. In N. G. Blurton Jones (Ed.), Ethological studies of child behavior (pp. 97–127). London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolden, R., & Moscarola, J. (2000). Bridging the quantitative-qualitative divide: the lexical approach to textual data analysis. Social Science Computer Review, 18, 450–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boring, E. G. (1953). A history of introspection. Psychological Bulletin, 50, 169–189.

  • Bühler, K. (1907). Tatsachen und Probleme zu einer Psychologie der Denkvorgänge I. Über Gedanken. Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie, 9, 297–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bühler, K. (1934/1982). Sprachtheorie. Die Darstellungsfunktion der Sprache. Stuttgart: UTB Gustav Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M., & Craik, K. H. (1983). The act frequency approach to personality. Psychological Review, 90, 105–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, J. (2013). Rethinking Introspection. A pluralist approach to the first-person perspective. Houndmills, Basingstroke, Hampshire: Palgrave Macmillan.

  • Calatayud, F., Montaudouin, S., Le Pape, G., & Bellengier, E. (2006). Analyse du comportement de l’animal ou analyse du discours de l’éthologiste? Réflexions sur l’assimilation des données comportementales à des données textuelles. In JADT 2006. Actes des 8èmes journées internationales d’analyse statistique des données textuelles, (pp. 211–222). Besançon, Franche-Comté: Presses Universitaires.

  • Caspi, A., & Roberts, B. W. (1999). Personality continuity and change across the life course. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 300–326). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. (1943). The description of personality II. Basic traits resolved into clusters. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 38, 476–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. (1946). The description and measurement of personality. New York: World Book.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cervone, D., Shadel, W. G., & Jencius, S. (2001). Social-cognitive theory of personality assessment. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 33–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, F. M., van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Leong, F. T. L. (2011). Toward a new approach to the study of personality in culture. American Psychologist, 66, 593–603.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Church, A. T. (2001). Personality measurement in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Personality, 69, 979–1006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Church, A. T., & Katigbak, M. S. (1988). The emic strategy in the identification and assessment of personality dimensions in a non-western culture: rationale, steps, and a Philippine illustration. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 19, 140–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Church, A. T., & Katigbak, M. S. (1989). Internal, external, and self-report structure of personality: an investigation of cross-language and cross-cultural generalizability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 857–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collingwood, R. G. (1940). An essay on metaphysics. Oxford, UK: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Still stable after all these years: Personality as a key to some issues in adulthood and old age. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim Jr. (Eds.), Life span development and behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 65–102). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO personality inventory (NEO–PI–R) and NEO Five-Factor inventory (NEO–FFI). Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diriwächter, R., & Valsiner, J. (2008). Striving for the whole: Creating theoretical syntheses. (Eds.). Somerset, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dong, W., Lepri, A., & Pentland, S. (2011). Modeling the so-evolution of behaviors and social relationships using mobile phone data, Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Mobile and Ubiquitous Multimedia, 134–143.

  • Dunn, J. (2005). Naturalistic observation of children and their families. In S. Greene & D. Hogan (Eds.), Researching children’s experience: Approaches and methods (pp. 87–101). Thousands Oaks CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eibl-Eibesfeld, I. (1986). Die Biologie des menschlichen Verhaltens. Grundriß der Humanethologie. (3. Aufl.). München: Piper Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1947). Dimensions of personality. London, UK: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1992). Four ways five factors are not basic. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 667–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fahrenberg, J. (2002). Psychologische Interpretation. Biographien - Texte - Tests. Bern: Huber.

  • Fahrenberg, J. (2008a). Gehirn und Bewusstsein. Neurophilosophische Kontroversen In: S. Gauggel und M. Herrmann (Hrsg.). Handbuch der Neuro- und Biopsychologie (S. 28–43). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

  • Fahrenberg, J. (2008b). Die Wissenschaftskonzeption der Psychologie bei Kant und Wundt. E-Journal Philosophie der Psychologie, 10. (download www.Jochen-Fahrenberg.de)

  • Fahrenberg, J. (2013). Zur Kategorienlehre der Psychologie. Komplementaritätsprinzip. Perspektiven und Perspektiven-Wechsel. Lengerich: Pabst Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahrenberg, J., & Myrtek, M. (Eds.). (2001). Progress in ambulatory assessment computer-assisted psychological and psychophysiological methods in monitoring and field studies. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahrenberg, J., Myrtek, M., Pawlik, K., & Perrez, M. (2007). Ambulatory assessment – monitoring behavior in daily life settings. A behavioral-scientific challenge for psychology. European Journal of Personality Assessment, 23, 206–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flick, U. (2008). Managing quality in qualitative research. London, UK: Sage.

  • Freud, S. (1923). The ego and the id. Standard Edition, 19, 1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gadamer, H. G. (1975). Wahrheit und Methode. Grundzüge einer philosophischen Hermeneutik. (4. Aufl.). Tübingen: Mohr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. (1884). Measurement of character. Fortnightly Review, 36, 179–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gödel, K. (1931). Über formal unentscheidbare Sätze der Principia Mathematica und verwandter Systeme I. Monatshefte für Mathematik und Physik, 38, 173–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. R. (1982). From Ace to Zombie: Some explorations in the language of personality’. In C. D. Spielberger & J. N. Butcher (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 1, pp. 203–234). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: The Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 59, 1216–1229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R., & Gough, H. C. (2006). The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 84–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunthert, K., Conner, T. S., Armeli, S., Tennen, H., Covault, J., & Kranzler, H. (2007). The serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and anxiety reactivity in daily life: a daily process approach to gene-environment interaction. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 762–768.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heisenberg, W. (1989). Encounters with Einstein: And other essays on people, places, and particles. Princeton NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Holt.

  • JCGM, Joint Committee for Guides in Metrology. (2008). International vocabulary of metrology – Basic and general concepts and associated terms (VIM) (3rd ed.), Working Group 2 (Eds.), Joint Committee for Guides in Metrology.

  • John, O. P., Angleitner, A., & Ostendorf, F. (1988). The lexical approach to personality: a historical. Review of trait taxonomic research. European Journal of Personality, 2, 171–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jovchelovitch, S. (2007). Knowledge in context: Representations, community and culture. London, UK: Routledge.

  • Kant, I. (1786/1968). Metaphysische Anfangsgründe der Naturwissenschaft (Hrsg. B. Erdmann; P. Menzer, & A. Hoıfler). Preussische Akademie der Wissenschaften, Textausgabe Band IV (pp. 465–565). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

  • Kant, I. (1781/1998). Kritik der reinen Vernunft (Hrsg. J. Timmermann). Hamburg: Felix Meiner Verlag.

  • Kant, I. (1798/2000). Anthropologie in pragmatischer Hinsicht (Hrsg. R. Brandt). Hamburg: Felix Meiner Verlag.

  • Kaplan, R. M., & Saccuzzo, D. P. (2008). Psychological testing: Principles, applications, and issues (7th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thompson. Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs (Vol. 1 and 2). New York, NY: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, J. E., & Figueredo, A. J. (1997). The five-factor model plus dominance in chimpanzee personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 257–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lahlou, S. (1996a). A method to extract social representations from linguistic corpora. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 278–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lahlou, S. (1996b). Propagation of social representations. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 26, 157–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lahlou, S. (1998). Penser-manger. Paris, France: Les Presses Universitaires de France.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lahlou, S. (2001). Functional aspects of social representations. In K. Deaux & G. Philogene (Eds.), Representations of the social: Bridging theoretical traditions (pp. 131–146). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahlou, S. (2008). L’Installation du Monde: De la représentation à l’activité en situation. Aix-en-Provence, Université de Provence: Habilitation à Diriger des Recherches en Psychologie, 375.

  • Lahlou, S. (2011). How can we capture the subject’s perspective?: An evidence-based approach for the social scientist. Social Science Information, 50, 607–655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lahlou, S., Nosulenko, V., & Samoylenko, E. (2012). La numérisation du travail. Théories, méthodes et expérimentations. Paris, France: Collection EDF R & D. Technique & Doc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larocco, S. (2014). Ideology, affect, semiotics: Towards a non-personal theory of personality. Integrated Psychological and Behavioral Science, 48, 129–142.

  • Laucken, U. (1974). Naive Verhaltenstheorie. Stuttgart: Klett.

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Pape, G., Reinert, M., Blois-Heulin, C., & Belzung, C. (1997). Découpage de l’activité exploratoire en sous-unités de comportement chez la souris. Sciences et Techniques de l’Animal de Laboratoire, 22, 131–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Poidevin, R. (2011). The experience and perception of time. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2011 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.), http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2011/entries/time-experience/

  • Lehner, P. N. (1998). Handbook of ethological methods. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, J. (2003). Experience and representation. In Q. Smith & A. Jokic (Eds.), Consciousness: New essays (pp. 121–136). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of topological psychology. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Little, B. R. (1987). Personality and the environment. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology (pp. 205–244). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Little, B. R. (2000). Persons, contexts, and personal projects: Assumptive themes of a methodological transactionalism. In S. Wapner, J. Demick, T. Yamamoto, & H. Minami (Eds.), Theoretical perspectives in environment-behavior research. Underlying assumptions, research problems, and methodologies (pp. 79–88). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, J., (1689). Essay concerning human understanding. Book I. The Project Gutenberg EBook #10615. http://www.gutenberg.org. retreived 08/09/2013

  • Loftus, G. R. (1996). Psychology will be a much better science when we change the way we analyze data. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 161–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Deary, I. J., & Whiteman, M. C. (2003). Personality traits (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P. (1992). The five-factor model in personality: a critical appraisal. Journal of Personality, 60, 329–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R. (2011). Personality theories for the 21st century. Teaching of Psychology, 38, 209–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509–516.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1999). A five-factor theory of personality. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 139–153). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60, 175–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mehl, M. R. & Conner, T. S. (Editors) (2012). Handbook of research methods for studying daily life. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

  • Millikan, R. (1993). White queen psychology and other essays for Alice. Bradford: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1977). The interaction of person and situation. In D. Magnusson & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Personality at the crossroads: Current issues in interactional psychology (pp. 333–352). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Mendoza-Denton, R. (2002). Situation-behavior profiles as a locus of consistency in personality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 50–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molenaar, P. C. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement: Interdisciplinary Research and Perspectives, 2, 201–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moscovici, S. (1961). La psychanalyse, son image et son public. Paris, PUF. Published in English as Moscovici, S. (2008). Psychoanalysis, its image and its public. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.

  • Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? The Philosophical Review, 83, 435–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuman, Y. (2014). Introduction to computational cultural psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Norman, T. (1967). 2,800 personality trait descriptors: Normative operating characteristics for a university population. Ann Arbor, MI: Department of Psychology, University of Michigan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ogden, C. K. (1932). Bentham’s theory of fictions. New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Omi, Y. (2012). Tension between the theoretical thinking and the empirical method: Is it an inevitable fate for psychology? Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 46, 118–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pauli, R. (1927). Einführung in die experimentelle Psychologie. Leipzig: Quelle und Meyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1901/1935). Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce (CP 7.218—1901, On the logic of drawing history from ancient documents especially from testimonies). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1903/1997). Lecture five: the normative sciences. In C. S. Peirce (Ed.), Pragmatism as a principle and method of right thinking (pp. 205–220). Edited by P. Turrisi. Albany, NY: Suny Press [Harvard Lectures on Pragmatism—CP 5.188-89].

  • Reinert, M. (1983). Une méthode de classification descendante hiérarchique: Application à l’analyse lexicale par contexte. Les cahiers de l’analyse des données, Vol VIII, n° 2.

  • Reinert, M. (1990). ALCESTE: Une méthodologie d’analyse des données textuelles et une application: Aurélia de Gérard de Nerval. Bulletin de Méthodologie Sociologique, 26, 24–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roivainen, E. (2013). Frequency of the use of English personality adjectives: implications for personality theory. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 417–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, P. J., & Valsiner, J. (2011). The un-making of a method: from rating scales to the study of psychological processes. Theory and Psychology, 21, 47–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotter, J. B. (1954). Social learning and clinical psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Royce, J. (1891). The religious aspect of philosophy: A critique of the bases of conduct and of faith. Boston: Houghton, Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saucier, G. (1997). Effects of variable selection on the factor structure of person-descriptors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1296–1312.

  • Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (1996a). The language of personality: Lexical perspectives on the five factor model. In J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 21–50). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (1996b). Evidence for the Big Five in analyses of familiar English personality adjectives. European Journal of Personality, 10, 61–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (1998). What is beyond the Big Five? Journal of Personality, 66, 495–524.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (2001). Lexical studies of indigenous personality factors: premises, products, and prospects. Journal of Personality, 69, 847–880.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (2002). Assessing the big five: Applications of 10 psychometric criteria to the development of marker scales. In B. de Raad & M. Perugini (Eds.), Big Five assessment (pp. 30–54). Ashland, OH: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. (1999). The seven sins of memory: Insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 54, 182–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). Constructive memory: ghosts of past and future. Nature, 445, 27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. P., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Sexual dimensions of person description: beyond or subsumed by the big five? Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 141–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schrödinger, E. (1958). Mind and matter. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, M. (2014). The living fossil of human judgment. The living fossil of human judgment. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 48, 211–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A., & D’Andrade, R. G. (1980). The systematic distortion hypothesis. In R. A. Shweder (Ed.), Fallible judgment in behavioral research: New directions for methodology of social and behavioral science (Vol. 4, pp. 37–58). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A., & Sullivan, M. A. (1990). The semiotic subject of cultural psychology. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality (pp. 399–416). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. K. (1973). Temporal clusters and individual differences in the behaviour of preschool children. In R. P. Michael & J. H. Crook (Eds.), Comparative ecology and behaviour of primates (pp. 751–798). London, U.K.: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. K., & Connolly, K. J. (1972). Patterns of play and social interaction in preschool children. In N. G. Blurton Jones (Ed.), Ethological studies of child behavior (pp. 65–95). London, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. K., & Connolly, K. J. (1980). The ecology of preschool behaviour. London, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, W. (1924). Wertphilosophie (Person und Sache. System des kritischen Personalismus. Dritter Band). Leipzig: Barth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tellegen, A. (1993). Folk concepts and psychological concepts of personality and personality disorder. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 122–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terracciano, A., & 78 Members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project. (2005). Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 547–561.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1993). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P. M., Cannon, T. D., & Toga, A. W. (2002). Mapping genetic influences on human brain structure. Annals of Medicine, 34, 523–536.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2008). Variables in psychology: a critique of quantitative psychology. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 42, 245–265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2009). How methodology became a toolbox – and how it escapes from that box. In J. Valsiner, P. Molenaar, M. Lyra, & N. Chaudhary (Eds.), Dynamic process methodology in the social and developmental sciences (pp. 45–66). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2011). Travel into a fairy land: a critique of modern qualitative and mixed methods psychologies. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 45, 21–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.). (2010). Methodological thinking in psychology: 60 years gone astray? Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uher, J. (2008a). Three methodological core issues of comparative personality research. European Journal of Personality, 22, 475–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uher, J. (2008b). Comparative personality research: methodological approaches. European Journal of Personality, 22, 427–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uher, J. (2011a). Individual behavioral phenotypes: an integrative meta-theoretical framework. Why ‘behavioral syndromes’ are not analogues of ‘personality’. Developmental Psychobiology, 53, 521–548.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uher, J. (2011b). Personality in nonhuman primates: What can we learn from human personality psychology? In A. Weiss, J. King, & L. Murray (Eds.), Personality and temperament in nonhuman primates (pp. 41–76). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Uher, J. (2013). Personality psychology: lexical approaches and assessment methods reveal only half of the story. A metatheoretical analysis. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 47, 1–55.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uher, J. (2014a). Conceiving “personality”: Psychologists’ challenges and basic fundamentals of the Transdisciplinary Philosophy-of-Science Paradigm for Research on Individuals. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science. doi:10.1007/s12124-014-9283-1

  • Uher, J. (2014b). Developing “personality” taxonomies: Metatheoretical and methodological rationales underlying selection approaches, methods of data generation and reduction principles. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science. doi:10.1007/s12124-014-9280-4

  • Uher, J. (2014c). Interpreting “personality” taxonomies: Why previous models cannot capture individual-specific experiencing, behaviour, functioning and development. Major taxonomic tasks still lay ahead. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science. doi:10.1007/s12124-014-9281-3

  • Uher, J. (2014d). Agency enabled by the Psyche: Explorations using the Transdisciplinary Philosophy-of-Science Paradigm for Research on Individuals. Annals of Theoretical Psychology, 12. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-10130-9-13

  • Uher, J., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2008). Personality assessment in the great apes: comparing ecologically valid behavior measures, behavior ratings, and adjective ratings. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 821–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uher, J., Asendorpf, J. B., & Call, J. (2008). Personality in the behavior of great apes: temporal stability, cross-situational consistency and coherence in response. Animal Behaviour, 75, 99–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uher, J., Addessi, E., & Visalberghi, E. (2013a). Contextualised behavioural measurements of personality differences obtained in behavioural tests and social observations in adult capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 427–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uher, J., Werner, C. S., & Gosselt, K. (2013b). From observations of individual behaviour to social representations of personality: developmental pathways, attribution biases, and limitations of questionnaire methods. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 647–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (1987). Culture and the development of children’s actions: A cultural–historical theory of developmental psychology. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (1998). The guided mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2000). Culture and human development. London, UK: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2012). A guided science: History of psychology in the mirror of its making. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Geert, P., & van Dijk, M. (2002). Focus on variability: new tools to study intra-individual variability in developmental data. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 340–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wagoner, B. (2009). The experimental methodology of constructive microgenesis. In J. Valsiner, P. Molenaar, N. Chaudhary, & M. Lyra (Eds.), Handbook of dynamic process methodology in the social and developmental sciences (pp. 99–121). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Walach, H. (2013) Psychologie. Wissenschaftstheorie, philosophische Grundlagen und Geschichte. Ein Lehrbuch. (3., überarb. Auflage). Unter Mitarbeit von N. v. Stillfried. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

  • Weber, M. (1904). Die “Objektivität” sozialwissenschaftlicher und sozialpolitischer Erkenntnis. Archiv für Sozialwissenschaft und Sozialpolitik, 19, 22–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, M. (1949). The methodology of the social sciences [Translated and edited by E.A. Shils and H.A. Finch]. New York, NY: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, A., Adams, M. J., Widdig, A., & Gerald, M. S. (2011). Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) as living fossils of hominoid personality and subjective well-being. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125, 72–83.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Westen, D. (1996). A model and a method for uncovering the nomothetic from the idiographic: an alternative to the five-factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 400–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, A. N. (1929). Process and reality. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wong, W.-C. (2006). Understanding dialectical thinking from a cultural-historical perspective. Philosophical Psychology, 19, 239–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, W.-C. (2009). Retracing the footsteps of Wilhelm Wundt: explorations in the disciplinary frontiers of psychology and in Völkerpsychologie. History of Psychology, 12, 229–265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, J. C., & Zakriski, A. L. (2003). When syndromal similarity obscures functional dissimilarity: distinctive evoked environments of externalizing and mixed syndrome children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 516–527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1894). Über psychische Kausalität und das Prinzip des psycho-physischen Parallelismus. Philosophische Studien, 10, 1–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1896). Grundriss der Psychologie. Stuttgart: Körner. Online at https://archive.org/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1904). Principles of physiological psychology. London, UK: Allen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1921). Logik. Eine Untersuchung der Prinzipien der Erkenntnis und der Methoden Wissenschaftlicher Forschung. Band 3. Logik der Geisteswissenschaften (4. Aufl.). Stuttgart: Enke.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to the editor Jaan Valsiner for the invitation to write this trilogy and I also thank him, Jochen Fahrenberg, Sheldon Zedeck and six anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on previous drafts (previously entitled “Methodological approaches to personality taxonomies: The Behavioural Repertoire x Environmental Situations Approach—A non-lexical alternative”). The views expressed herein are mine and should not be attributed to any of the persons who provided commentaries. I gratefully acknowledge support from a research grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft DFG (Grant Number UH249/1-1).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jana Uher.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Uher, J. Developing “Personality” Taxonomies: Metatheoretical and Methodological Rationales Underlying Selection Approaches, Methods of Data Generation and Reduction Principles. Integr. psych. behav. 49, 531–589 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-014-9280-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-014-9280-4

Keywords

Navigation