Introduction

Although antineoplastic treatment options have evolved in the past decades, cancer remains a leading cause of death globally [1,2,3]. To a certain degree, physicians are able to gain insight into advanced cancer patients’ prognosis and disclose the life-limiting nature of the disease. However, recent therapeutic developments and associated altered disease outcomes challenge oncologists’ prognostic assessments [4•]. Moreover, estimating an individual’s life expectancy is undeniably complex [4•, 5]. Nevertheless, in an era of patient autonomy and shared decision-making, physicians’ provision of prognostic information is considered important for patients to make informed treatment choices [6,7,8,9]. In addition, disclosure of prognosis might be necessary for them to prepare for the end of life [10]. A vast majority of patients with metastatic cancer wishes to be informed about the expected outcome of their illness [11, 12].

Yet, internationally, half of patients with advanced cancer are not aware of their prognosis [13,14,15]. Patients with incurable cancer often misunderstand the palliative intent of their treatment and overestimate their life expectancy compared with their oncologists [7, 13, 16,17,18]. What is more, some studies indicate that these misconceptions grossly remain unchanged over time as death approaches [19, 20].

A lack of prognostic awareness could lead to decisions in disaccord with patients’ actual values [17]. While accurate prognostic understanding is commonly associated with a preference for comfort care [17, 21], engagement in advance care planning [22, 23] and improved quality of life [24,25,26,27,28], misunderstanding of prognosis is associated with the administration of aggressive anticancer therapy [16, 17, 22, 29] and life-sustaining treatment [23, 25] at the end of life. Additionally, documentation of end-of-life preferences [22, 30] and usage of hospice services [30,31,32,33] can be complicated by prognostic unawareness.

Prognostic unawareness might result from the way physicians and patients communicate [34]. Oncologists are often reluctant to discuss prognosis [15, 34,35,36,37] and seem to worry about damaging the physician-patient relationship, patients’ hope or psychological well-being [6, 38, 39]. When oncologists provide prognostic information, current literature suggests that they use imprecise qualitative terms (e.g. “months to years”) instead of quantitative point estimates (e.g. means or medians) or survival rates (e.g. percentages) [34]. Furthermore, challenged with the fine art of balancing hope and honesty, physicians regularly emphasise the presence of beneficial prognostic markers, the best case scenario and “years” instead of “months” [34, 38, 40,41,42].

Most past research has focused either on the prevalence and consequences of prognostic unawareness or on patients’ information preferences, using observational or exploratory methods [43•]. However, information on how to best engage in prognostic discussions is limited. More specifically, few studies investigate the independent effect of prognostic communication on patient outcomes in palliative oncology, or factors that influence this relationship [34]. Hence, existing guidelines are mostly based on descriptive studies or expert consensus. Finally, there is no up-to-date synthesis of literature on this specific topic [44, 45].

Comprehensive information about the effect of different approaches to prognostic communication on advanced cancer patients is needed. This effort could assist oncologists in these challenging conversations, optimise the delivery of prognostic information and enhance patient outcomes. Integrating knowledge on moderating factors could help to tailor communication to individual patients, and ultimately formulate evidence-based advice for physicians’ clinical practice. Therefore, this systematic review addresses the following research questions:

  1. I.

    What are the effects of prognostic disclosure on patient outcomes in palliative cancer care?

  2. II.

    What are the effects of different strategies to prognostic disclosure on patient outcomes in palliative cancer care?

  3. III.

    What patient and context characteristics moderate the effect of prognostic communication on patient outcomes in palliative cancer care?

Methods

The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement was used as a guideline for this systematic review [46]. No protocol was registered.

Literature search

From July till September 2019, PubMed and PsycInfo databases were searched for studies published in English, using no restriction on publication year. The following search terms were used as main index terms or free-text words: “prognosis” and “communication” or “physician patient interaction” and “neoplasms” and “palliative care”. Additionally, synonyms and closely related words were used (Appendix 1). One recently published, not yet indexed article was found through a PubMed article alert.

Eligibility criteria

Original quantitative studies describing the association between physician-patient communication about prognosis and patient outcomes in palliative cancer care were eligible. Palliative cancer care was defined as care for patients with incurable, metastatic cancer, including end-of-life care. Studies with samples partially matching the target population were only included if subgroup results were available. Communication about prognosis was defined as communicating the absence of cure, terminal nature of the disease and/or life expectancy. Papers addressing “bad news” without specifying its definition did not suffice. Communication could be real (e.g. self-reported by physicians or patients or observed in medical records, audio-recorded consultations or individually adapted consultations) or hypothetical (e.g. manipulated in video-recorded or written vignettes). Studies qualified if the independent effect of (strategies for) prognostic disclosure on (any type of) patient outcome(s) was examined. Hence, studies investigating more general interventions, such as advance care planning, early palliative care, decisions aids, question prompt lists or communication skills training, were excluded. Qualitative and non-empirical research, case reports and studies investigating minors or caregivers were also excluded.

Study selection

Duplicates were removed. Two authors (NV, MM) double screened 10% of the resulting records based on title and abstract (N = 377). These authors independently agreed on the inclusion, exclusion or the necessity to retrieve full-text papers for 96% of this sample. For 4% (N = 15), judgement differed, but only on the necessity for further evaluation. The last author (IH) was involved to jointly decide on inclusion of papers with questionable eligibility and to specify the criteria. After resolving all differences, the remaining 90% (N = 3393) of records were screened individually based on title and abstract (NV, MM). Discussion between NV, MM and IH took place in case of doubt. These authors jointly evaluated potentially relevant papers in full text (N = 54) and decided on the final inclusion with 100% agreement.

Data extraction

Data were extracted independently by NV and MM with a standardised extraction form including first author, year of publication, country, study aims, design, sample (description and size), setting, type of prognostic communication (prognostic disclosure and/or disclosure strategy), definition of prognosis (predictor), assessment of predictor, patient outcome and assessment and relevant main and moderating effects (direction and significance with p values or confidence intervals). Interaction terms and predictors of patients’ reactions to manipulated prognostic messages were regarded as moderating factors. The congruence of the independently extracted data was judged by MM. In case of doubt, discussion with NV, MM and IH took place until agreement was reached.

Quality assessment

Appendix 2 displays the quality assessment tools used. The adapted Newcastle-Ottawa Scale was adjusted to assess the quality of non-experimental studies [47, 48]. Eight items were scored with 0 to 2 points, leading to a maximum score of 16. A quality checklist for experimental studies was self-developed to suit the various designs of the included studies. Items were based on the adapted Newcastle-Ottawa Scale [47, 48], the Cochrane Consumer and Communication Review Group criteria [49, 50] and the Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias [49]. Experiments using a within-subjects design were rated with 8 items (part A). Experiments using a between-subjects design were additionally assessed with 5 more items (part B). Items were scored with 0 or 1 point, leading to a maximum score of 8 or 13 points, respectively. Self-constructed items assessing the clarity of the definition of prognostic communication were included in both quality assessment tools.

Total scores and percentages of the maximum score were calculated. Papers attaining < 50% of the maximum score were considered of relatively low quality. Scores of ≥ 50% reflected satisfactory quality. Importantly, mutual comparison is only appropriate for studies using similar designs.

Methodological quality was assessed by NV, MM and IH. After these authors reached consensus during double assessment of one third of the papers (N = 6), MM and IH evaluated the remaining papers independently (N = 12). In case of doubt, discussion between all assessors took place. Agreement on quality was reached for all studies.

Results

The search yielded 3770 non-duplicate records (Fig. 1). Eighteen papers were included. Study characteristics are shown in Table 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow diagram for article search and selection

Table 1 Characteristics and results of the included studies

Quality assessment

Considering the twelve non-experimental studies, the definition of prognosis, sampling strategy, use of validated tools for patient outcomes, statistical tests and controlling for confounders were often satisfactory. However, a justified and satisfactory sample size, and comparability of responders and non-responders, or of responders with the target population, was frequently lacking. Hence, there might be problems with power and selection bias. Eight non-experimental papers attained ≥ 50% of the maximum score. Four studies had relatively low quality (Table 2 in Appendix 3).

The manipulation of the predictor was well described in all six experimental studies; the definition of prognosis was clear in most. Additionally, randomisation, allocation, blinding of patients, comparison of groups and presence of equivalent conditions were adequate in both of the controlled trials. Blinding of the data analyst was not done in any of the experiments, and most lacked comparison of responders and non-responders, or comparison with the target population. Altogether, six papers showed methodological quality of ≥ 50% (Table 3 in Appendix 4).

Prognostic disclosure

The effect of disclosing prognosis on advanced cancer patients was investigated by thirteen studies. Patient outcomes were categorised into information-related outcomes (e.g. understanding of prognosis), physical outcomes (e.g. symptoms), physiological outcomes (e.g. arousal), psychological outcomes (e.g. depression), relational outcomes (e.g. physician-patient relationship) or care preferences (e.g. documentation of treatment choices).

Information-related outcomes

Five studies assessed the association between disclosure of incurability and/or life expectancy and patients’ prognostic understanding. All papers demonstrated significant positive associations [22, 59,58,59,62], although two examined a partially similar sample [59, 62]. Furthermore, a significant negative association between disclosure of life expectancy and length of patients’ life expectancy self-estimates, and a positive association with the accuracy of these estimates, was found [22].

Physical outcomes

One study investigated the association between disclosure of incurability and physical symptoms, such as pain, and did not show a significant difference [60].

Physiological outcomes

Two studies examined the association between disclosure of incurability and physiological outcomes. In the experiment of Sep et al. [53], a significant increase of skin conductance was detected after physicians revealed the absence of cure in a simulated bad news consultation. In a similar experiment, Danzi et al. [58] studied activation of the autonomic nervous system through cardiac measures. Findings suggested significantly reduced parasympathetic activity, as well as increased sympathetic activity, among a part of the sample following disclosure of incurability [58]. Increased physiological arousal, revealed in both studies, can be regarded as a marker of emotional stress [53, 58].

Psychological outcomes

Five studies investigated the association between prognostic disclosure and psychological outcomes, of which two had relatively low quality (< 50%) [63, 64]. Cripe et al. [63] discovered a significant negative association of patient-rated extent of discussions about life expectancy with depressive symptoms. Moreover, depressive symptoms were less prevalent among respondents perceiving a worse prognosis when a full (versus no or a brief) discussion about life expectancy had taken place [63]. Similarly, more conversations about the likely disease outcome were significantly associated with less depressive symptoms [65]. Regarding anxiety, significantly lower levels were found in patients and their family when incurability was disclosed [60]. Other findings did not show differences in depression [22, 63], anxiety [22, 63,62,65] or feelings of confusion, insecurity, support or confidence in relation to prognostic discussions [64]. In contrast with previous findings, Cripe et al. [63] additionally revealed significantly more anxiety among respondents who perceived a worse prognosis after a full (versus no or a brief) discussion about life expectancy. Another study discovered more distress in patients who requested cure rates and survival estimates during an individually adapted consultation compared with patients who did not [64].

Relational outcomes

Four studies examined the association of prognostic conversations with relational outcomes. One showed a significant positive association between the disclosure of incurability and communication between patients, family and professionals [60]. Another study demonstrated significantly increased therapeutic alliance 3 months after discussions about incurability and survival [66]. Therapeutic alliance did not change from baseline to 2 days to 7 days after the consultation, nor did patients’ confidence in obtaining information and attention of their physicians at any time point [66]. A third paper revealed no significant association between conversations about life expectancy and the physician-patient relationship [22]. Finally, satisfaction with an individually adapted consultation did not differ between patients receiving requested information about cure rates and life expectancy and patients who denied such information [64].

Care preferences

Three studies assessed the association between prognostic disclosure and care preferences. Bradley et al. [56] demonstrated that patients reporting conversations about life expectancy were more likely to have had physician-patient discussions about life-sustaining treatment preferences. In a sample mainly consisting of terminal cancer patients, a significant association between the disclosure of patients’ life-limiting illness and discussing care preferences with family, as well as a positive trend for this discussion with physicians, was observed in bivariate analyses only [61]. One study additionally revealed a significant association between disclosure and the documentation of a do-not-resuscitate order [56]. Another paper, with relatively low quality, could not confirm this result, nor were significant differences found in length of the last admission before death or the administration of sedation near death [67]. No significant associations between prognostic disclosure and documentation of a living will, health care proxies or durable power of attorney for health care were reported either [56, 61].

Disclosure strategies

Different strategies for prognostic communication, potentially influencing the effect of disclosure, were investigated by nine studies. Disclosure strategies were categorised into the provision of explicit prognostic information, framing of prognostic information, affective communication and general communication behaviours. Patient outcomes were categorised as previously reported.

Explicitness of prognostic information

Five papers assessed the association between explicit prognostic information and patient outcomes. Explicit communication encompassed disclosure of more specific prognostic information (e.g. life expectancy in addition to incurability) [60] or provision of quantitative (e.g. survival rate, median survival time and/or range) instead of, or supplemental to, qualitative survival information [54, 57, 64, 68].

Considering information-related outcomes, one cross-sectional study noted significantly better recognition of disease condition among patients who received more specific prognostic information [60]. Concerning physical outcomes, specific prognostic information was not associated with physical symptoms in general, nor with pain or any other specific symptoms [60]. Regarding psychological outcomes, uncertainty was significantly lower after the communication of quantitative versus qualitative information (in bivariate [57] and multivariate [54] analyses). Van Vliet and colleagues [54] additionally revealed enhanced self-efficacy regarding patients’ ability to deal with the future following quantitative survival estimates, although Mori et al. [57] could not detect a similar effect. None of the included studies discovered a significant impact of explicitness on anxiety, distress, confusion, confidence, insecurity or feeling overwhelmed [54, 57, 60, 64]. Advanced cancer patients rated the provision of statistics about life expectancy evenly hopeful, neutral and not hopeful [68]. In terms of relational outcomes, communication between patients, family and professionals was significantly better for patients receiving more specific prognostic information [60]. Furthermore, two experimental studies demonstrated more satisfaction with communication of quantitative instead of qualitative survival estimates [54, 57]. Another paper reported no significant difference in patients’ satisfaction with requested qualitative or supplemental quantitative information [64]. Lastly, studying care preferences, explicitness did not affect patients’ willingness to discuss advance care planning with their oncologist [57].

Framing of prognostic information

Two experimental studies assessed the effect of framing prognostic information on patient outcomes by incorporating pessimistic, neutral or optimistic statements [69] or using the worst, typical and best case scenario to explain life expectancy [70].

Concerning information-related outcomes, one study showed that patients were generally more optimistic about their prognosis than their physicians but were more likely to agree with their oncologist’s estimated chance of cure when physicians made at least one statement of pessimism about the disease outcome [69]. Statements of optimism and uncertainty did not influence physician-patient concordance about the estimated chance of cure [69]. Furthermore, significantly more patients agreed that using the worst, typical and best case scenario to explain life expectancy, versus communicating median survival time, would improve their prognostic understanding, make sense, be helpful, help family and carers and help in making plans for the future [70]. In terms of psychological outcomes, significantly more patients agreed that presenting multiple survival scenarios would be reassuring, hopeful and less upsetting and would decrease anxiety, compared with median survival time [70].

Affective prognostic communication

Three experimental studies examined the impact of affective prognostic communication on patient outcomes [53, 54, 58]. The effect of affective communication was investigated by comparing respondents’ reactions to video-recorded simulated bad news consultations, including communication about survival and treatment options, with and without physicians’ reassurance of non-abandonment [54] and support [53, 58]. The absence of affective messages was referred to as standard communication [53, 54, 58].

Concerning information-related outcomes, one randomised controlled trial discovered better recall during affective versus standard communication [53], while another could not find a similar effect on self-perceived recall ability or actual recall [58]. Regarding physiological outcomes, affective communication led to a significantly stronger decrease of physiological arousal compared with standard communication, based on skin conductance levels [53]. Findings on cardiac measures varied, suggesting more sympathetic as well as parasympathetic activity of the autonomic nervous system during standard versus affective communication [58]. Combining information-related and physiological outcomes, both trials investigated whether the association of physiological arousal with recall differed between the affective and standard communication groups. Based on decreased skin conductance levels [53] and increased parasympathetic activity [58], reduced physiological arousal only led to improved recall during affective communication. However, based on increased heart rate variability, reduced physiological arousal only led to improved recall during standard instead of affective communication [58]. In terms of psychological outcomes, anxiety and uncertainty scores were significantly lower following physicians’ reassurance of non-abandonment, while self-efficacy was significantly higher compared with standard communication [54]. Lastly, relational outcomes reflected participants’ perceptions of the oncologist’s behaviour in the simulated consultation. Affective communication, as compared to standard communication, led to significantly higher rates of satisfaction, perceived physician’s support, non-abandonment, reassurance and empathy, but not engagement with the patient [53, 54, 58].

General communication behaviours

Hagerty and colleagues [68] investigated wide-ranging communication behaviours of physicians, which might influence hope during prognostic discussions. Physicians offering the most up-to-date treatment, appearing to know everything about a patient’s cancer, using humour occasionally, telling pain will be controlled and communicating all treatment options were rated as most hope giving by advanced cancer patients. Those communication behaviours were labelled as expert, positive and collaborative approaches. In contrast, physicians appearing to be nervous, giving prognosis to family first, using euphemisms, avoiding talking about cancer, only discussing treatment and giving good news before bad news were not perceived as hope conveying in this experimental study. Those behaviours were labelled avoidant. Similar to providing survival statistics, physicians’ expression of uncertainty about the disease course was rated evenly hopeful, neutral and not hopeful by metastatic cancer patients [68].

Moderating factors

Factors moderating the effect of disclosure strategies on patient outcomes were investigated by three studies. Considering explicit prognostic communication, patients with a strong monitoring coping style (i.e. often seeking detailed information) were significantly more anxious and uncertain and less self-efficacious and satisfied after explicitness than patients with lower monitoring scores [54]. A blunting coping style (i.e. often avoiding information) did not alter the effect of explicit survival information on patient outcomes [54]. With respect to framing prognostic information, higher educated patients were significantly more likely to agree that a typical case scenario, as well as a best case scenario, should be included when communicating life expectancy. Higher education was also associated with agreeing that the best case scenario conveyed hope, and that explaining three scenarios was helpful. No associations were found between education and agreeing that all scenarios were easy to understand [70]. According to the same study, females were significantly more likely to agree that all survival scenarios would help in making plans for the future, and that explaining a worst case scenario was upsetting. Being diagnosed with cancer more than 1 year ago and age younger than 70 were associated with the latter opinion as well. Furthermore, breast cancer patients were significantly less likely to agree that communicating a typical, best and worst case scenario was reassuring, as compared to patients with different primary tumour sites. Instead they were more likely to find this strategy upsetting [70]. Finally, concerning general communication behaviours, more anxious patients rated expert, positive, collaborative and empathic approaches to communicate prognosis (e.g. appearing to know everything, working as a team, expressing feelings) as significantly more hope conveying than less anxious patients [68]. The same result was found for older versus younger patients [68].

Discussion

Main findings

Prognostic disclosure

Integrating the results of all included studies examining prognostic disclosure, and considering methodological quality, evidence shows an association with improved prognostic awareness [22, 59,58,59,62]. This suggests that the previously mentioned high rate of prognostic unawareness [13,14,15] could reasonably be addressed by physicians through prognostic discussions. Moreover, Yun and colleagues [28] illustrate that it might be important for physicians to inform patients of their terminal status, as patients who became aware by their worsening condition or by chance reported lower quality of life.

Addressing oncologists’ worries about harming patients through disclosure [6, 38, 39], the available evidence does not seem to confirm or deny this concern indisputably. Based on two experiments of satisfactory quality, disclosing incurability likely increases immediate physiological arousal [53, 58], indicating that the confrontation with prognostic information affects patients emotionally in that very moment. Literature examining the association of disclosure with longer-term psychological outcomes is limited and of varying quality. Furthermore, these findings are mixed, for which explanations are not found. Taken with caution, however, most point to either no or positive associations between prognostic disclosure and psychological well-being [22, 60, 63,62,65], which might be reassuring for physicians.

The reported discrepancy between physiological indices of emotional stress and psychological self-report measures could reflect the difference between immediate and delayed responses. Still, it should be taken into consideration that existing measures may not be sensitive enough to capture the complex emotional consequences of prognostic disclosure. Moreover, the effect of prognostic communication is often examined at group level, possibly disregarding subgroups with different reactions.

Accounting for physicians’ worries about the physician-patient relationship [6, 38, 39], two papers show no or positive associations with communicating life expectancy [22, 66]. Additionally, three studies hint to a potential positive association between disclosure and physician-patient communication in general [60] or the discussion of care preferences specifically [56, 61]. Hence, these articles of satisfactory quality suggest that prognostic communication could strengthen the relation and stimulate patients’ role in decision-making [61]. Sufficient evidence for an association between disclosure and the documentation of care preferences, actual care provided or physical outcomes could not be established [56, 60, 61, 67]. Remarkably, even though a key argument for prognostic disclosure is informed decision-making, very few papers investigated the association between prognostic communication and treatment decision-making.

Disclosure strategies

The limited number of studies, variety of examined strategies and hypothetical nature of the included experiments complicate drawing conclusions about the effect of different disclosure strategies. Hence, further research is needed to formulate strong recommendations. Based on the existing literature, we tentatively identify four approaches to prognostic communication.

The first strategy encompasses physicians’ provision of more explicit, rather than imprecise, prognostic information. Three studies of satisfactory (and one with lower) quality suggest either no or a beneficial effect of explicitness on patients’ psychological well-being and satisfaction with the consultation [54, 57, 60, 64]. One indicates improved recognition of disease condition [60]. According to the broader literature, being transparent about the difficulty of formulating individual survival estimates is considered helpful when communicating prognosis [44, 54]. Nonetheless, individual differences should be acknowledged, as another paper shows equally large proportions of respondents rating the provision of statistics and communication of uncertainty hopeful, neutral and not hopeful [68].

Second, physicians’ tendency to stress the best case scenario, possibly inducing patients’ overestimation of life expectancy [34, 38, 40, 41], could be complemented with a typical and worst case scenario. Both multiple survival scenarios and pessimistic statements improve patients’ prognostic awareness, according to two studies of satisfactory quality [69, 70]. This realistic strategy might tackle oncologists’ fear of leading patients to focus on a single number [34, 39], help patients to hope for the best, but prepare for the worst [34], and may prevent troublesome consequences of prognostic unawareness [16, 17, 22, 29,30,31,32,33].

A third disclosure strategy involves physicians’ reassurance about non-abandonment and support while communicating life expectancy, investigated by three experiments of satisfactory quality. Findings hint to a beneficial effect of affective communication on patients’ physiological arousal, psychological well-being, satisfaction with the consultation and most measures of perceived physician’s support [53, 54, 58]. The effect of affective communication on recall of information, as well as the joint relation with physiological arousal, remains inconclusive [53, 58]. Nonetheless, this approach should satisfy patients’ need to be looked after and allow for a sense of hope [54].

Fourth, hope might be fostered by physicians through expert, positive and collaborative behaviour during prognostic communication [68]. Incorporating wider research, oncologists are advised to address other sources of hope as well, rather than just medical information, like faith, inner peace, dignity, meaningful life events, relationships or humour [44, 54].

The combination of explicit and affective communication, multiple survival scenarios and expert, positive, collaborative behaviour might be most promising, although more scientific support is needed.

Moderating factors

Substantial evidence to identify patient and context characteristics that determine individual reactions to prognostic communication is limited. One study demonstrates that patients who tend to seek detailed information are more anxious and uncertain, and less self-efficacious and satisfied following explicitness [54]. An explanation to this counterintuitive finding might be high monitors’ generally lower satisfaction with information compared with patients showing a low monitoring coping style [54, 71, 72]. Another study suggests that higher educated patients are more likely to find explaining three survival scenarios helpful, and that breast cancer patients are more likely to find this strategy upsetting [70]. An obvious explanation for these findings was not provided, and the authors recommend communicating multiple scenarios to all advanced cancer patients preferring disclosure nevertheless [70]. Lastly, one paper indicates that more anxious and older patients particularly feel hopeful after prognostic communication displaying expertise, positivity, collaboration and empathy [68]. The latter is in line with earlier research, confirming that sensitive communication with emotional support is especially important for older cancer patients [73].

Limitations

This paper presents a novel attempt to offer comprehensive, evidence-based guidance for physicians’ clinical practice by examining the independent effect of prognostic communication on advanced cancer patients. Despite the suggested disclosure strategies, some patients might prefer and fare better with prognostic ignorance [11, 74, 75]. Reasonably, some experts in the field make a case for prognostic silence, emphasising its protective function [76•, 77]. Exploring a patient’s personal information preferences beforehand, indeed, is essential in prognostic conversations [44, 54]. Caregivers’ information needs should be addressed additionally, as potential dissimilarities could influence prognostic discussions [44].

The reviewed studies have some limitations, which should be taken into account when interpreting the results. Firstly, prognostic disclosure is often assessed with self-report or medical record registration instead of direct observations, which might not reflect the actual extent of prognostic discussions [22, 59]. Secondly, the used study designs entail restrictions. Non-experimental study designs cannot imply causation [63, 66], whereas the controlled context of experiments diminishes the complexity of clinical interactions [54, 57]. Furthermore, respondents acting as analogue patients in experimental studies may react differently than actual patients [53, 54]. Still, since manipulation of prognostic communication in real-life settings is ethically unfeasible, we have to rely on the combination of these study types.

The current paper has shortcomings too. The authors solely searched literature indexed by PubMed and PsycInfo, possibly excluding relevant articles in other databases. Furthermore, attempts to facilitate prognostic communication by means of interventions are not discussed. Studies on advance care planning, early palliative care, decision aids, question prompt lists or communication skills training may provide supplemental information on the effect of prognostic messages.

Future directions

Considering the limits of literature to-date, future research should expand knowledge about the influence of different prognostic communication formats on patients to convey more pronounced advice, with a focus on psychological well-being over time and treatment decision-making. Studies should attempt to overcome the one-size-fits-all approach, by exploring patients’ individual information preferences and differences in reactions to prognostic communication. Knowledge of moderators, as well as enhanced prognostic prediction models, could assist oncologists in tailoring their messages. Finally, future research should optimise the development and implementation of communication interventions to put guidelines into practice and ultimately improve prognostic communication.

Conclusions

Altogether, this systematic review synthesised today’s literature on the effect of prognostic communication on patient outcomes in palliative cancer care and moderators of this relation. Addressing the research aims, we conclude cautiously that, if preferred by patients, oncologists can disclose prognosis using sensible strategies. Displaying expertise, positivity and collaboration, while offering explicit prognostic information with multiple survival scenarios and reassurance of support, likely offers an inclusive approach physicians can rely on.

Opinion statement

Making more deliberate communicative choices starts with physicians’ awareness of their propensity to deliver ambiguous messages. Based on current knowledge and ethical principles, we recommend oncologists to communicate prognosis to patients who wish to know, using a balanced approach. Allowing for preparation as well as hope, we advise mentioning the worst, typical and best case scenario of survival, instead of one number or the optimistic scenario only. Worries about harming patients by truth-telling should not prevent physicians from at least offering prognostic information, as these worries might also originate from personal fears or discomfort. We stimulate physicians to acknowledge patients’ emotions, which often reflect a normal reaction to life-changing messages, and may not necessarily persist. Additionally, we encourage physicians to support patients by reassuring non-abandonment and fostering different types of hope. Oncologists can incorporate the uncertainty of individual estimates by stressing the possible deviation from group-based survival information. Finally, we emphasise the need to individualise care. It is essential to be aware of patients’ frame of reference and to recognise potential differences with one’s own social, cultural and religious context. Discussing prognosis is considered an ongoing process. Therefore, individual information needs have to be explored timely, thoroughly and repeatedly. Subsequently tailoring to patients’ preferences would be most advantageous.