Skip to main content
Log in

Causes of variation of darkness in flocks of starlings, a computational model

  • Published:
Swarm Intelligence Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The coordinated motion of large flocks of starlings is fascinating for both laymen and scientists. During their aerial displays, the darkness of flocks often changes, for instance dark bands propagate through the flock (so-called agitation waves) and small or large parts of the flock darken. The causes of dark bands in agitation waves have recently been shown to depend on changes in orientation of birds relative to the observer rather than changes in density of the flock, but what causes other changes in darkness need to be studied still and this is the aim of the present investigation. Because we cannot empirically relate changes in darkness in flocks to quantities, such as position and orientation of the flock and of its members relative to the observer, we study this in a computational model. We use StarDisplay, a model of collective motion of starlings, because its flocks resemble empirical data in many properties, such as their three-dimensional shape, their manner of turning, the correlation of heading of its group-members, and its internal structure regarding density and stability of neighbors. We show that the change in darkness in the flocks perceived by an observer on the ground mostly depends on the observer’s distance to the flock and on the degree of exposure of the wing surface of flock members to the observer, and that darkness appears to decrease when birds roll during sharp turns. Remarkably, the darkness of the flock perceived by the observer was neither affected by the orientation of the flock relative to the observer nor by the density of the flock. Further studies are needed to investigate changes in darkness for flocks under predation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Attanasi, A., Cavagna, A., Del Castello, L., Giardina, I., Jelic, A., Melillo, S., Parisi, L., Pohl, O., Shen, E., & Viale, M. (2015). Emergence of collective changes in travel direction of starling flocks from individual birds’ fluctuations. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 12(108), 20150319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Axelsen, B. E., Anker-Nilssen, T., Fossum, P., Kvamme, C., & Nøttestad, L. (2001). Pretty patterns but a simple strategy: Predator-prey interactions between juvenile herring and Atlantic puffins observed with multibeam sonar. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 79(9), 1586–1596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ballerini, M., Cabibbo, N., Candelier, R., Cavagna, A., Cisbani, E., Giardina, I., Orlandi, A., Parisi, G., Procaccini, A., Viale, M., & Zdravkovic, V. (2008a). Empirical investigation of starling flocks: A benchmark study in collective animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 76(1), 201–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ballerini, M., Cabibbo, N., Candelier, R., Cavagna, A., Cisbani, E., Giardina, I., Lecomte, V., Orlandi, A., Parisi, G., Procaccini, A., Viale, M., & Zdravkovic, V. (2008b). Interaction ruling animal collective behavior depends on topological rather than metric distance: Evidence from a field study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(4), 1232–1237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialek, W., Cavagna, A., Giardina, I., Mora, T., Silvestri, E., Viale, M., & Walczak, A. M. (2012). Statistical mechanics for natural flocks of birds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(13), 4786–4791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bode, N. W., Franks, D. W., & Wood, A. J. (2011). Limited interactions in flocks: Relating model simulations to empirical data. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 8(55), 301–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carere, C., Montanino, S., Moreschini, F., Zoratto, F., Chiarotti, F., Santucci, D., & Alleva, E. (2009). Aerial flocking patterns of wintering starlings, Sturnus vulgaris, under different predation risk. Animal Behaviour, 77(1), 101–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavagna, A., Cimarelli, A., Giardina, I., Parisi, G., Santagati, R., Stefanini, F., & Viale, M. (2010). Scale-free correlations in starling flocks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(26), 11865–11870.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavagna, A., Queirós, S. D., Giardina, I., Stefanini, F., & Viale, M. (2013). Diffusion of individual birds in starling flocks. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1756), 20122484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaté, H., Ginelli, F., Grégoire, G., Peruani, F., & Raynaud, F. (2008). Modeling collective motion: Variations on the Vicsek model. The European Physical Journal B, 64(3–4), 451–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cleveland Bent, A. (1927). Life histories of North American shore birds (No. 598.3397 C5). Dover Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronin, T. W., Johnsen, S., Marshall, N. J., & Warrant, E. J. (2014). Visual ecology. Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, J. M. (1980). The coordinated aerobatics of dunlin flocks. Animal Behaviour, 28(3), 668–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costanzo, A., & Hemelrijk, C. K. (2018). Spontaneous emergence of milling (vortex state) in a Vicsek-like model. Journal of Physics D: Applied Physics, 51(13), 134004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Couzin, I. D., Krause, J., James, R., Ruxton, G. D., & Franks, N. R. (2002). Collective memory and spatial sorting in animal groups. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 218(1), 1–11.

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Feare, C. J. (1984). The starling. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillies, J., Bacic, M., Thomas, A., Taylor, G., & Yuan, F. (2008). Modeling and identification of steppe eagle (Aquila nipalensis) dynamics. In: AIAA Modeling and Simulation Technologies Conference and Exhibit (p. 7096).

  • Hemelrijk, C. K., & Hildenbrandt, H. (2011). Some causes of the variable shape of flocks of birds. PloS one, 6(8), e22479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hemelrijk, C. K., & Hildenbrandt, H. (2012). Schools of fish and flocks of birds: Their shape and internal structure by self-organization. Interface Focus, 2(6), 726–737.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hemelrijk, C. K., & Hildenbrandt, H. (2015). Diffusion and topological neighbours in flocks of starlings: Relating a model to empirical data. PLoS One, 10(5), e0126913.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hemelrijk, C. K., & Hildenbrandt, H. (2015b). Scale-free correlations, influential neighbours and speed control in flocks of birds. Journal of Statistical Physics, 158(3), 563–578.

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Hemelrijk, C. K., van Zuidam, L., & Hildenbrandt, H. (2015). What underlies waves of agitation in starling flocks. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 69(5), 755–764.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hildenbrandt, H., Carere, C., & Hemelrijk, C. K. (2010). Self-organized aerial displays of thousands of starlings: A model. Behavioral Ecology, 21(6), 1349–1359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krause, J., Ruxton, G. D., Ruxton, G., & Ruxton, I. G. (2002). Living in groups. Oxford University Press.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Kunz, H., Züblin, T., & Hemelrijk, C. K. (2006). On prey grouping and predator confusion in artificial fish schools. In: Proceedings of the Tenth International Conference of Artificial Life. MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.

  • Nagy, M., Ákos, Z., Biro, D., & Vicsek, T. (2010). Hierarchical group dynamics in pigeon flocks. Nature, 464(7290), 890–893.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, D. J., Miller, A. M., Rowlands, G., & Turner, M. S. (2014). Role of projection in the control of bird flocks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(29), 10422–10426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Major, P. F., & Dill, L. M. (1978). The three-dimensional structure of airborne bird flocks. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 4(2), 111–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Procaccini, A., Orlandi, A., Cavagna, A., Giardina, I., Zoratto, F., Santucci, D., Chiarotti, F., Hemelrijk, C. K., Alleva, E., Parisi, G., & Carere, C. (2011). Propagating waves in starling, Sturnus vulgaris, flocks under predation. Animal Behaviour, 82(4), 759–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pomeroy, H., & Heppner, F. (1992). Structure of turning in airborne rock dove (Columba livia) flocks. The Auk, 109(2), 256–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potts, W. K. (1984). The chorus-line hypothesis of manoeuvre coordination in avian flocks. Nature, 309(5966), 344–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storms, R. F., Carere, C., Zoratto, F., & Hemelrijk, C. K. (2019). Complex patterns of collective escape in starling flocks under predation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 73(1), 10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strandburg-Peshkin, A., Twomey, C. R., Bode, N. W., Kao, A. B., Katz, Y., Ioannou, C. C., & Couzin, I. D. (2013). Visual sensory networks and effective information transfer in animal groups. Current Biology, 23(17), R709–R711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sumpter, D. J. (2010). Collective animal behavior. Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vicsek, T., & Zafeiris, A. (2012). Collective motion. Physics Reports, 517(3–4), 71–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zoratto, F., Carere, C., Chiarotti, F., Santucci, D., & Alleva, E. (2010). Aerial hunting behaviour and predation success by peregrine falcons Falco peregrinus on starling flocks Sturnus vulgaris. Journal of Avian Biology, 41(4), 427–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This study was funded by NWO-TTW (Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research), Project nr 14723.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to A. Costanzo.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

This article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals performed by any of the authors.

Consent for publication

All author gave their consent.

Data availability

https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.8273549

Code availability

Available upon request.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 271 kb)

The video shows a flock of 5000 birds observed from the side. The centre of the roosting site is at position (0,0,0) and the observer is in position (100,0,0)

Appendix

Appendix

In the appendix we show additional graphs reporting the darkness as a function of the distance d from a static flock of 10.000 individuals (Fig. 7) and the darkness as a function of the parameter sigma of the Gaussian filter (Fig. 8).

Fig. 7
figure 7

Darkness as a function of distance d from the static flock of 10.000 individuals. Dashed line: flock showing maximal wing surface: center of mass of the flock at (0,0,d), average heading vector (1,0,0), average up vector (0,0,1), and camera at (0,0,0). Solid line: flock showing minimal wing surface center of mass of the flock at (d,0,150), average heading vector (1,0,0), average up vector (0,0,1), and camera at (0,0,150). Solid vertical lines are guides to the eye that indicate the range of distances reached in the simulations of moving flocks

Fig. 8
figure 8

Darkness as a function of the parameter sigma of the Gaussian filter. Flock of 10.000 individuals with center of mass at (0,0,250), mean heading vector (1,0,0), and mean up vector (0,0,1), NND = 0.6 (dashed lines) and NND = 1.0 (solid lines). Right panel is a zoom on same data. The value of sigma = 3 chosen for our simulations is a value small enough to get a difference in darkness for different nearest neighbor distances, which confirms that the value of sigma = 3, that was chosen so that the flock was considered as a whole, because white pixels between birds become non-white, is a good choice for our simulations. A smaller value of sigma would produce white pixels between birds such that we could not identify and measure the area of the flock. A larger value of sigma would have the negative effect of flocks with unprecise contours and flocks with different nearest neighbor distances producing the same darkness

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Costanzo, A., Hildenbrandt, H. & Hemelrijk, C.K. Causes of variation of darkness in flocks of starlings, a computational model. Swarm Intell 16, 91–105 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11721-021-00207-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11721-021-00207-4

Keywords

Navigation