Skip to main content
Log in

On diminutives and plurals in Moroccan Arabic

  • Published:
Morphology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In Moroccan Arabic, like in many other Afroasiatic languages, certain nouns may have more than one plural form. For instance, t ʕ es ʕ wera ‘photo’ has plurals t ʕ s ʕ awer and t ʕ s ʕ werat. However, their diminutive systematically resorts to -at suffixation in the plural. The aim of this study is twofold. First, it presents an interface approach which aims to determine the structural location of number and capture the empirical contrast between broken and sound plurals. It is argued that the sound plurals are associated with the standard Num projection, whereas the broken plurals are associated lower in the structure with the n projection. Second, it provides a templatic analysis of the diminutive formation, showing that the diminutive and the internal plural markers compete for the same templatic position. External evidence for the analysis is drawn from the phenomenon of emphasis spread. The nP is presented as the maximal domain of emphasis spread in nouns.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. One might argue that affix ordering in double plurals runs counter the hypothesis put forth here since the suffix -ó, which derives regular plurals, is added before -yáal. Yet these formations differ from the ones at hand in MA since they involve double pluralization. As far as the parallel holds, we can assume that Somali plurals using -ó are generated under nP (see Lecarme 2002: 193), similarly to what we propose for MA internal plurals.

  2. One of the anonymous reviewers rightly pointed out that certain numerals in MA select broken plurals. For instance, tlata ‘three’ can be used with broken plurals, as opposed to ʒuʒ ‘two’ which combines with sound plurals. In fact, these numerals behave differently since they involve distinct syntactic structures.

    1. (i)
      figure b

    As the careful reader will have noticed, a genitive preposition d intervenes between tlata and the following broken plural but not between ʒuʒ and its sound plural. The complement of the genitive preposition when plural can be only internal, with a collective reading which requires a numeral in order to be quantified. For the sake of completeness, we should also note that the absence of the genitive preposition in (b) leads to a construct state formation where the sound plural t ʕ əs ʕ werat occurs without a determiner, as opposed to the broken plural tt ʕ s ʕ awər whose geminate results from the concatenation of the determiner l- and the stem-initial consonant.

  3. One could argue that ʕəd ʕ mat alternates with feminine singular ʕəd ʕ ma whereas masculine ʕd ʕ əm has plural ʕd ʕ am, therefore suggesting that masculine forms select only internal plurals while feminine forms opt for external plurals. This correspondence is, however, far from being systematic as we find plenty of feminine nouns which resort to internal plural formation. Singular forms ending with -a in (4) are actually feminine but they all have internal plurals as well as external ones. The behaviour of feminine nouns clearly shows that gender specification is irrelevant in the choice of the type of plural formation.

  4. The reader is referred to Lowenstamm (2008), Acquaviva (2008) and Kramer (2012) for further details about number and plurality.

  5. See Marantz (2001, 2007, 2013) and Arad (2003, 2005) who show that words derived by combining a root and a category-defining head are more prone to morpho-phonological and semantic irregularities, as opposed to words derived from other words.

  6. In MA, mass nouns are singular just as English water and sugar. This is expressed through their agreement:

    1. (i)
      figure e

    Their plural, systematically formed by means of -at suffixation, makes them count. This further argues for the fact that sound plurals indicate a definite number.

  7. Some authors, arguing that MA does not allow complex onsets, view these forms as having three syllables, (Boudlal 2001; Kiparsky 2003; Dell and Elmedlaoui 2002). A form like bnita is thus syllabified [b.ni.ta], where /b/ stands for a syllable of its own (see also Shaw et al. 2009).

  8. It should be noted here that j-gemination is subject to variation. According to Elmdari (1999: 82), forms like in (4c–e) display a non-geminated glide in the MA variety of Marrakech.

  9. Such prosodic analyses are tenable only at the expense of stipulating that neither ambisyllabic geminates nor word-final consonants contribute to weight. In addition, as Boudlal (2001: 253) noticed, prosodic circumscription fails to account for forms like ʒməl ‘camel’ since the diminutive suffix should be added after the circumscribed syllable məl, leading to ʒəmli rather than ʒmijjəl. Similar problems arise with biconsonantal bases such as bit ‘room’, in which neither a CV nor a CəC syllable type is available.

  10. The idea of reducing the number of templates in the verbal conjugation of Classical Arabic is not new. McCarthy (1979: 135) has already suggested expressing the regularities that the verbal forms and their canonical patterns show by means of two templates: CV((CV)[+seg])CVC and CCV([+seg])CVC. The first template abbreviates the patterns CVCVC, CVCCVC, CVVCVC, CVCVCCVC and CVCVVCVC. The second abbreviates the patterns CCVCVC, CCVCCVC and CCVVCVC.

  11. It is worth noting that no single semantic feature is associated with the derivational CV: verbs using medial-consonant gemination may be intensive, causative or just transitive, while those with a long vowel are not always reciprocal (e.g. sa:fara ‘he travelled’, d ʕ a:ʕafa ‘he doubled’).

  12. In this respect, it is interesting to note that nouns differ from verbs in that the latter use internal morphology between the first two root consonants, while nouns generally resort to infixation between the last two consonants (e.g. kita:b ‘book’, rima:l ‘sands’)

  13. This phenomenon is not limited to diminutives. It is also found in the plural formation (e.g. ʒili (sg) / ʒilijjat (pl) ‘vest’, biru (sg) / biruwwat (pl) ‘office’). Further examples and discussion is found in Boudlal (2001: 280). It should be noted that in certain varieties of MA, the glide following the diminutive marker surfaces as non-geminated. The reader is referred to Elmdari (1999: 82) about the MA variety of Marrakech.

  14. As stated earlier in Sect. 1, MA does show a few double plurals, such as mwasat ‘knives’ (plural of plural mwas) and qwasat ‘arches’ (plural of plural qwas). This supports our assumption that both nP and numP are the locus of plurality in MA.

  15. Perlmutter (1988) analyzed the internal plural marker -əm as part of suppletive forms, lexically specified as such. Alternatives to this approach are proposed in Lowenstamm (2008) and Newell (2008).

  16. For a thorough analysis of morpho-phonological and semantic irregularities within the domain of category-defining projections, close to the root, the reader is referred to Marantz (2013). See also Arad (2003) about the interpretation of roots in Hebrew.

  17. Further evidence for the phase-hood character of category-determining projections in MA comes from glide-vowel alternations. Like Berber (see Guerssel 1986; Lahrouchi 2013), MA has many instances of glide-vowel alternations which call for an analysis in terms of phasal derivation. Compare, for instance, xu ‘brother’ to xwatat ‘sisters’. The high vowel in the first form turns into a glide since it is immediately followed by the vowel -a. This alternation is automatic within a single domain, namely nP. However, in certain cases the same vowel remains unchanged as in xu-ja ‘my brother’, while the possessive clitic turns its vowel -i into [j] in order to avoid hiatus. The reason that the high vowel u alternates with the glide w in xu / xwatat but not in xu / xu-ja can easily be explained if we assume that the alternation is automatic within the nP phase. In xu-ja, the segments following /u/, including the possessive clitic -i(a), have no access to the phonological material already spelled-out.

References

  • Acquaviva, P. (2008). Lexical plurals: A morphosemantic approach. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Al Ghadi, A. (1990). Moroccan Arabic plurals and the organization of the lexicon. D.E.S thesis, Université Mohammed 5, Faculté des Lettres, Rabat.

  • Ali, L., & Daniloff, R. (1972). A contrastive cinefluorographic investigation of the articulation of emphatic-non emphatic cognate consonants. Studia Linguistica, 26, 81–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arad, M. (2003). Locality constraints on the interpretations of roots. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 21, 737–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arad, M. (2005). Roots and patterns: Hebrew morpho-syntax. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M. (1985). The mirror principle and morphosyntactic explanation. Linguistic Inquiry, 16(3), 373–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benhallam, A. (1980). Syllable structure and rule types in Arabic. PhD dissertation, University of Florida.

  • Bennis, S. (1992). La formation du causatif en arabe marocain. Mémoire de D.E.S. Rabat: Faculté des Lettres.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booij, G. (2002). The morphology of Dutch. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borer, H. (2005). In name only, structuring sense, vol. I, Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudlal, A. (2001). Constraint interaction in the phonology and morphology of Casablanca Moroccan Arabic. Doctorat d’etat thesis, Mohammed V University, Rabat.

  • Chomsky, N. (2001). Derivation by phase. In M. Kenstowicz (Ed.), Ken Hale: A life in language (pp. 1–52). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2008). On phases. In R. Freidin, C. P. Otero, & M. L. Zubizarreta (Eds.), Foundational issues in linguistic theory: Essays in honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud (pp. 133–166). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, S. (1995). Emphasis spread and grounded phonology. Linguistic Inquiry, 26, 465–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Belder, M., Faust, N., & Lampitelli, N. (2015). On a low and a high diminutive: Evidence from Italian and Hebrew. In A. Alexiadou, H. Borer, & F. Schäfer (Eds.), The syntax of roots and the roots of syntax (pp. 149–163). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, F., & Elmedlaoui, M. (2002). Syllables in Tashlhiyt Berber and in Moroccan Arabic. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Elmdari, F. (1999). Aspects phonologiques et morphologiques du parler de Marrakech. PhD dissertation, Paris 8 University.

  • Embick, D., & Marantz, A. (2008). Architecture and blocking. Linguistic Inquiry, 39, 1–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Embick, D., & Noyer, R. (2007). Distributed morphology and the syntax/morphology interface. In Gillian Ramchand & C. Reiss (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic interfaces (pp. 289–324). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frajzyngier, Z. (2003). Tone and vowel deletion, insertion, and syllable structure. In R.–J. Anyanwu (Ed.), Stress and tone: The African experience. Frankfurter afrikanistische Blätter (Vol. 15, pp. 83–98). Cologne: Koeppe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frajzyngier, Z. (2008). A grammar of Gidar. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghazali, S. (1981). La diffusion de l’emphase. Analyses-Théorie, 1, 122–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guerssel, M. (1986). Glides in Berber and syllabicity. Linguistic Inquiry, 17(1), 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guerssel, M., & Lowenstamm, J. (1990). The derivational morphology of the classical Arabic verbal system. Ms. UQAM & Université Paris 7.

  • Halle, M., & Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In K. Hale & S. J. Keyser (Eds.), View from the building 20th (pp. 111–176). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heath, J. (1987). Ablaut and ambiguity: Phonology of a Moroccan Arabic dialect. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaye, J., Lowenstamm, J., & Vergnaud, J.-R. (1990). Constituent structure and government in phonology. Phonology, 7(2), 193–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenstowicz, M., & Louriz, N. (2009). Reverse engineering: Emphatic consonants and the adaptation of vowels in French loanwords into Moroccan Arabic. Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics, 1, 41–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Khattab, G., Al-Tamimi, F., & Heselwood, B. (2006). Acoustic and auditory differences in the /t/-/t/ opposition in male and female speakers of Jordanian Arabic. In S. Boudelaa (Ed.), Perspectives on Arabic linguistics XVI: Papers from the sixteenth annual symposium on Arabic linguistics (pp. 131–160). Cambridge: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, P. (1982). From cyclic to lexical phonology. In H. van der Hulst & N. Smith (Eds.), The structure of phonological representations, vol. I (pp. 131–175). Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, P. (1985). Some consequences of lexical phonology. Phonology Yearbook, 2, 83–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, P. (2003). Syllables and moras in Arabic. In C. Féry & R. van de Vijver (Eds.), The syllable in optimality theory (pp. 147–182). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, R. (2012). A split analysis of plurality: Evidence from Amharic. In N. Arnett & R. Bennett (Eds.), The proceedings of WCCFL 30 (pp. 226–236). Somerville: Cascadilla.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahrouchi, M. (2013). A propos de I et de u en berbère: de la phonologie, de la morphologie et des phases. In A. Tifrit (Ed.), Phonologie, morphologie, syntaxe (pp. 21–30). Rennes: Presses Universitaires de Rennes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahrouchi, M., & Lampitelli, N. (2014). On plurals, noun phrase and number in Moroccan Arabic and Djibouti Somali. In S. Bendjaballah, N. Faust, M. Lahrouchi, & N. Lampitelli (Eds.), The structure of form, the form of structure: Essays in honor of Jean Lowenstamm (pp. 303–314). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lampitelli, N. (2010). Nounness, gender, class and syntactic structures in Italian nouns. In R. Bok-Bennema, B. Kampers-Manhe, & B. Hollebrandse (Eds.), Romance languages and linguistic theory 2008: Selected papers from ‘going Romance’, Groningen 2008 (pp. 195–214). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lecarme, J. (2002). Gender polarity: Theoretical aspects of Somali nominal morphology. In P. Boucher (Ed.), Many morphologies (pp. 109–141). Somerville: Cascadilla.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowenstamm, J. (1991). Vocalic length and centralization in two branches of Semitic (Ethiopic and Arabic). In A. S. Kaye (Ed.) Semitic studies in honor of Wolf Leslau on the occasion of his eighty-fifth birthday. (pp. 949–965). Wiesbaden: Otto Harrassowitz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowenstamm, J. (1996). CV as the only syllable type. In J. Durand & B. Laks (Eds.), Current trends in phonology: Models and methods (Vol. 2, pp. 419–441). Salford: European Studies Research Institute, University of Salford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowenstamm, J. (2003). A propos des gabarits. Recherches Linguistiques de Vincennes, 32, 7–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenstamm, J. (2008). On n, nP, \(\sqrt{}\) and types of nouns. In J. Hartmann, V. Hegedűs, & H. van Riemsdijk (Eds.), The sounds of silence: Empty elements in syntax and phonology (pp. 105–144). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, A. (2001). Words. Ms. Cambridge: MIT Press.

  • Marantz, A. (2007). Phases and words. In Phases in the theory of grammar (pp. 191–222). Seoul: Dong.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, A. (2013). Locality domains for contextual allomorphy across the interfaces. In O. Matushansky & A. Marantz (Eds.), Distributed morphology today (pp. 95–115). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Marvin, T. (2002). Topics in the stress and syntax of words. PhD dissertation, MIT.

  • Mascaró, J. (1976). Catalan phonology and the phonological cycle. Cambridge: MIT dissertation.

  • McCarthy, J. (1979). Formal problems in Semitic phonology and morphology. PhD dissertation, MIT. Distributed by Indiana University Linguistics Club. New York: Garland Press.

  • McCarthy, J. (1981). A prosodic theory of nonconcatenative morphology. Linguistic Inquiry, 12, 373–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J., & Prince, A. (1990). Foot and word in prosodic morphology: The Arabic broken plurals. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 8, 209–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melissaropoulou, D., & Ralli, A. (2008). Headedness in diminutive formation: Evidence from modern Greek and its dialectal variation. Acta Linguistica Hungarica, 55, 183–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milenova, M. (2009). Diminutive suffixes in Bulgarian. Patras Working Papers in Linguistics, 1, 127–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mohanan, Puthanveettil, K. (1982). Lexical phonology. MIT dissertation, Cambridge.

  • Newell, H. (2008). Aspects of the morphology and phonology of phases. PhD dissertation, McGill University, Montréal.

  • Newman, P. (2000). The Hausa language: An encyclopedic reference grammar. Yale: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obrecht, D. (1968). Effects of the second formant on the perception of velarization consonants in Arabic. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perlmutter, D. (1988). The split morphology hypothesis: evidence from Yiddish. In M. Hammond, & M. Noonan (Eds.), Theoretical Morphology (pp. 79–99). San Diego: Academic Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Piggott, G., & Newell, H. (2014). Interactions at the syntax–phonology interface: Evidence from Ojibwe. Lingua, 150, 332–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puglielli, A., & Siyaad, C. M. (1984). La flessione del nome. In A. Puglielli (Ed.), Aspetti morfologici, lessicali e della focalizzazione (Studi Somali 5) (pp. 53–112). Rome: MAE Dipartimento per la Cooperazione allo Sviluppo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritter, E. (1993). Where’s gender? Linguistic Inquiry, 24(4), 795–803.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubach, J. (1985). Lexical phonology: Lexical and postlexical derivations. Phonology Yearbook, 2, 157–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, B. (2010). Phonological derivation by phase: Evidence from Basque. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 16, 166–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scalise, S. (1988). The notion of head in morphology. Yearbook of Morphology 1, 229–245.

  • Shaw, J., Gafos Adamantios, I., Philip, H., & Zeroual, C. (2009). Syllabification in Moroccan Arabic: evidence from patterns of temporal stability in articulation. Phonology, 26, 187–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shoul, K. (2007). Etude physiologique, articulatoire, acoustique, perceptive de l’emphase en arabe marocain oriental. Thèse de doctorat, Université Paris 3.

  • Wahba, K. (1993). A sociolinguistic treatment of the feature of emphasis in Egypt. PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.

  • Younes, M. (1993). Emphasis spread in two Arabic dialects. In M. Eid & C. Holes (Eds.), Perspectives on Arabic linguistics (pp. 119–145). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Zeroual, C. (2000). Propos controversés sur la phonétique et la phonologie de l’arabe marocain. Thèse de doctorat, Université Paris 8.

  • Zeroual, C., Hoole, P., Fuchs, S., & Esling, J. H. (2007). EMA study of the coronal emphatic and non-emphatic plosive consonants of Moroccan Arabic. In Proceedings of the XVIth ICPHS, Saarbrücken (pp. 397–400).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mohamed Lahrouchi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lahrouchi, M., Ridouane, R. On diminutives and plurals in Moroccan Arabic. Morphology 26, 453–475 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-016-9290-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-016-9290-7

Keywords

Navigation