Skip to main content
Log in

Component-dependent allomorphy and paradigm accessibility: evidence from Hebrew

  • Published:
Morphology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examines allomorphy in vowel selection in Hebrew. We address the formation of adjectives and passive verbs in a specific morphological pattern with stem-medial glottals. While the normative vowel that precedes the glottal is o, there are cases where it is colloquially u. We provide empirical evidence for this variation (or lack thereof), arguing that vowel selection results from the distinction between adjectives and passive verbs and, more generally, from the component of the grammar where they are derived, the lexicon and the syntax. Studies of Hebrew have shown that a has a more morphological-lexical status, as it is typical of word formation processes, while e has a phonological status as it is the default vowel in epenthesis. A previous study has also shown that a is more typical of the lexicon and e is more typical of the syntax. We propose a similar distinction between o and u; u tends to be selected in passive formation in the syntax, while o is selected for words stored in the lexicon. In addition, we claim that the occurrence of each allomorph is triggered by uniformity to other forms in different paradigms that are taken into account in word formation. The study provides further evidence to a new type of allomorphy, whose evidence for conditioning is not purely phonological or morpho-syntactic, but rather depends on the locus of word formation. It highlights the strong correlation between form, meaning and the relevant grammatical component in word formation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See also Jaeggli (1986), Baker et al. (1989), Collins (2005), Hallman (2013) among others.

  2. There are also passive verbs that are formed in niCCaC based on CaCaC transitive verbs. The status of niCCaC is irregular with respect to both morpho-phonology and the types of verbs that it hosts, in comparison to other binyanim (see Berman 1979; Schwarzwald 1996, 2008; Bat-El 2002; Doron 2003, among others). This discussion is beyond the scope of this paper.

  3. Some adjectival passives entail the existence of an entity that causes the event while others do not, as discussed in the above mentioned references. This distinction is irrelevant for the purposes of the current study. See Freidin (1975), Bresnan (1982), Grimshaw (1990), Carrier and Randall (1992), Borer (1998), Doron (1999, 2013), Baker (2003), among many others for further discussion of the operation.

  4. For different contrasting approaches see Borer (1998, 2004), Doron and Rappaport-Hovav (2009), Halle and Marantz (1993), Marantz (2013) among others.

  5. For example, the Hebrew verb kilel ‘curse’, for example, has no reciprocal counterpart that denotes ‘curse each other’.

  6. It is important to note, though, that not every lexical operation demonstrates low productivity. However, all operations that apply in the syntax are productive.

  7. It is important note that these morphological differences distinguish between derivational processes that apply in the lexicon and in the syntax, like valence changing operations. Inflectional processes, that are assumed to apply mostly in the syntax, are also manifested via morphological processes, like affixation, that apply in both components. We thank the reviewers for this clarification.

  8. For more studies of the participle form in Hebrew, see also Doron (1999), Taube (2009), Zewi and Reshef (2009), Meltzer-Asscher (2010, 2011).

  9. Meltzer-Asscher (2011) shows that the class of adjectival passives consists of two sub-classes; for the purposes of this study we treat all adjectival passive as one group.

  10. For the discussion of glottals in Hebrew, see for example, Morag (1977), Bolozky (1978, 1995, 1997), Schwarzwald (1979), Blau (1981, 1986), Ravid and Shlesinger (2001), Bar-Asher (2002), Shatil (2006), Bolozky and Kreitman (2007), Gonen (2008, 2009), Faust (2005, 2013), Pariente (2012), and references therein.

  11. In addition, it could be the case that (some) glottal consonants were not produced at all in different stages of MH therefore the term ‘weakening’ can be misleading. This study does not pretend to determine whether glottals did and do exist in Hebrew. We take it as a point of departure that some forms demonstrate the historical effect of glottals and examine to what extent the effect is active or not. We would like to thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing this out.

  12. This change into o is historically the result of vowel lowering. This change can also be accounted for by a constraint that prohibits the ua sequence, assuming that the glottal stop is not pronounced (Faust 2005). For the purposes of this study, we do not propose which process indeed takes place, but we examine whether this change is still an active part of the grammar and to what extent. In addition, the process of vowel lowering was historically preceded by degemination and compensatory lengthening. However, since MH has no geminates, this is irrelevant for the current study.

  13. The process of vowel lowering also applies with stem medial ʁ (e.g. mesoʁakmesuʁak ‘is combed’). Rhotics have a special phonological status in general (Lindau 1985; Itô and Mester 2001; Hoole et al. 2013; Savu 2013 among others) and in Hebrew (Bolozky and Kreitman 2007; Cohen 2013, 2015 among others). This merits a separate study which is beyond the scope of this paper.

  14. The term ‘faithfulness’ is based on studies of Optimality Theory (see Prince and Smolensky 1993, among many others). Faithfulness constraints require identity across various forms that are grammatically related to each other, and therefore penalize any change. We use this term its general sense without necessarily committing our analysis to Optimality.

  15. In niCCaC, in comparison, there was noticeable variation for other reasons that we do not discuss in this paper.

  16. We would like to thank Abigail Amsterdamer for her help and contribution in conducting the experiment.

  17. As noted by an anonymous reviewer, the forms that are presented as almost exclusively verbal passive forms, can be used as adjectives in specific contexts. A web search reveals that such cases are relatively rare in comparison to the forms that we classified as ambiguous. We would like to thank the reviewer for this clarification.

  18. See for example, Ferris (1993), Dixon and Aikhenvald (2004).

  19. See Ravid (2012) and references therein for the discussion of the interaction between orthography and morphology.

  20. There is a small group of intransitive non-passive verbs with a passive morphology, e.g. huksam ‘become charmed’, derived from hiksim ‘charmed’ (see Landau 2002 and Meltzer-Asscher 2011).

  21. We currently do not make any predictions regarding the differences between adjectives that are related or not related to verbs, but only between forms which are unambiguously adjectives and forms that are ambiguous.

References

  • Albright, A. (2008). Explaining universal tendencies and language particulars in analogical change. In J. Good (Ed.), Linguistic universals and language change (pp. 144–184). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Anagnostopoulou, E. (2003). Participles and voice. In A. Alexiadou, M. Rathert, & A. von Stechow (Eds.), Perfect explorations (pp. 1–36). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, S. R. (1981). Where is morphology? In T. Thomas-Finders (Ed.), Extended word-and-paradigm theory (pp. 228–261). Los Angeles: UCLA working papers.

  • Anderson, S. R. (1992). A-morphous morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Arad, M. (2005). Roots and patterns—Hebrew morpho-syntax. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, M. (1976). Word formation in generative grammar. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, M. (1994). Morphology by itself. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, M. (2007). In the beginning was the word. Language, 83, 803–830.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M. C. (1988). Incorporation: a theory of grammatical function changing. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M. (2003). Lexical categories: verbs, nouns, and adjectives. Cambridge studies in linguistics: Vol. 102. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M., Johnson, K., & Roberts, I. (1989). Passive arguments raised. Linguistic Inquiry, 20, 219–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bao, Z. (1990). Fanqie languages and reduplication. Linguistic Inquiry, 21, 317–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Asher, M. (2002). Modern Hebrew and the tradition of generations. Te’uda, 18, 203–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bat-El, O. (1989). Phonology and word structure in Modern Hebrew. Ph.D. Dissertation, Los Angeles: UCLA.

  • Bat-El, O. (1994a). Stem modification and cluster transfer in Modern Hebrew. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 12, 572–596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bat-El, O. (1994b). The optimal acronym word in Hebrew. In Koskinen (Ed.), Proceedings of the 1994 annual conference of the Canadian linguistic association (pp. 23–37). Toronto working papers in linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bat-El, O. (2002). Semitic verb structure within a universal perspective. In J. Shimron (Ed.), Languages processing and acquisition in languages of semitic, root-based, morphology (pp. 29–59). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bat-El, O. (2005). Competing principles of paradigm uniformity: evidence from Hebrew imperative paradigm. In L. J. Downing, T. A. Hall, & R. Raffelsiefen (Eds.), Paradigms in phonological theory (pp. 44–64). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bat-El, O. (2011). Semitic templates. In M. van Oostendorp, C. Ewen, E. Hume, & K. Rice (Eds.), Blackwell companion to phonology (pp. 2586–2608). Malden: Wiley–Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benua, L. (1995). Identity effects in morphological truncation. In J. Beckman, S. Urbanczyk, & L. Walsh (Eds.), Papers in optimality theory: University of Massachusetts occasional papers in linguistics (pp. 77–136). Amherst: GLSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benua, L. (1997). Transderivational identity: phonological relations between words. Ph.D. Dissertation, Amherst: University of Massachusetts.

  • Berman, R. A. (1978). Modern Hebrew structure. Tel Aviv: University Publishing Projects.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (1979). Form and function: passives, middles, and impersonals in Modern Hebrew. In Proceedings of the annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (Vol. 5, pp. 1–27).

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (1987). Productivity in the lexicon: new-word formation in Modern Hebrew. Folia Linguistica, 21, 425–461.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (1994). Formal, lexical, and semantic factors in the acquisition of Hebrew resultative particles. In S. Gahl, A. Dolbey, & C. Johnson (Eds.), Proceedings of the annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (Vol. 20, pp. 82–92).

    Google Scholar 

  • Blau, J. (1981). Weakening of the gutturals. Leshonenu, 45, 32–39 (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Blau, J. (1986). Phonology and morphology. Tel-Aviv: Hakibbutz Hame’uxad Publishing (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Blevins, J. P. (2005). Word-based declensions in Estonian. In Yearbook of Morphology (pp. 1–25).

    Google Scholar 

  • Blevins, J. P. (2006). Word-based morphology. Journal of Linguistics, 42(3), 531–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bochner, H. (1993). Simplicity in generative morphology. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bolozky, S. (1978). Word formation strategies in MH verb system: denominative verbs. Afroasiatic Linguistics, 5, 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolozky, S. (1981). Note on frequency in phonetic change. Hebrew Annual Review, 5, 15–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolozky, S. (1995). Traces of “Gutturals” in Modern Hebrew and implications for teaching. Bulletin of Higher Hebrew Education, 7–8, 67–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolozky, S. (1997). Israeli Hebrew phonology. In A. S. Kaye & P. Daniels (Eds.), Phonologies of Asia and Africa (pp. 287–311). Winona Lake: Eisenbrauns.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolozky, S. (1999a). Measuring productivity in word formation: the case of Israeli Hebrew. Leiden: Brill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolozky, S. (1999b). On the special status of the vowels a and e in Israeli Hebrew. Hebrew Studies, 40, 233–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolozky, S. (2003). Phonological and morphological variation in spoken Hebrew. In B. H. Hary (Ed.), Corpus linguistics and Modern Hebrew: towards the Compilation of the Corpus of Spoken Hebrew (CoSIH) (pp. 19–156). Tel Aviv: Tel Aviv University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolozky, S., & Becker, M. (2006). Living lexicon of Hebrew nouns. Ms, Amherst: University of Massachusetts. Available at: http://becker.phonologist.org/LLHN.

  • Bolozky, S., & Kreitman, R. (2007). Uvulars in Israeli Hebrew—their phonetic and phonological status. In The National Association of professors of Hebrew’s international conference on Hebrew language, literature and culture, Sydney, Australia, July 2–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booij, G. E. (1995). Inherent versus contextual inflection and the split morphology hypothesis. In G. E. Booij & J. van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology (pp. 1–16). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booij, G. E. (2008). Paradigmatic morphology. In B. Fradin (Ed.), La Raison morphologique. Hommage á la mémoire de Danielle Corbin (pp. 29–38). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Borer, H. (1991). The causative-inchoative alternation: a case study in parallel morphology. The Linguistic Review, 8, 119–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borer, H. (1998). Deriving passive without theta grids. In S. Lapointe, D. Brentari, & P. Farrell (Eds.), Morphology and its relation to phonology and syntax (pp. 60–99). Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borer, H. (2004). Determining structures: an exo-skeletal trilogy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosch, A., & de Jong, K. J. (1997). The prosody of Barra Gaelic epenthetic vowels. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences, 27(1), 2–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bresnan, J. W. (1982). The passive in lexical theory. In J. Bresnan (Ed.), The mental representation of grammatical relations (pp. 3–86). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broselow, E. (1999). Stress, epenthesis, and segment transformation in Selayarese loans. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting, Berkeley Linguistics Society (Vol. 25, pp. 311–325).

    Google Scholar 

  • Burzio, L. (1998). Multiple correspondence. Lingua, 104, 79–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrier, J., & Randall, J. H. (1992). The argument structure and syntactic structure of resultatives. Linguistic Inquiry, 23(2), 173–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chierchia, G. (2004) (written in 1989). A semantics for unaccusatives and its syntactic consequences. In A. Alexiadou, E. Anagnostpoulou, & M. Everaert (Eds.), The unaccusativity puzzle (pp. 288–331). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1970). Remarks on nominalization. In A. Jacobs & P. Rosenbaum (Eds.), Readings in transformational grammar (pp. 184–221). Waltham: Ginn and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, E.-G. (2010). Predicting adaptation patterns: multiple sources of Hebrew vowels in English loanwords. In A. Holler & C. Scherer (Eds.), Nicht-native Einheiten und Strukturen. Strategien der Integration und Isolation in der Sprache (pp. 51–63). Tübingen: Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, E.-G. (2013). Prosodic factors in the adaptation of Hebrew rhotics in loanwords from English. In L. Spreafico & A. Vietti (Eds.), Rhotics: new data and perspectives (pp. 191–206). Bozen-Bolzano: Bozen-Bolzano University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, E.-G. (2015). Phoneme complexity and frequency in the acquisition of Hebrew rhotics. J. Child Lang. Acquis. Dev., 3(1), 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, C. (2005). A smuggling approach to the passive in English. Syntax, 8(2), 81–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corbin, D. (1989). Form, structure and meaning of constructed words in an associative and stratified lexical component. In G. Booij & J. van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1989 (pp. 31–54). Foris: Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, R. M. W. & Aikhenvald, A. Y. (Eds.) (2004). Adjective classes: a cross-linguistic typology. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doron, E. (1999). The passive participle. Balshanut Ivrit, 47, 39–62 (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Doron, E. (2003). Agency and voice: the semantics of the semitic templates. Natural Language Semantics, 11, 1–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doron, E. (2013). The interaction of adjectival passive and voice. In H. Borer, A. Alexiadou, & F. Schaeffer (Eds.), The roots of syntax and the syntax of roots. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doron, E., & Rappaport-Hovav, M. (2009). A unified approach to reflexivization in semitic and romance. In Brill’s annual of Afroasiatic languages and linguistics (Vol. 1, pp. 75–105).

    Google Scholar 

  • Faust, N. (2005). The Fate of Hebrew Gutturals. MA Thesis, Tel-Aviv: Tel-Aviv University.

  • Faust, N. (2012). Non-concatenative realization in the verbal inflection of Modern Hebrew. Morphology, 22(4), 453–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faust, N. (2013). The glottals that survived. Verb inflection in light of the historical effects of the glottal consonants. A paper presented the colloquium of the Department of Hebrew, The Hebrew University, Jerusalem.

  • Ferris, C. (1993). The meaning of syntax: a study in the adjectives of English. Harlow: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freidin, R. (1975). The analysis of passives. Language, 51, 384–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gai, A. (1995). The category ‘adjective’ in Semitic languages. Journal of Semitic Studies, 1, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gesenius (1910). In E. Kautzsch (Ed.), Gesenius’ Hebrew grammar, revised by A. E. Cowley. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, G. (1985). On verbal structure and the Hebrew verb. In M. Bar-Asher (Ed.), Language studies, Jerusalem (Vol. 1, pp. 295–348).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonen, E. (2008). Vowel changes in spoken Hebrew influenced by the rule of vowel reduction: a field study. Leshonenu, 71(1–2), 167–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonen, E. (2009). Is the rule of vowel reduction active in spoken Hebrew? Leshonenu, 71(3–4), 401–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gouskova, M., & Hall, N. (2009). Acoustics of epenthetic vowels in Lebanese Arabic. In S. Parker (Ed.), Phonological argumentation: essays on evidence and motivation (pp. 203–225). London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimshaw, J. (1990). Argument structure. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, N. (2006). Cross-linguistic patterns of vowel intrusion. Phonology, 23, 387–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halle, M. (1973). Prolegomena to a theory of word formation. Linguistic Inquiry, 4, 3–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halle, M., & Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed morphology. In K. Hale & S. K. Keyser (Eds.), The view from building 20 (pp. 111–176). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hallman, P. (2013). Predication and movement in passive. Lingua, 125, 76–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoole, P., Poulier, M., Benus, S., & Bombien, L. (2013). Articulatory coordination in obstruent-sonorant clusters and syllabic consonants: data and modelling. In L. Spreafico & A. Vietti (Eds.), Rhotics. New data and perspectives (pp. 81–98). Bozen-Bolzano: Bozen-Bolzano University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horvath, J., & Siloni, T. (2008). Active lexicon: adjectival and verbal passives. In S. Armon-Lotem, G. Danon, & S. Rothstein (Eds.), Current issues in generative hebrew linguistics (pp. 105–134). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Horvath, J., & Siloni, T. (2011). Anticausatives: against reflexivization. Lingua, 121, 2176–2186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Itô, J., & Mester, A. (2001). Structure preservation and stratal opacity in German. In L. Lombardi (Ed.), Segmental phonology in optimality theory (pp. 261–295). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeggli, O. (1986). Passive. Linguistic Inquiry, 17, 587–622.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenstowicz, M. (1996). Base-identity and uniform exponence: alternatives to cyclicity. In J. Durand & B. Laks (Eds.), Current trends in phonology: models and methods (pp. 363–393). Salford: University of Salford Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koontz-Garboden, A. (2010). The lexical semantics of derived statives. Linguistics and Philosophy, 33(4), 285–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laks, L. (2007). Morphology and thematic arity operations: evidence from Standard Arabic. In M. Mughazy (Ed.), Perspectives on Arabic linguistics (Vol. 20, pp. 51–67). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Laks, L. (2011). Morpho-phonological and morpho-thematic relations in Hebrew and Arabic verb formation. Doctoral Dissertation, Tel-Aviv University.

  • Laks, L., Cohen, E.-G., & Azulay-Amar, S. (2016). Paradigm uniformity and the locus of derivation: the case of vowel epenthesis in Hebrew verbs. Lingua, 170, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, I. (2002). A typology of psych passives. In M. Hirotani (Ed.), Proceedings of the 32nd conference of the North Eastern Linguistic Society (pp. 271–286). Amherst: University of Massachusetts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, J. (1987). Between epenthetic and excrescent vowels. In Proceedings of the West Coast conference on formal linguistics (Vol. 6, pp. 187–201).

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, B., & Rappaport-Hovav, M. (1986). The formation of adjectival passives. Linguistic Inquiry, 17, 623–661.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, B., & Rappaport-Hovav, M. (1995). Unaccusativity: at the syntax lexical semantics interface. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindau, M. (1985). The story of /r/. In V. Fromkin (Ed.), Phonetic linguistics: essays in honor of Peter Ladefoged (pp. 157–168). Orlando: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, A. (2013). Verbal argument structure: events and participants. Lingua, 130, 152–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J. (2005). Optimal paradigms. In L. J. Downing, T. A. Hall, & R. Raffelsiefen (Eds.), Paradigms in phonological theory (pp. 170–207). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J., & Prince, A. (1986). Prosodic morphology. Ms, University of Massachusetts, Amherst and Brandeis University.

  • McCarthy, J., & Prince, A. (1990). Foot and word in prosodic morphology: the Arabic broken plural. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 8, 209–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J., & Prince, A. (1995). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In J. J. Beckman, L. Walsh Dickey, & S. Urbanczyk (Eds.), University of Massachusetts occasional papers 18: Papers in optimality theory (pp. 249–384). Amherst: University of Massachusetts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meir, I. (2006). Morphological levels and diachronic change in Modern Hebrew plural formation. Studies in Languages, 30(4), 756–827.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzer-Asscher, A. (2010). Present participles: categorial classification and derivation. Lingua, 120, 2211–2239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzer-Asscher, A. (2011). Adjectival passives in Hebrew: evidence for parallelism between the adjectival and verbal systems. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 29, 815–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merkin, R. (1966). mefo_al pattern. Leshonenu, 32, 140–152 (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Morag, S. (1977). The Hebrew language tradition of Baghdadi community: phonology. Eda ve-Lashon (A). Jerusalem: The Hebrew University (in Hebrew).

  • Ornan, U. (1971). Verbal-patterns, stems: inflections and derivations. Ha-Universita, 16, 15–22 (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ornan, U. (2003). The final word. Haifa: Haifa University (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Pariente, I. (2012). Grammatical paradigm uniformity. Morphology, 22, 485–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perlmutter, D. (1988). The split-morphology hypothesis: evidence from Yiddish. In M. Hammond & M. Noonan (Eds.), Theoretical morphology: approaches in modern linguistics (pp. 79–99). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plada, N. (1959). Phoneme frequency in Hebrew. Leshonenu, 23, 235–242 (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piggott, G. L. (1995). Epenthesis and syllable weight. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 13, 283–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (1993). Optimality theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar. Technical Report CU-CS-696-93, Department of Computer Science, University of Colorado at Boulder, and Technical Report TR-2, Rutgers Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ University of Massachusetts, Amherst and Rutgers University.

  • Raffelsiefen, R. (1995). Conditions for stability: the case of Schwa in German (p. 282). Düsseldorf: Arbeiten des Sonderforschungsbereich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (1990). Internal structure constraints on new-word formation in Modern Hebrew. Folia Linguistica, 24, 289–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (2004). Later lexical development in Hebrew: derivational morphology revisited. In R. A. Berman (Ed.), TILAR (Trends in Language Acquisition Research) series. Language development across childhood and adolescence: psycholinguistic and crosslinguistic perspectives (pp. 53–82). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (2012). Spelling morphology: the psycholinguistics of Hebrew spelling. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Shlesinger, Y. (2001). Vowel reduction in Modern Hebrew: traces of the past and current variation. Folia Linguistica, 35(3–4), 371–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Levie, R. (2010). Hebrew adjectives in later language text production. First Language, 30, 27–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reinhart, T. (2016, to appear). The theta system: unaccusative and experiencer derivation. Ms., Tel-Aviv University. Text prepared by T. Siloni.

  • Reinhart, T., & Siloni, T. (2005). The lexicon-syntax parameter: reflexivization and other arity operations. Linguistic Inquiry, 36(3), 389–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosén, H. B. (1956). mefo_al in Israeli Hebrew. Leshonenu, 20, 139–148 (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosén, H. B. (1977). Contemporary Hebrew. Mouton: The Hague.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Savu, C. (2013). Another look at the structure of [ɾ]: constricted intervals and vocalic elements. In L. Spreafico & A. Vietti (Eds.), Rhotics. New data and perspectives (pp. 145–158). Bozen-Bolzano: Bozen-Bolzano University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scalise, S. (1984). Generative morphology. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzwald, O. R. (1979). Normativism and naturalness in the application of a morpho-phonemic rule in Hebrew. Bar Ilan, 15, 171–192 (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzwald, O. R. (1981a). Grammar and reality in the Hebrew verb. Ramat Gan: Bar Ilan University Press (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzwald, O. R. (1981b). Frequency factors as determinants in the binyanim meanings. Hebrew Studies, 22, 131–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzwald, O. R. (1996). Syllable structure, alternations and verb complexity: the Modern Hebrew verb patterns reexamined. Israel Oriental Studies, 1, 99–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzwald, O. R. (2001). Modern Hebrew. Munchen: Lincom Europa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzwald, O. R. (2002). Hebrew morphology. Tel Aviv: The Open University (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzwald, O. R. (2008). The special status of Nif’al in Hebrew. In S. Armon-Lotem, S. Rothstein, & G. Danon (Eds.), Generative approaches to Modern Hebrew linguistics (pp. 61–75). Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzwald, O. R. (2012). On vowel polarity in Hebrew. Leshonenu, 74, 79–92 (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzwald, O. R., & Cohen-Gross, D. (2000). The productive noun patterns in Hebrew. In M. Horvits (Ed.), The language of contemporary press: Mina Efron’s memorial volume (pp. 148–161). Tel-Aviv: The Mofet Institute (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Shatil, N. (2006). The reflection of ħ and other historical gutturals in intermorphemic environments in spoken language. Hebrew Linguistics, 57, 57–88 (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Shatil, N. (2014). Developments in Contemporary Hebrew. Jerusalem: The Academy of Hebrew Language (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Siloni, T. (2002). Active lexicon. Theoretical Linguistics, 28, 383–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siloni, T. (2008). On the Hitpa’el template. In G. Hatav (Ed.), Theoretical Hebrew linguistics (pp. 111–138). Jerusalem: The Hebrew University Magnes Press (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Siloni, T. (2011). Reciprocal verbs and symmetry. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 29, 1–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, A. (1988). Bracketing paradoxes and the English lexicon. Language, 64, 663–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steriade, D. (1988). Reduplication and transfer in Sanskrit and elsewhere. Phonology, 5(1), 73–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steriade, D. (1999). Lexical conservatism in French adjectival liaison. In M. Authier, B. Bullock, & L. Reed (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th linguistic colloquium on romance languages (pp. 243–270). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steriade, D. (2000). Paradigm uniformity and the phonetic-phonology boundary. In M. Broe & J. Pierrehumbrett (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology V: acquisition and the lexicon (pp. 313–334). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stump, G. T. (1991). Paradigm-based theory of morphosemantic mismatches. Language, 67, 675–725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stump, G. T. (2001). Inflectional morphology. A theory of paradigm structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Taube, D. (2009). The passive participle in Modern Hebrew. In G. Goldenberg & A. Shisha-Halevy (Eds.), Egyptian, semitic and general grammar (pp. 317–336). Jerusalem: The Israel Academy of Sciences and Humanities.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ussishkin, A. (1999). The inadequacy of the consonantal root: Modern Hebrew denominal verbs and output-output correspondence. Phonology, 16, 401–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ussishkin, A. (2005). A fixed prosodic theory of non-concatenative templatic morphology. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 23, 169–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Marle, J. (1985). On the paradigmatic dimension of morphological creativity. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wasow, T. (1977). Transformations and the lexicon. In P. Culicover, T. Wasow, & A. Akmajian (Eds.), Formal syntax (pp. 327–360). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yip, M. (1992). Reduplication with fixed melodic material. Proceedings of NELS 22. Amherst: GLSA, University of Massachusetts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zadok, G., & Bat-El, O. (2015). Inte-rparadigm leveling in Hebrew verbal system. Morphology, 25, 271–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zewi, T., & Reshef, Y. (2009). The active participle and temporal expressions in Hebrew. Leshonenu, 71, 315–344 (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to express our deep gratitude to Outi Bat-El, Tal Siloni, Noam Faust and anonymous reviewers for their useful comments and suggestions. We would like to thank Abigail Amsterdamer for her help in conducting the experiment.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lior Laks.

Appendix

Appendix

This appendix includes all the sentences used in the experiment, organized according to the three categories: adjectives, verbs, ambiguous forms. With the ambiguous forms, in each pair with the same form, the adjectival meaning precedes the verbal one.

Adjectives

  1. 1.

    haja ʃam jeled mevohal/mevuhal ʃe-kol ha-zman baxa

    ‘There was a frightened boy there who was constantly crying’

  2. 2.

    haja ʃam pesel mexoʔaʁ/mexuʔaʁ ʃe-kulam hitbonenu bo

    ‘There was an ugly statue there that everyone was looking at’

  3. 3.

    ha-student ha - mejoʔaʃ/mejuʔaʃ bikeʃ lekabel moʔed mejuxad ki jeʃ lo ʃaloʃ ʔavodot lehagiʃ ad maxaʁ

    ‘The desperate student asked for a special exam date because he had three papers to submit by tomorrow’

  4. 4.

    ledaʔati kol ha-simanim maʁʔim ʃe-medubaʁ be-naʔaʁ meʔohav/meʔuhav ʔad meʔal haʁoʃ

    ‘I think all signs indicate that we’re talking about a boy completely in love

  5. 5.

    ha-netsigim ʃel ha-miʃlaxat ha-kalkalit bikʁu be-ʔezoʁ metoʔas/metuʔas jaxad ʔim ha-saʁ

    ‘The representatives of the economic delegation visited the industrialized area together with the minister’

  6. 6.

    ʁaʔinu ʃam binjan mefoʔaʁ/mefuʔaʁ ʃe-ʔanaxnu ʁak jexolim laxlom laguʁ bo paʔam

    ‘We saw a fancy building there which we can only ever dream of living in’

Verbs

  1. 7.

    biglal ʃehu kol kax meluxlax ha-sadin menoʔaʁ/menuʔaʁ be-xozka ʔal jedej hajeled

    ‘Because it is so dirty, the sheet is being shaken forcefully by the boy’

  2. 8.

    be-jamim ʔele ha-saxkan ha-xadaʃ melohak/meluhak ʔal jedej ha-soxnut le-tafkid xadaʃ

    ‘These days the new actor is being cast by the agency in a new role’

  3. 9.

    hem medabʁim ʔaxʃav ve-hamatsav ha-ze metoʔaʁ/metuʔaʁ ʔal jedej moʃe la-studentim

    ‘They are talking now and this situation is being described by Moshe to the students’

  4. 10.

    kol ma ʃehu masbiʁ metoʔad/metuʔad be-ʁegaʔim ʔele ʔal jedej ha-taxkiʁan

    ‘Everything he is explaining is being recorded at this time by the researcher’

  5. 11.

    ha-makom ha-xadaʃ ha-ze menohal/menuhal kaʁega ʔal jedej dina

    ‘The new place is being managed at the moment by Dina’

  6. 12.

    ha-maʔamats ʃe-jaʔiʁ maʃkia ba-pʁojekt metoʔal/metuʔal ʔal jedej ha-menahel le-davaʁ ʔexad bilvad

    ‘The effort Yair is investing in the project is being channeled by the manager for a single purpose’

Ambiguous forms

  1. 13.

    ʔanaxnu mexapsim bajit meʁohat/meʁuhat ve-benatajim lo matsanu klum

    ‘We are looking for a furnished house and in the meantime we’ve found nothing’

  2. 14.

    ha-bajit meʁohat/meʁuhat kaʁega ʔal jedej ha-meʔatsev ha-xadaʃ ʃe-saxaʁnu

    ‘The house is being furnished at the moment by the new decorator we hired’

  3. 15.

    hu ʔamaʁ ʃe-nahaʁ kol kax mezoham/mezuham hu makom meʔod mesukan lisxot

    ‘He said that such a polluted river is a very dangerous place to swim’

  4. 16.

    ha-nahaʁ ha-ze kol hazman mezoham/mezuham ʔal jedej xevʁot ʃe-maʃlixot ʔelav psolet

    ‘This river is constantly being polluted by the companies that dispose of waste into it’

  5. 17.

    tenase limtso ʃam ʔejze beged megohats/meguhats. ʔen li koax lexapes maʃehu ʔaxeʁ

    ‘Try and find some ironed item of clothing there. I don’t feel like looking for something else’

  6. 18.

    ba-zman ʃe-dina mexina ʔet ha-matanot, ha-beged ha-xadaʃ megohats/meguhats ʔal jedej moti

    ‘While Dina is preparing the presents, the new item of clothing is being ironed by Moti’

  7. 19.

    ʔani xoʃev ʃe-ʔekne la-jeladim ʃeli tanax mevoʔaʁ/mevuʔaʁ kedej ʃe-jihije lahem joteʁ kal

    ‘I think I will buy my children an annotated bible so it will be easier for them’

  8. 20.

    be-mahalax ha-haʁtsaʔa ha-pasuk ha-spetsifi ha-ze mevoʔaʁ/mevuʔaʁ ʔal jedej ha-ʁav ha-xadaʃ

    ‘During the lecture, this specific verse is being translated (=annotated) by the new rabbi’

  9. 21.

    lamʁot kol ha-sfekot ha-ʁevax ha-naki he- metoʔam/hametuʔam li-ʃnat 2014 ʔala be-5 %

    ‘Despite all the doubts the total adjusted profit for 2014 grew by 5 %’

  10. 22.

    galja meʔod smexa ʃe-ʔaxʃav hem medabʁim ve-habikuʁ metoʔam/metuʔam ʔal jedej ha-netsigim la-ʃavua haba

    ‘Galia is very happy that they are now talking and the visit is being coordinated (=adjusted) by the representatives for next week’

  11. 23.

    ʔadajin lo baʁuʁ ʔex jeʁaʔe naxal kiʃon ha - metohaʁ/ha - metuhaʁ ʔaxaʁej kol ha-peʔulot ha-ʔele

    ‘It’s still not clear how the purified Kishon river will look after all these actions’

  12. 24.

    be-misgeʁet ʃvua ʔejxut ha-sviva ha-nahaʁ metohaʁ/metuhaʁ ʔal jedej mitnadvim kimʔat kol jom

    ‘Within the framework of the environment week, the river is purified by the volunteers almost every day’

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Laks, L., Cohen, EG. Component-dependent allomorphy and paradigm accessibility: evidence from Hebrew. Morphology 26, 425–452 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-016-9288-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-016-9288-1

Keywords

Navigation