Skip to main content
Log in

Does consciousness overflow cognitive access? Novel insights from the new phenomenon of attribute amnesia

  • Review
  • Published:
Science China Life Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The moment we open our eyes, we experience a rich and detailed visual world, but the amount of information available to report is rather limited. This dissociation relates to a major debate regarding the nature of visual consciousness. The overflow argument suggests that our conscious experience is quite rich and far beyond what can be reported, standing in sharp contrast to the no-overflow argument that visual consciousness is severely impoverished and limited to what can be reported. In this paper, we systematically reviewed existing evidence in favor of the overflow argument, including studies of several variations of the iconic memory paradigm and the divided attention paradigm, as well as studies of neural correlates of consciousness. Simultaneously, we expounded some critical objections and alternative interpretations to such evidence, as well as some opposing evidence. Finally, we introduced a series of our recent studies based on a striking phenomenon of attribute amnesia, which we believe could provide new insight into the overflow view of visual consciousness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ariely, D. (2001). Seeing sets: Representation by statistical properties. Psychol Sci 12, 157–162.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aru, J., Bachmann, T., Singer, W., and Melloni, L. (2012). Distilling the neural correlates of consciousness. Neurosci BioBehav Rev 36, 737–746.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Awh, E., Vogel, E.K., and Oh, S.H. (2006). Interactions between attention and working memory. Neuroscience 139, 201–208.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B.J. (2005). Global workspace theory of consciousness: toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience. Prog Brain Res 150, 45–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Babiloni, C., Vecchio, F., Rossi, S., De Capua, A., Bartalini, S., Ulivelli, M., and Rossini, P.M. (2007). Human ventral parietal cortex plays a functional role on visuospatial attention and primary consciousness. A repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cereb Cortex 17, 1486–1492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, D.M., Muggleton, N., Walsh, V., and Lavie, N. (2006). Right parietal cortex plays a critical role in change blindness. Cereb Cortex 16, 712–717.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behav Brain Sci 18, 227–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (2005). Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci 9, 46–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (2008). Consciousness and cognitive access. Proc Aristotel Soc 108, 289–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (2011). Perceptual consciousness overflows cognitive access. Trends Cogn Sci 15, 567–575.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (2019). What is wrong with the no-report paradigm and how to fix it. Trends Cogn Sci 23, 1003–1013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boehler, C.N., Schoenfeld, M.A., Heinze, H.J., and Hopf, J.M. (2008). Rapid recurrent processing gates awareness in primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105, 8742–8747.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bor, D., Duncan, J., Wiseman, R.J., and Owen, A.M. (2003). Encoding strategies dissociate prefrontal activity from working memory demand. Neuron 37, 361–367.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bor, D., and Owen, A.M. (2007). A common prefrontal-parietal network for mnemonic and mathematical recoding strategies within working memory. Cereb Cortex 17, 778–786.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borst, J.P., and Anderson, J.R. (2013). Using model-based functional MRI to locate working memory updates and declarative memory retrievals in the fronto-parietal network. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110, 1628–1633.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfman, Z.Z., Brezis, N., Jacobson, H., and Usher, M. (2014). We see more than we can report: “cost free” color phenomenality outside focal attention. Psychol Sci 25, 1394–1403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H., Carlson, R.A., and Wyble, B. (2018). Is source information automatically available in working memory? Psychol Sci 29, 645–655.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H., Swan, G., and Wyble, B. (2016). Prolonged focal attention without binding: Tracking a ball for half a minute without remembering its color. Cognition 147, 144–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H., and Wyble, B. (2015). Amnesia for object attributes: Failure to report attended information that had just reached conscious awareness. Psychol Sci 26, 203–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H., and Wyble, B. (2016). Attribute amnesia reflects a lack of memory consolidation for attended information. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 42, 225–234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H., and Wyble, B. (2018). The neglected contribution of memory encoding in spatial cueing: A new theory of costs and benefits. Psychol Rev 125, 936–968.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H., Yan, N., Zhu, P., Wyble, B., Eitam, B., and Shen, M. (2019a). Expecting the unexpected: Violation of expectation shifts strategies toward information exploration. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 45, 513–522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H., Yu, J., Fu, Y., Zhu, P., Li, W., Zhou, J., and Shen, M. (2019b). Does attribute amnesia occur with the presentation of complex, meaningful stimuli? The answer is, “it depends”. Mem Cogn 47, 1133–1144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, X., Hoffmann, K.P., Albright, T.D., and Thiele, A. (2012). Effect of feature-selective attention on neuronal responses in macaque area MT. J Neurophysiol 107, 1530–1543.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chun, M.M., and Turk-Browne, N.B. (2007). Interactions between attention and memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol 17, 177–184.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M.A., Alvarez, G.A., and Nakayama, K. (2011). Natural-scene perception requires attention. Psychol Sci 22, 1165–1172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M.A., Cavanagh, P., Chun, M.M., and Nakayama, K. (2012). The attentional requirements of consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci 16, 411–417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M.A., and Dennett, D.C. (2011). Consciousness cannot be separated from function. Trends Cogn Sci 15, 358–364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M.A., Dennett, D.C., and Kanwisher, N. (2016). What is the bandwidth of perceptual experience? Trends Cogn Sci 20, 324–335.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M.A., Ortego, K., Kyroudis, A., and Pitts, M. (2020). Distinguishing the neural correlates of perceptual awareness and postperceptual processing. J Neurosci 40, 4925–4935.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cotton, K., and Ricker, T.J. (2020). Working memory consolidation improves long-term memory recognition. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000954.

  • D’Aloisio-Montilla, N. (2017). Imagery and overflow: We see more than we report. Philos Psychol 30, 545–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dakin, S.C., and Watt, R.J. (1997). The computation of orientation statistics from visual texture. Vision Res 37, 3181–3192.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Gardelle, V., Sackur, J., and Kouider, S. (2009). Perceptual illusions in brief visual presentations. Conscious Cogn 18, 569–577.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2014). Consciousness and the Brain: Deciphering How the Brain Codes Our Thoughts. New York: Viking Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., and Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J Exp Psychol General 122, 371–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., and Changeux, J.P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron 70, 200–227.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Changeux, J.P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., and Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends Cogn Sci 10, 204–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., and Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79, 1–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Del Cul, A., Dehaene, S., Reyes, P., Bravo, E., and Slachevsky, A. (2009). Causal role of prefrontal cortex in the threshold for access to consciousness. Brain 132, 2531–2540.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J., and Mattingley, J.B. (1998). Parietal neglect and visual awareness. Nat Neurosci 1, 17–22.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duysens, J., Orban, G.A., Cremieux, J., and Maes, H. (1985). Visual cortical correlates of visible persistence. Vision Res 25, 171–178.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eitam, B., Shoval, R., and Yeshurun, Y. (2015). Seeing without knowing: Task relevance dissociates between visual awareness and recognition. Ann NY Acad Sci 1339, 125–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eitam, B., Yeshurun, Y., and Hassan, K. (2013). Blinded by irrelevance: Pure irrelevance induced “blindness”. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 39, 611–615.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eklund, R., and Wiens, S. (2018). Visual awareness negativity is an early neural correlate of awareness: A preregistered study with two Gabor sizes. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 18, 176–188.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., and d’Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Math Cogn 2, 95–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Förster, J., Koivisto, M., and Revonsuo, A. (2020). ERP and MEG correlates of visual consciousness: The second decade. Conscious Cogn 80, 102917.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frässle, S., Sommer, J., Jansen, A., Naber, M., and Einhäuser, W. (2014). Binocular rivalry: frontal activity relates to introspection and action but not to perception. J Neurosci 34, 1738–1747.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Haberman, J., and Whitney, D. (2009). Seeing the mean: ensemble coding for sets of faces. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 35, 718–734.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, D.E., and Thomas, L.E. (2008). Visual sensory memory. In: Luck, S. J., and Hollingworth, A., eds. Visual Memory. New York: Oxford University Press. 9–41.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson-Nielsen, M., Cohen, M.A., and Pitts, M.A. (2017). Perception of ensemble statistics requires attention. Conscious Cogn 48, 149–160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, J., Rich, A.N., Williams, M.A., and Woolgar, A. (2017). Feature-selective attention in frontoparietal cortex: multivoxel codes adjust to prioritize task-relevant information. J Cogn Neurosci 29, 310–321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, Y., Zhou, K., and He, S. (2007). Human visual cortex responds to invisible chromatic flicker. Nat Neurosci 10, 657–662.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, Y.V., Shupe, J.M., Swallow, K.M., and Tan, D.H. (2016). Memory for recently accessed visual attributes. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 42, 1331–1337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanai, R., Muggleton, N.G., and Walsh, V. (2008). TMS over the intraparietal sulcus induces perceptual fading. J Neurophysiol 100, 3343–3350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, C. (2004). The Quest for Consciousness a Neurobiological Approach. Greenwood Village: Roberts & Company Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, C., and Tsuchiya, N. (2007). Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes. Trends Cogn Sci 11, 16–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koivisto, M., and Grassini, S. (2016). Neural processing around 200 ms after stimulus-onset correlates with subjective visual awareness. Neuropsychologia 84, 235–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koivisto, M., and Revonsuo, A. (2003). An ERP study of change detection, change blindness, and visual awareness. Psychophysiology 40, 423–429.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koivisto, M., and Revonsuo, A. (2010). Event-related brain potential correlates of visual awareness. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34, 922–934.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koivisto, M., Revonsuo, A., and Lehtonen, M. (2006). Independence of visual awareness from the scope of attention: an electrophysiological study. Cereb Cortex 16, 415–424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koivisto, M., Revonsuo, A., and Salminen, N. (2005). Independence of visual awareness from attention at early processing stages. Neuroreport 16, 817–821.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koivisto, M., Salminen-Vaparanta, N., Grassini, S., and Revonsuo, A. (2016). Subjective visual awareness emerges prior to P3. Eur J Neurosci 43, 1601–1611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kouider, S., de Gardelle, V., Sackur, J., and Dupoux, E. (2010). How rich is consciousness? The partial awareness hypothesis. Trends Cogn Sci 14, 301–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamme, V.A.F. (2000). Neural mechanisms of visual awareness: A linking proposition. Brain Mind 1, 385–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamme, V.A.F. (2001). Blindsight: the role of feedforward and feedback corticocortical connections. Acta Psychol 107, 209–228.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lamme, V.A.F. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends Cogn Sci 7, 12–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamme, V.A.F. (2004). Separate neural definitions of visual consciousness and visual attention; a case for phenomenal awareness. Neural Netw 17, 861–872.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamme, V.A.F. (2006). Towards a true neural stance on consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci 10, 494–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamme, V.A.F. (2010). How neuroscience will change our view on consciousness. Cogn Neurosci 1, 204–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamme, V.A.F., and Roelfsema, P.R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends Neurosci 23, 571–579.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamy, D., Salti, M., and Bar-Haim, Y. (2009). Neural correlates of subjective awareness and unconscious processing: an ERP study. J Cogn Neurosci 21, 1435–1446.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landman, R., Spekreijse, H., and Lamme, V.A.F. (2003). Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Res 43, 149–164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Li, F.F., VanRullen, R., Koch, C., and Perona, P. (2002). Rapid natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99, 9596–9601.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, S., Yu, Q., Tse, P.U., and Cavanagh, P. (2019). Neural correlates of the conscious perception of visual location lie outside visual cortex. Curr Biol 29, 4036–4044.e4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lumer, E.D., Friston, K.J., and Rees, G. (1998). Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. Science 280, 1930–1934.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lumer, E.D., and Rees, G. (1999). Covariation of activity in visual and prefrontal cortex associated with subjective visual perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96, 1669–1673.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mack, A., Erol, M., and Clarke, J. (2015). Iconic memory is not a case of attention-free awareness. Conscious Cogn 33, 291–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mashour, G.A., Roelfsema, P., Changeux, J.P., and Dehaene, S. (2020). Conscious processing and the global neuronal workspace hypothesis. Neuron 105, 776–798.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, J., Schröder, P., Kaunitz, L., van Boxtel, J.J.A., and Tsuchiya, N. (2018). Conscious access in the near absence of attention: critical extensions on the dual-task paradigm. Phil Trans R Soc B 373, 20170352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCormick-Huhn, J.M., Chen, H., Wyble, B.P., and Dennis, N.A. (2018). Using attribute amnesia to test the limits of hyper-binding and associative deficits in working memory. Psychol Aging 33, 165–175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitroff, S.R., Simons, D.J., and Levin, D.T. (2004). Nothing compares 2 views: Change blindness can occur despite preserved access to the changed information. Percept Psychophys 66, 1268–1281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Naccache, L. (2018). Why and how access consciousness can account for phenomenal consciousness. Phil Trans R Soc B 373, 20170357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Naccache, L., Marti, S., Sitt, J.D., Trübutschek, D., and Berkovitch, L. (2016). Why the P3b is still a plausible correlate of conscious access? A commentary on Silverstein et al., 2015. Cortex 85, 126–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, R.K., and Mishkin, M. (1980). Blindness in monkeys following non-visual cortical lesions. Brain Res 188, 572–577.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, R.K., and Mishkin, M. (1986). Chronic ‘blindness’ following lesions of nonvisual cortex in the monkey. Exp Brain Res 63, 173–184.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Regan, J.K., and Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behav Brain Sci 24, 939–973.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ojanen, V., Revonsuo, A., and Sams, M. (2003). Visual awareness of low-contrast stimuli is reflected in event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology 40, 192–197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Overgaard, M., and Fazekas, P. (2016). Can no-report paradigms extract true correlates of consciousness? Trends Cogn Sci 20, 241–242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, A., and Walsh, V. (2001). Fast backprojections from the motion to the primary visual area necessary for visual awareness. Science 292, 510–512.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, I.B. (2011). Perception and iconic memory: what Sperling doesn’t show. Mind Lang 26, 381–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pitts, M.A., Padwal, J., Fennelly, D., Martínez, A., and Hillyard, S.A. (2014). Gamma band activity and the P3 reflect post-perceptual processes, not visual awareness. NeuroImage 101, 337–350.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Qin, P., Wu, X., Huang, Z., Duncan, N.W., Tang, W., Wolff, A., Hu, J., Gao, L., Jin, Y., Wu, X., et al. (2015). How are different neural networks related to consciousness? Ann Neurol 78, 594–605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, L., Wilken, P., and Koch, C. (2004). Face-gender discrimination is possible in the near-absence of attention. J Vision 4, 4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, L., Reddy, L., and Koch, C. (2006). Face identification in the near-absence of focal attention. Vision Res 46, 2336–2343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, L., Moradi, F., and Koch, C. (2007). Top-down biases win against focal attention in the fusiform face area. NeuroImage 38, 730–739.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rees, G., Kreiman, G., and Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nat Rev Neurosci 3, 261–270.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reynvoet, B., and Brysbaert, M. (1999). Single-digit and two-digit Arabic numerals address the same semantic number line. Cognition 72, 191–201.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ro, T., Breitmeyer, B., Burton, P., Singhal, N.S., and Lane, D. (2003). Feedback contributions to visual awareness in human occipital cortex. Curr Biol 13, 1038–1041.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salti, M., Bar-Haim, Y., and Lamy, D. (2012). The P3 component of the ERP reflects conscious perception, not confidence. Conscious Cogn 21, 961–968.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scrivener, C.L., Malik, A., Marsh, J., Lindner, M., and Roesch, E.B. (2019). An EEG study of detection without localisation in change blindness. Exp Brain Res 237, 2535–2547.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sergent, C., and Naccache, L. (2012). Imaging neural signatures of consciousness: ‘What’, ‘When’, ‘Where’ and ‘How’ does it work? Arch Ital Biol 150, 91–106.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sergent, C., Wyart, V., Babo-Rebelo, M., Cohen, L., Naccache, L., and Tallon-Baudry, C. (2013). Cueing attention after the stimulus is gone can retrospectively trigger conscious perception. Curr Biol 23, 150–155.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silvanto, J., Cowey, A., Lavie, N., and Walsh, V. (2005). Striate cortex (V1) activity gates awareness of motion. Nat Neurosci 8, 143–144.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D.J., and Levin, D.T. (1998). Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction. Psychon Bull Rev 5, 644–649.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D.J., and Chabris, C.F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception 28, 1059–1074.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D.J., Chabris, C.F., Schnur, T., and Levin, D.T. (2002). Evidence for preserved representations in change blindness. Conscious Cogn 11, 78–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sligte, I.G., Scholte, H.S., and Lamme, V.A.F. (2008). Are there multiple visual short-term memory stores? PLoS ONE 3, e1699.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sligte, I.G., Scholte, H.S., and Lamme, V.A.F. (2009). V4 activity predicts the strength of visual short-term memory representations. J Neurosci 29, 7432–7438.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sligte, I.G., Wokke, M.E., Tesselaar, J.P., Steven Scholte, H., and Lamme, V.A.F. (2011). Magnetic stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dissociates fragile visual short-term memory from visual working memory. Neuropsychologia 49, 1578–1588.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soto, D., Mäntylä, T., and Silvanto, J. (2011). Working memory without consciousness. Curr Biol 21, R912–R913.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychol Monogr General Appl 74, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stazicker, J. (2011). Attention, visual consciousness and indeterminacy. Mind Lang 26, 156–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterzer, P., and Kleinschmidt, A. (2007). A neural basis for inference in perceptual ambiguity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104, 323–328.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sterzer, P., Kleinschmidt, A., and Rees, G. (2009). The neural bases of multistable perception. Trends Cogn Sci 13, 310–318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stoerig, P., and Cowey, A. (1997). Blindsight in man and monkey. Brain 120, 535–559.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Supèr, H., Spekreijse, H., and Lamme, V.A.F. (2001). Two distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex (V1). Nat Neurosci 4, 304–310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swan, G., Wyble, B., and Chen, H. (2017). Working memory representations persist in the face of unexpected task alterations. Atten Percept Psychophys 79, 1408–1414.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, L.E., and Irwin, D.E. (2006). Voluntary eyeblinks disrupt iconic memory. Percept Psychophys 68, 475–488.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G. (2004). An information integration theory of consciousness. BMC Neurosci 5, 42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G., Boly, M., Massimini, M., and Koch, C. (2016). Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate. Nat Rev Neurosci 17, 450–461.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsuchiya, N., Wilke, M., Frässle, S., and Lamme, V.A.F. (2015). No-report paradigms: extracting the true neural correlates of consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci 19, 757–770.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turatto, M., Sandrini, M., and Miniussi, C. (2004). The role of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in visual change awareness. Neuroreport 15, 2549–2552.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Vugt, B., Dagnino, B., Vartak, D., Safaai, H., Panzeri, S., Dehaene, S., and Roelfsema, P.R. (2018). The threshold for conscious report: Signal loss and response bias in visual and frontal cortex. Science 360, 537–542.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenbroucke, A.R.E., Sligte, I.G., and Lamme, V.A.F. (2011). Manipulations of attention dissociate fragile visual short-term memory from visual working memory. Neuropsychologia 49, 1559–1568.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenbroucke, A.R.E., Sligte, I.G., de Vries, J.G., Cohen, M.X., and Lamme, V.A.F. (2015). Neural correlates of visual short-term memory dissociate between fragile and working memory representations. J Cogn Neurosci 27, 2477–2490.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, E.J., Bear, A., and Scholl, B.J. (2016). Can you perceive ensembles without perceiving individuals?: The role of statistical perception in determining whether awareness overflows access. Cognition 152, 78–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, E.J., and Scholl, B.J. (2015). Inattentional blindness reflects limitations on perception, not memory: Evidence from repeated failures of awareness. Psychon Bull Rev 22, 722–727.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiskrantz, L. (1986). Blindsight: A case study and implications. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiskrantz, L. (1996). Blindsight revisited. Curr Opin Neurobiol 6, 215–220.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wyble, B., Hess, M., O’Donnell, R.E., Chen, H., and Eitam, B. (2019). Learning how to exploit sources of information. Mem Cogn 47, 696–705.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xia, M., Wang, J., and He, Y. (2013). BrainNet Viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS ONE 8, e68910.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, M., Fu, Y., Yu, J., Zhu, P., Shen, M., and Chen, H. (2020). Source information is inherently linked to working memory representation for auditory but not for visual stimuli. Cognition 197, 104160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31771201), National Science Foundation for Distinguished Young Scholars of Zhejiang Province, China (LR19C090002), Humanities and Social Sciences Foundation of the Ministry of Education of China (17YJA190001).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Mowei Shen or Hui Chen.

Additional information

Compliance and ethics

The author(s) declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fu, Y., Yan, W., Shen, M. et al. Does consciousness overflow cognitive access? Novel insights from the new phenomenon of attribute amnesia. Sci. China Life Sci. 64, 847–860 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11427-020-1831-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11427-020-1831-8

Keywords

Navigation