Skip to main content
Log in

Situating Moods

  • Published:
Philosophia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The paper aims to better identify the relationship between moods and emotions showing their link to the overall environment. Adopting a Situated Approach to Emotions (Griffiths and Scarantino 2009; Mendonça Transactions of Charles Peirce Society, 48(2), 209–227, 2012; Stephan Emotion Review, 4(2), 157–162, 2012; Stephen et al. Philosophical Psychology, 27(1), 65–81 2014) enables showing that the link to emotions to the environment is best understood using the term situation, while moods’ link to the environment is best captured by the notion of context. Exploring the difference points out that what is selected as the situation versus the context in a given environment may explain the relationship between emotions and moods, and clarify why, for instance, certain emotions can alter a mood while others do not. The paper begins putting forward the general take on emotions from a Situated Approach to Emotions in line with contemporary discussions of the Situated Approach to Mind (Gallagher 2009; Griffiths and Scarantino 2009). Then, following John Dewey’s conception of situation, the paper distinguishes situation from context, and reviews the hypothesis of equating them with emotions and moods respectively. The analysis is then connected to previous research done on the difference between emotions and moods and, given the insightful outcomes of connecting moods and emotions to other theoretical aspects of the mind, it shows how the link to the environment is a crucial trait to understand emotions and moods dynamics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aldao, A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). The influence of context on the implementation of adaptive emotion regulation strategies. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50, 493–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Armon-Jones, C. (1991). Varieties of affect. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., & Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(5), 286–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Ze’ve, A. (2001). The subtlety of emotions. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonanno, G. A., Pat-Horenczyk, R., & Noll, J. (2011). Coping flexibility and trauma: the perceived ability to cope with trauma (PACT) scale. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice and Policy, 3, 117–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, T. (2000). What is a situation? History and Philosophy of Logic, 21(2), 95–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, C. (2001). Assessing coping flexibility in real-life and laboratory settings: a multimethod approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 814–833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58, 7–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (pp. 323–417). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colombetti, G. (2009). From affect programs to dynamical discrete-emotions. Philosophical Psychology, 22, 407–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colombetti, G. (2014). The feeling body: Affective science meets the enactive mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1925a). Experience and nature. Volume 1: 1925. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), John Dewey: the later works: 1925–1953 (1988). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

  • Dewey, J. (1925b) Value, objective reference and criticism. Volume 2: 1925-1927 Essays, Reviews, Miscellany, and The Public and Its Problems In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), John Dewey: the later works: 1925–1953 (1988, pp. 78–97). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

  • Dewey, J. (1934). Art as Esperience. Volume 10: 1934 In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), John Dewey: the later works: 1925–1953 (1989). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

  • Dewey, J. (1938). Logic: the theory of inquiry. Volume 12: 1938. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), John Dewey: the later works: 1925–1953 (1991). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

  • Ekma, P., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). The nature of emotion: fundamental questions. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1994). Moods, emotions and traits. In P. Ekman & R. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp. 56–58). Oxford: Oxfrod University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eldar, E., Rutledge, R. B., Dolan, R. J., & Niv, Y. (2015). Mood as representation of momentum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.010.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1994). Varieties of affect: Emotions and episodes, moods, and sentiments. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion. Fundamental questions (pp. 59–67). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2009). Philosophical antecedents of situated cognition. In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of situated cognition (pp. 35–51). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2012). Time, emotion, and depression. Emotion Review, 4(2), 127–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldie, P. (2000). The emotions. A philosophical exploration. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

  • Griffiths, P. E. (2013). Current emotion research in philosophy. Emotion Review, 5(2), 215–222.

  • Griffiths, P., & Scarantino, A. (2009). Emotions in the wild: the situated perspective on emotion. In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition (pp. 437–453). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 865–878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, R. (1987). Objectless emotions. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 48(1), 107–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. (1998). The emotional brain. New York: Phoenix.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lormand, E. (1985). Towards a theory of moods. Philosophical Studies, 4, 385–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mackay, D. S. (1942). What does Mr. Dewey mean by an “indeterminate situation”? Volume 15: 1942–1948 Essays, reviews and miscellany. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), John Dewey: the later works: 1925–1953 (1991, pp. 393–401). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press. 

  • Mendonça, D. (2012). Pattern of sentiment-following a Deweyan suggestion. Transactions of Charles Peirce Society, 48(2), 209–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesquita, B. (2010). Emoting. A contextualized process. In B. Mesquita, L. F. Barrett, & E. R. Smith (Eds.), The mind in context. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, W. N. (1989). Mood. The frame of mind. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Northoff, G. (2012). From emotions to consciousness – A neurophenomenal and neuro-relational approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(303), 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parkinson, B., Fisher, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2005). Emotions in social relations: Cultural, group and interpersonal processes. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, P., & Aydede, M. (Eds.). (2009). The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohlfing, K., Rehm, M., & Goecke, K. U. (2003). Situatedness: the interplay between context(s) and situation. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 3(2), 132–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, E. L. (1998). Levels of analysis and the organization of affect. Review of General Psychology, 2, 247–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rottenberg, J. (2005). Mood and emotion in major depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 167–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rottenberg, J., Gross, J. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2005). Emotion context insensitivity in major depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 627–639.

  • Siemer, M. (2009). Mood experience: implications of a dispositional theory of moods. Emotion Review, 1(3), 256–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R. (2008). The Philosophy of emotions. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 3–16). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, A. (2012). Emotions, existential feelings, and their regulation. Emotion Review, 4(2), 157–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephen, A., Walter, S., & Wilutzky, W. (2014). Emotions beyond brain and body. Philosophical Psychology, 27(1), 65–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

An earlier version of the paper was presented at the Workshop of the European Society for the Study of Emotions (EPSSE) on The Meaning of Moods in December, 2015 in Basel, Switzerland, organized by Professor Angelika Krebs, Professor Aaron Ben-Ze’ev and Professor Heleen Pott. I like to thank the organizers and all other participants for fruitful and insightful conversations. The author’s research is supported by post-doc fellowship granted by Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (SFRH/BPD/102507/2014).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dina Mendonça.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mendonça, D. Situating Moods. Philosophia 45, 1453–1467 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11406-017-9844-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11406-017-9844-x

Keywords

Navigation