Skip to main content
Log in

On the role of contextual factors in cognitive neuroscience experiments: a mechanistic approach

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Experiments in cognitive neuroscience build a setup whose set of controlled stimuli and rules elicits a cognitive process in a participant. This setup requires researchers to decide the value of quite a few parameters along several dimensions. We call ‘’contextual factors’’ the parameters often assumed not to change the cognitive process elicited and are free to vary across the experiment’s repetitions. Against this assumption, empirical evidence shows that many of these contextual factors can significantly influence cognitive performance. Nevertheless, it is not entirely clear what it means for a cognitive phenomenon to be context-sensitive and how to identify context-sensitivity experimentally. We claim that a phenomenon can be context-sensitive either because it is only triggered within a specific context or because different contexts change its manifestation conditions. Assessing which of these forms of context-sensitivity is present in a given phenomenon requires a criterion for individuating it across contextual variations. We argue that some inter-level experiments that, within the mechanistic approach to explanation, are required to identify relations of constitutive relevance between a phenomenon and a mechanism, are also necessary for individuating the phenomenon across its contextual variations. We articulate a criterion according to which behavioral variations across contexts indicate different phenomena if and only if the mechanistic activities, components and/or organizational properties recruited in each context are different. We support this approach by showing how it is applied in paradigmatic studies addressing cognitive performance differences resulting from contextual variations of task features, such as stimulus type and response modality. Finally, we address the challenge that a form of context-sensitivity possessed by the so-called ‘multifunctional mechanisms’ is incompatible with our proposal because it entails that the same mechanism can be recruited in different contexts to produce different phenomena. We examine key cases of multifunctionality and argue that they are consistent with our proposal because a single mechanism can have different components, activities and/or organizational properties in different contexts. Thus, these modifications may not affect the identity of a mechanism, and they could explain how it produced different phenomena in those contexts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. see Burnston (2020) for an alternative approach showing that context-sensitivity is consistent with decomposition.

  2. See Wajnerman Paz (2018) for a discussion of a more fine-grained (neural-coding level) approach to the mechanistic basis of grounded cognition.

  3. We thank an anonymous reviewer for this observation.

  4. We thank an anonymous reviewer for this observation.

References

  • Adolph, K. E. (2020). Ecological validity: Mistaking the lab for real life. My Biggest Research Mistake: Adventures and Misadventures in Psychological Research (pp. 187–190). SAGE Publications, Inc. 2455 Teller Road, Thousand Oaks California 91320

  • Al, E., Iliopoulos, F., Forschack, N., Nierhaus, T., Grund, M., Motyka, P., et al. (2020). Heart-brain interactions shape somatosensory perception and evoked potentials. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117(19), 10575–10584

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. L. (2010). Neural reuse: a fundamental organizational principle of the brain. The Behavioral and brain sciences, 33(4), 245–66; discussion 266–313

  • Ashworth, E. T., Cotter, J. D., & Kilding, A. E. (2021). Impact of elevated core temperature on cognition in hot environments within a military context. European journal of applied physiology, 121(4), 1061–1071

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Azzalini, D., Rebollo, I., & Tallon-Baudry, C. (2019). Visceral Signals Shape Brain Dynamics and Cognition. Trends in cognitive sciences, 23(6), 488–509

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bale, M. R., & Petersen, R. S. (2009). Transformation in the neural code for whisker deflection direction along the lemniscal pathway. Journal of neurophysiology, 102(5), 2771–2780

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2016). Situated conceptualization: Theory and applications. In Y. Coello (Ed.), Foundations of embodied cognition: Perceptual and emotional embodiment, (pp (Vol. 290, pp. 11–37). New York, NY, US: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group, viii

  • Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, K., Barbey, W., A. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in cognitive sciences, 7(2), 84–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel, W., & Abrahamsen, A. (2005). Explanation: a mechanist alternative. Studies in history and philosophy of biological and biomedical sciences, 36(2), 421–441

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beshel, J., Kopell, N., & Kay, L. M. (2007). Olfactory bulb gamma oscillations are enhanced with task demands. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 27(31), 8358–8365

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binder, J. R. (2016). In defense of abstract conceptual representations. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 23(4), 1096–1108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boone, W., & Piccinini, G. (2016). The cognitive neuroscience revolution. Synthese, 193(5), 1509–1534

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, J. S., Vankov, I. I., Damian, M. F., & Davis, C. J. (2016). Why do some neurons in cortex respond to information in a selective manner? Insights from artificial neural networks. Cognition, 148, 47–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, J. C. (1972). Thorndike’s puzzle boxes. Journal of the history of the behavioral sciences, 8(2), 159–167

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnston, D. C. (2020). Getting over atomism: Functional decomposition in complex neural systems.The British journal for the philosophy of science,000–000

  • Chance, P. (1999). Thorndike’s Puzzle Boxes And The Origins Of The Experimental Analysis Of Behavior. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 72(3), 433–440

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Correa, C. M. C., Noorman, S., Jiang, J., Palminteri, S., Cohen, M. X., Lebreton, M., & van Gaal, S. (2018). How the Level of Reward Awareness Changes the Computational and Electrophysiological Signatures of Reinforcement Learning. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 38(48), 10338–10348

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craver, C. F. (2007). Explaining the Brain. Oxford University Press. Accessed 2 October 2021

  • Craver, C. F. (2014). The Ontic Account of Scientific Explanation. In M. I. Kaiser, O. R. Scholz, D. Plenge, & A. Hüttemann (Eds.), Explanation in the Special Sciences: The Case of Biology and History (pp. 27–52). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Donders, F. C. (1969). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(69)90065-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J. (2001). An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex. Nature reviews Neuroscience, 2(11), 820–829

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evarts, E. V. (1966). Methods for recording activity of individual neurons in moving animals. Methods in medical research (pp. 241–250). Chicago, IL: Year Book Medical Publishers

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B. (1953). The use of the free operant in the analysis of behavior. Psychological bulletin, 50(4), 263

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foldiak, P., & Endres, D. (2008). Sparse coding. Scholarpedia journal, 3(1), 2984

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, D. E., Brown, A., Brim, E., Mehta, N., Vujovic, M., & Kay, L. M. (2016). Gamma and Beta Oscillations Define a Sequence of Neurocognitive Modes Present in Odor Processing. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 36(29), 7750–7767

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, D. E., Rojas-Líbano, D., Scott, M., & Kay, L. M. (2011). Rat behavior in go/no-go and two-alternative choice odor discrimination: differences and similarities. Behavioral neuroscience, 125(4), 588–603

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freiberger, J. J., Derrick, B. J., Natoli, M. J., Akushevich, I., Schinazi, E. A., Parker, C., et al. (2016). Assessment of the interaction of hyperbaric N2, CO2, and O2 on psychomotor performance in divers. Journal of applied physiology, 121(4), 953–964

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, W. R. (1953). An informational analysis of absolute judgments of loudness. Journal of experimental psychology, 46(5), 373–380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, A. (2015). The Connection Between Varying Treatment Effects and the Crisis of Unreplicable Research: A Bayesian Perspective. Journal of management, 41(2), 632–643

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodhew, S. C., & Edwards, M. (2019). Translating experimental paradigms into individual-differences research: Contributions, challenges, and practical recommendations. Consciousness and cognition, 69, 14–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagner, M. (2012). The electrical excitability of the brain: toward the emergence of an experiment. Journal of the history of the neurosciences, 21(3), 237–249

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heron, W. T., & Skinner, B. F. (1939). An apparatus for the study of animal behavior. The Psychological record, 3, 166–176

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasper, H., Ricci, G. F., & Doane, B. (1958). Patterns of cortical neuronal discharge during conditioned responses in monkeys. In G. E. W. Wolstenholme, & C. O’Connor (Eds.), Neurological basis of behaviour (pp. 277–294). Boston, MA: Little, Brown and Company

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, C. (2012). Cognitive Ontology and Region- versus Network-Oriented Analyses. Philosophy of science, 79(5), 952–960

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehky, S. R., Sejnowski, T. J., & Desimone, R. (2005). Selectivity and sparseness in the responses of striate complex cells. Vision research, 45(1), 57–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leutgeb, S., Leutgeb, J. K., Barnes, C. A., Moser, E. I., McNaughton, B. L., & Moser, M. B. (2005). Independent codes for spatial and episodic memory in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Science, 309(5734), 619–623

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liljeholm, M., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2012). Contributions of the striatum to learning, motivation, and performance: an associative account. Trends in cognitive sciences, 16(9), 467–475

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Machamer, P., Darden, L., & Craver, C. F. (2000). Thinking about Mechanisms. Philosophy of science, 67(1), 1–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, A. A., Rhoads, S. A., & Ryan, R. M. (2019). A multi-semester classroom demonstration yields evidence in support of the facial feedback effect. Emotion, 19(8), 1500–1504

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C., Beshel, J., & Kay, L. M. (2007). An olfacto-hippocampal network is dynamically involved in odor-discrimination learning. Journal of neurophysiology, 98(4), 2196–2205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCaffrey, J. B. (2015). The Brain’s Heterogeneous Functional Landscape. Philosophy of science, 82(5), 1010–1022

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, L. C., Shaikh, S. J., Jeong, D. C., Wang, L., Gillig, T. K., Godoy, C. G., et al. (2019). Causal Inference in Generalizable Environments: Systematic Representative Design. Psychological inquiry, 30(4), 173–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mountcastle, V. B. (1978). Brain mechanisms for directed attention. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 71(1), 14–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller-Pinzler, L., Gazzola, V., Keysers, C., Sommer, J., Jansen, A., Frässle, S., et al. (2015). Neural pathways of embarrassment and their modulation by social anxiety. Neuroimage, 119, 252–261

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, P. R., Vandekerckhove, J., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2014). Pupil-linked arousal determines variability in perceptual decision making.PLoS computational biology, 10(9), e1003854

  • Noah, T., Schul, Y., & Mayo, R. (2018). When both the original study and its failed replication are correct: Feeling observed eliminates the facial-feedback effect. Journal of personality and social psychology, 114(5), 657–664

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Page, M. (2000). Connectionist modelling in psychology: a localist manifesto. The Behavioral and brain sciences, 23(4), 443–67; discussion 467–512

  • Park, H. D., Correia, S., Ducorps, A., & Tallon-Baudry, C. (2014). Spontaneous fluctuations in neural responses to heartbeats predict visual detection. Nature neuroscience, 17(4), 612–618

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Verifying different-modality properties for concepts produces switching costs. Psychological science, 14(2), 119–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perl, O., Ravia, A., Rubinson, M., Eisen, A., Soroka, T., Mor, N., et al. (2019). Human non-olfactory cognition phase-locked with inhalation. Nature human behaviour, 3(5), 501–512

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M., & Raichle, M. E. (1988). Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing. Nature, 331(6157), 585–589

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piazza, M., Mechelli, A., Price, C. J., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Exact and approximate judgements of visual and auditory numerosity: an fMRI study. Brain research, 1106(1), 177–188

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piccinini, G., & Craver, C. (2011). Integrating psychology and neuroscience: functional analyses as mechanism sketches. Synthese, 183(3), 283–311

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollack, I. (1952). The Information of Elementary Auditory Displays. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 24(6), 745–749

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., & Raichle, M. E. (1988). Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. Science, 240(4859), 1627–1631. Accessed 21 July 2021

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price, C. J., & Friston, K. J. (2005). Functional ontologies for cognition: The systematic definition of structure and function. Cognitive neuropsychology, 22(3), 262–275

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quian Quiroga, R., & Panzeri, S. (2009). Extracting information from neuronal populations: information theory and decoding approaches. Nature reviews Neuroscience, 10(3), 173–185

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quiroga, R. Q., & Panzeri, S. (2013). Principles of Neural Coding. CRC Press

  • Reilly, J., Peelle, J. E., Garcia, A., & Crutch, S. J. (2016). Linking somatic and symbolic representation in semantic memory: the dynamic multilevel reactivation framework. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 23(4), 1002–1014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rinberg, D., Koulakov, A., & Gelperin, A. (2006). Speed-accuracy tradeoff in olfaction. Neuron, 51(3), 351–358

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seghier, M. L., & Price, C. J. (2018). Interpreting and Utilising Intersubject Variability in Brain Function. Trends in cognitive sciences, 22(6), 517–530

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., & Mendelsohn, A. (2019). Real-Life Neuroscience: An Ecological Approach to Brain and Behavior Research. Perspectives on psychological science: a journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 14(5), 841–859

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1930). On the conditions of elicitation of certain eating reflexes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 16(6), 433–438

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinborn, M. B., & Huestegge, L. (2020). Socially alerted cognition evoked by a confederate’s mere presence: analysis of reaction-time distributions and delta plots. Psychological research, 84(5), 1424–1439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153(3736), 652–654

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, M. G., Kusunoki, M., Sigala, N., Nili, H., Gaffan, D., & Duncan, J. (2013). Dynamic coding for cognitive control in prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 78(2), 364–375

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F. (2016, November). Reflection on the Smiling Registered Replication Report. Perspectives on psychological science: a journal of the Association for Psychological Science

  • Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of personality and social psychology, 54(5), 768–777

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, J. A. (2008). The multiplicity of experimental protocols: a challenge to reductionist and non-reductionist models of the unity of neuroscience. Synthese, 167(3), 511

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, J. A. (2010). Reconsidering “spatial memory” and the Morris water maze. Synthese, 177(2), 261–283

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suri, R. E., & Schultz, W. (2001). Temporal difference model reproduces anticipatory neural activity. Neural computation, 13(4), 841–862

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swets, J. A. (1973). The Relative Operating Characteristic in Psychology: A technique for isolating effects of response bias finds wide use in the study of perception and cognition. Science, 182(4116), 990–1000

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, S. (1995). Visual recognition: From retina to inferotemporal cortex. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 5(4), 389–410

    Google Scholar 

  • Tian, X., Fang, Z., & Liu, W. (2021). Decreased humidity improves cognitive performance at extreme high indoor temperature. Indoor air, 31(3), 608–627

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valero-Cuevas, F. J., Venkadesan, M., & Todorov, E. (2009). Structured variability of muscle activations supports the minimal intervention principle of motor control. Journal of neurophysiology, 102(1), 59–68

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Bavel, J. J., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Brady, W. J., & Reinero, D. A. (2016). Contextual sensitivity in scientific reproducibility. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(23), 6454–6459

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, A., Hennig, R. M., & Ronacher, B. (2005). Increase of neuronal response variability at higher processing levels as revealed by simultaneous recordings. Journal of neurophysiology, 93(6), 3548–3559

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagenmakers, E. J., Beek, T., Dijkhoff, L., Gronau, Q. F., Acosta, A., Adams, R. B., et al. (2016). Registered Replication Report: Strack, Martin, & Stepper (1988). Perspectives on psychological science: a journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 11(6), 917–928

  • Wahn, B., Rohe, T., Gearhart, A., Kingstone, A., & Sinnett, S. (2020). Performing a task jointly enhances the sound-induced flash illusion. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 73(12), 2260–2271

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wajnerman Paz, A. (2018). An efficient coding approach to the debate on grounded cognition. Synthese, 195(12), 5245–5269

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watrous, A. J., Deuker, L., Fell, J., & Axmacher, N. (2015). Phase-amplitude coupling supports phase coding in human ECoG. eLife, 4. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.07886

  • Woolgar, A., Hampshire, A., Thompson, R., & Duncan, J. (2011). Adaptive coding of task-relevant information in human frontoparietal cortex. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 31(41), 14592–14599. Accessed 29 March 2022

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by Fondo Nacional de Desarrollo Científico y Tecnológico Fondecyt, Proyecto 11190604, awarded to DRL. AWP receives funding from Fondo Nacional de Desarrollo Científico y Tecnológico Fondecyt, Proyectos 11220327 (IR: Abel Wajnerman Paz), 1210091 (IR: Juan Manuel Garrido) and 1200197 (IR: Francisco Pereira). AWP thanks members of the Fondecyt project 1210091(specially Juan Manuel Garrido, José Tomás Alvarado, Jorge Fuentes Muñoz) for their generous and ongoing feedback on different versions of this manuscript. Also, a very special thanks to the reviewers from Synthese for a really insightful and constructive discussion that was critical for arriving at the final version of the manuscript. Finally, he would like to thank DRL for a very rewarding and enjoyable co-authoring process. DRL would like to thank Francisco Parada, Alejandra Rossi, and Christian Salas for enriching and insightful discussions and for making the CENHN an inspiring place to do neuroscience. He also warmly thanks to AWP for his enthusiasm and dedication to a delightful co-thinking and co-writing process. Finally, DRL also thanks the anonymous reviewers for their suggestions and comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AWP and DRL contributed to the study’s conception and development. AWP and DRL wrote the first draft of the manuscript and revised all subsequent versions. AWP and DRL read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel Rojas-Líbano.

Ethics declarations

Statements and declarations

The authors have no competing interests to declare relevant to this article’s content.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wajnerman-Paz, A., Rojas-Líbano, D. On the role of contextual factors in cognitive neuroscience experiments: a mechanistic approach. Synthese 200, 402 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-022-03870-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-022-03870-0

Keywords

Navigation