1 Introduction

International large-scale assessments (ILSAs) such as PISA or TIMSS reveal considerable variation in both the mean performance levels and the extent of social inequality that exists within participating countries. Regarding social inequality, the most commonly studied social categories are socioeconomic status (SES), immigration status, and student gender (e.g., Andon et al., 2014; Rosén et al., 2022; Jerrim et al., 2019). The answers to our research questions could have multiple implications for educational monitoring, as well as for research on social inequality in student achievement. If achievement gaps between different social categories are highly correlated, then examining them separately adds little value for educational monitoring, and their reporting should be reframed. In this scenario, it also seems plausible that research findings on the institutional determinants of social inequality would be consistent across different social categories. But if different social gaps are largely uncorrelated, there is a need for a differentiated perspective in education policy and research.

This study empirically examines whether there is a single broad social inequality, or whether there is a need to distinguish between different forms of social inequalities corresponding to the categories of SES, immigration background, and gender. Are there countries which systematically show social inequality in student performance across different categories, or are countries characterized by a higher degree of inequality in one social category and lower in another? To address this question, we first use data from an international large-scale assessment to compute social inequalities in SES, immigration status, and gender. We use this data to review the variability in social inequality for each social category across countries, before evaluating the correlation between the three measures at country-level. To further validate these analyses, we conduct a comparative study and investigate the association between various institutional features and the three forms of social inequalities. Specifically, are institutional features of school systems consistently associated with all forms of social inequality or only to specific ones? This study is explorative and aims to contribute to the discussion on how researchers can evaluate education systems.

2 Social Inequalities in Achievement on International Analyses

2.1 The Concept of Social Inequality

Inequality in education can be conceptualized using different terms, with different normative ideas about injustice and the evaluation of education systems (Strietholt, 2014; Brighouse & Swift, 2008). The concept of social inequality—similar to ‘inequity’ and ‘inequality of opportunities’ (c.f. UNESCO, 2018)—problematizes achievement differences that originate from the social origin of the student, rather than from effort or ability. In educational research, the most commonly used social categories are SES, immigration background, and gender. While these are the three categories studied in this article, we acknowledge that there are other characteristics determined by social origin that are also related to inequalities within education, such as religion, sexual orientation, ethnicity, and place of residence (e.g., urbanicity).

Understanding the categories of social inequality presents a dilemma. Different categories of social inequalities have some common aspects. First, since there are great differentials in the outcomes and trajectories of students, we can expect that there are groups that are able to take more advantage than others in a systematic way. This is especially true in contexts of high general inequality, i.e., high dispersion in outcomes, where differentials between students are bigger and there is more variance that can be unevenly distributed. Second, we selected these three categories of social inequality because they are present in most education systems in the world and have been an ongoing topic in educational research for decades. The global relevance of these three categories enable us to hypothesize that there is one broad ‘umbrella’ social inequality, in which these social distinctions (SES, immigration, gender) are associated with the distribution of outcomes simultaneously, i.e., highly associated between the three of them. However, different categories of social inequality within an education system emerge for different reasons. The association between each social category with performance outcomes could run on parallel paths, implying null correlations between them.

Researchers have been able to measure the different categories of social inequality in achievement on an international perspective since the mid-twentieth century. Currently, the three largest ongoing international large-scale assessments measuring achievement in school students are the OECD’s Program for International Student Assessment (PISA), the International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement’s (IEA) Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS), and the Progress in International Reading Literacy Study (PIRLS). Below, we present a short review of the current international evidence and the theories explaining each category of social inequality. We will explore prominent theories regarding the emergence of achievement gaps related to SES, gender, and immigration. Our aim is to demonstrate that the underlying mechanisms behind these gaps are fundamentally distinct from one another. Subsequently, we will examine previous research that investigates the correlation between institutional features of educational systems and the three distinct types of inequality. In addition, it is worth noting that previous research in this area has been somewhat fragmented. There has been a lack of systematic evaluations where the same data were utilized to study the relationships between institutional features and the various forms of inequality. Moreover, the few studies that have attempted this approach have yielded inconsistent findings.

2.2 SES Inequality in Achievement

The association between family SES and student achievement can be explained by the tendency of children from families with a lower socioeconomic background to receive fewer resources for their education. This difference in resources accumulates along the children’s developmental trajectory and generates disparities in achievement between children from different families. This is further exacerbated by the inheritability of resources between generations that increases the resource gap between families. According to Bourdieu’s theory, these resources are manifested first as economic resources (e.g., families with higher incomes can send their students to private schools or afford private tutoring) and later manifest in cultural and social capital (Bourdieu, 1986; Broer et al., 2019; Coleman, 1988, 1990).

The association between a student’s SES background and performance has been a common finding across studies, cycles, and subjects, though with differences between countries in the association’s magnitude (Hopfenbeck et al., 2018). PISA 2018 (OECD, 2019a) shows a positive association between SES and achievement in all countries and in all three subjects (reading, mathematics, and science), with SES explaining between 2 and 24% of the variance in performance, depending on the country and subject. TIMSS (Mullis et al., 2020) and PIRLS (Mullis et al., 2017) presented similar patterns in their latest editions in 2019 and 2016, respectively. While these are recent results, SES inequality in performance is not new and has even increased in some countries (Broer et al., 2019; Chmielewski, 2019).

2.3 Immigration Inequality in Achievement

The association between immigration background and student achievement can be explained by two groups of mechanisms: structural and cultural (Nauck, 2019). The structural mechanism is the inherent disadvantage experienced by immigrant groups due to their economic reality. Families with immigrant backgrounds show lower academic performance or take different educational choices due to their poorer access to resources (both economic and social). Cultural explanations ultimately focus on why certain groups of immigrants or ethnic groups perform better than others; the disadvantage is explained in terms of different mindsets. Studies have shown that the gap between immigrant and native students is not only due to immigrant families’ lower SES, but also due to speaking a different language at home, sociocultural factors, system-wide factors of the origin and destination countries (such as political stability, economic development, and religion), and the destination countries’ policies (Buchmann & Parrado, 2006; Dronkers & Levels, 2007; Jackson, 2012; Levels et al., 2008; Schmid, 2001; Strand, 2011, 2014).

Most international research in English on the association between immigration and achievement has focused on Western European countries and the USA. In European countries, students who speak a different language at home perform worse in PISA, especially at reading (Lenkeit et al., 2017). Moreover, most studies using ILSAs data have each only investigated a limited set of countries, focusing on the differences between immigrant groups within a country, e.g., the disadvantage of Turkish communities within Germany (Söhn & Özcan, 2006), or how immigrants are disadvantaged in the USA depending on their origin country (Worrell, 2014). As most research is centered in European and North American contexts, some other contexts are excluded. In Qatar and United Arab Emirates, immigrant children perform better than natives, supposedly because these countries attract high-skilled immigrants and their education systems are tailored to this (Bouhlila, 2017). Overall, the achievement gap varies greatly across the assessed countries, contents, and cycles (Andon et al., 2014).

2.4 Gender Inequality in Achievement

There are different and longstanding theories on why gender gaps in student achievement tests occur, and they can be divided into two broad explanations: nature and nurture (see overviews by Halpern, 2012; Hyde, 2014). The nature category includes theories that assume innate, stable differences between boys and girls that affect learning processes. The comprehensive literature on cognitive gender gaps suggests, however, that boys and girls mostly score equally on cognitive ability tests (cf. Gender Similarity Hypothesis; Hyde, 2014; Zell et al., 2015). In contrast, the nurture category includes theories about environmental influences differing between boys and girls. Nurture-related theoretical perspectives all suggest that societal gender norms and existing gender differences in education transmit to students, perpetuating educational gender inequalities. For instance, stereotypical beliefs about science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) subjects being male domains and a higher representation of men in STEM majors at school and university level or in the STEM labor market can lead to girls underestimating their abilities in these subjects, potentially impacting their achievement (Eccles et al., 1990; Halpern, 2012; Neuville & Croizet, 2007).

International comparative studies document pronounced gender gap differences between countries and academic achievement domains. Girls outperform boys in reading in most countries at both the primary and secondary school level. Gender gaps in the participating countries range between non-existent reading gender gaps to large advantages for girls (Mullis et al., 2017; OECD, 2019a). Gender gaps are more varied in mathematics, with medium advantages for boys in some countries, some countries without gender gaps, and even some countries with medium advantages for girls (Mullis et al., 2020; OECD, 2019a). Interestingly, gender gaps in reading and mathematics appear to correlate; countries with pronounced reading advantages for girls also tend to show mathematics advantages for girls, and countries without reading advantages for girls tend to show mathematics advantages for boys (Guiso et al., 2008; Stoet & Geary, 2013). Furthermore, gender gaps in academic achievement appear to be quite stable over time (Rosén et al., 2022; Steinmann, et al., 2023; Meinck & Brese, 2019).

2.5 Covariates of Social Inequalities

Within each category of social inequality, the associations between social origin with performance vary between countries. This suggests that institutional features of education systems generate variations in social inequality (Jerrim et al., 2019). We next review some studies that have identified institutional features related to social inequality in achievement. We explore whether previous studies suggest that institutional covariates are associated in the same way with different forms of social inequality.

2.5.1 Education-System Factors

One important feature of education systems is the level of differentiation, seen in policies such as between-school tracking, in which students are sorted into different types of schools. If transitions and school choice are affected by social characteristics, either by the achievement differential between social groups or by different decisions taken after considering children’s skills, differentiation in the education system should lead to larger social achievement gaps. Previous international studies have found that educational differentiation (specifically between-school tracking) increases SES inequality in achievement (Strello et al., 2021; Lavrijsen & Nicaise, 2016; van de Werfhorst, 2018; van de Werfhorst & Mijs, 2010). There is less research on the effect of tracking on immigration inequality in achievement and the findings are inconsistent; some studies suggest a positive effect while others do not (Bodovski & Munoz, 2020; Ruhose & Schwerdt, 2016; Teltemann & Schunck, 2016). Between-school tracking has mixed effects on gender inequality, with studies consistently showing that later tracking increases the gender gap in reading (in favor of girls), but heterogeneous results regarding the effect on mathematics and science (Bodovski & Munoz, 2020; Hermann & Kopasz, 2019; Scheeren & Bol, 2022). Similar results have been found in studies on general education-system differentiation indexes (Ayalon & Livneh, 2013; van Hek et al., 2019; van Langen et al., 2006).

2.5.2 External Factors

A common factor in comparative research is the level of economic development of a country or education system. In general, previous studies have found mixed evidence on its effect on social inequality. Measures such as GDP (gross domestic product) per capita are inconsistently associated with SES achievement inequality (Bodovski & Munoz, 2020; Chmielewski, 2019; Ferreira & Gignoux, 2014; Schütz et al., 2008). Previous studies have also found mixed results regarding the association between SES inequality and public expenditure on education, although the association seems more markedly negative when considering countries’ development levels (Strietholt et al., 2019). Chmielewsky (2019) found that income inequality (measured as Gini) has a positive association with SES inequality in mid and low-income countries. Using TIMSS 2011 data, Bodovski and Munoz (2020) found an inverse association between GDP per capita and the immigrant achievement gap (in particular, richer countries have a lower gap between immigrants and native students), but found no association with the gender achievement gap.

Cultural features may also play a role in gender inequalities in achievement. In more gender-egalitarian countries, the relative performance of girls over boys is higher, especially in reading (see review of Rosén et al., 2022; González de San Román & de La Rica, 2016; Guiso et al., 2008; Marks, 2008; Reilly, 2012). Nosek et al. (2009) found that in societies with more marked stereotypes (e.g., regarding science as a male domain and liberal arts as a female domain), the gap in favor of boys is larger in mathematics.

3 The Present Study

Previous research on social inequality has identified a number of social categories related to student achievement, with the most prominent categories in international comparative research being SES, immigration, and gender. The theories explaining the emergence of each performance gap differ, and research on the three areas has developed relatively independently.

Only a few studies have explicitly compared the different gaps. Lenkeit et al. (2017) studied the relative importance of the three categories of achievement gaps, though only in four Western European countries (Germany, Sweden, France, and United Kingdom). They estimated multilevel models using data from PISA 2000 to 2012. The authors concluded that each category of social inequality is important for explaining the disparities between students, and that results have remained stable in those four countries. Bodovski et al. (2020) studied how different country-level predictors may mitigate the three categories of social inequality. The authors used information from TIMSS 2011 with a sample of 45 countries. They found mixed effects between the different social inequality domains, showing that the role of school system features cannot be generalized over the different categories of social inequality. Whether large gaps in one social category are associated with large gaps in another social category has not been the subject of research to date.

In this study, we explore the relationship between the three categories of social inequality on achievement, and the degree to which they are correlated or uncorrelated. We investigate whether countries can be evaluated as more or less socially unequal based on one only category, or how important it is to evaluate the effects of certain policies on different categories of inequality. Specifically, we aim to answer the following research question:

How correlated are the three categories of social inequality in achievement (socioeconomic status, immigration background, and gender)?

A high correlation between the different types of social inequality would suggest that the differentiation between the three types of social inequality has no additional empirical value, whereas low correlations would underpin the importance of a differentiated view of social inequalities.

Furthermore, we proceed to examine the nomological linkages between the three distinct types of social inequality and external variables. This line of inquiry aligns with the principles of construct validity (Cronbach & Meehl, 1955). By assessing how different types of inequality correlate with relevant variables, we can gather evidence supporting their meaningful distinction. More specifically, our investigation focuses on the relationship between education system-level features and social inequality across the various categories of social inequality.

Different patterns in the regression parameters would provide evidence that the three types of inequality need to be differentiated when analyzing social inequality. If there are no differences in the regression estimates, however, differentiating between the types of social inequality would not provide additional empirical value.

4 Methods

4.1 Data Sources

To study the correlation between the different categories of social inequalities, we use the OECD’s Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA). This study measures 15-year-olds’ proficiency in mathematics, reading, and science. Specifically, we use the dataset of PISA 2018 focusing on the mathematics assessment. We remove Korea and Vietnam from the sample as they sampled fewer than 20 students with immigrant backgrounds (as defined in the Variables chapter). The remaining sample of n = 76 education systemsFootnote 1 is heterogeneous and covers all parts of the world. Each country contains a sample between 3296 to 35,493 students (mean: 7791), with the number of schools ranging between 44 and 1089 schools (mean: 279). The total sample contains 592,145 students from 21,264 schools. Table 1 shows the total N of students and schools per country.

Table 1 Proportion of social groups and subsamples N, N of students, N of schools by country

PISA draws a stratified two-stage sampling. The first stage samples schools within the country or education system, and the second stage samples 15-year-old students within those schools. The results are representative of the population at both the student-level and the school-level. However, PISA sample only students enrolled within schools, meaning that interpretations of these results must consider that some specific countries/regions have lower proportions of secondary-school attainment and therefore exclude early school leavers (Steinmann & Rutkowski, 2023).

4.2 Analysis

Do all measures of inequality show the same picture, or is it necessary to differentiate between multiple types of social inequality? Are social achievement gaps consistent, or are there countries in which certain social gaps are high and others low? To address these questions empirically, we examine whether different measures of social inequality in student achievement lead to the same or different rankings in international comparisons. All analyses are based on the three types of social inequality in student achievement available for the n = 76 participants in PISA: SES, immigrant background, and gender.

Our analysis consists of three steps. First, we identify the three gaps per country (see Variables section below). Second, we examine the correlation of these different types of social inequality at the country level. Third, we attempt to validate the correlational analyses by regression analyses. We regress the three types of social inequalities on a set of institutional features and compare the regression parameters for the three outcomes. We use cross-sectional data, and the aim of the regression analyses is not to estimate causal effects or bring substantive conclusions, but rather to examine whether different social inequalities are associated differently with various system-level features.

4.3 Variables

4.3.1 Social Achievement Gap on Mathematics

The main variables of interest are measures of three categories of social inequalities in achievement. Achievement scores in PISA are calculated so that they had an international mean of 500 and an international standard deviation of 100 points in the first edition in 2000. The scores are designed to be comparable between countries. In our analysis, we focus on mathematics achievement.

The three achievement gaps were calculated for gender, SES, and immigration status, using the simple mean difference between the groups (described in the next section). We divided these gaps by the standard deviation of the mathematics scores observed in the respective country, to account for cross-country variation in the dispersion of the test scores. Therefore, all gaps are measured as Cohen’s standardized effect sizes d. For example, a gender gap of 1 means that boys perform on average one standard deviation better than girls. Probability weights were used in the estimation of the achievement gaps and standard error account for the sample design using replications weights. We followed the Balanced Repeated Replication (BRR) method, as indicated by PISA guidelines (OECD, 2019b). All ten plausible values available in the PISA public database were used on the analyses following Rubin’s rules (Rubin, 1987). The three achievement gaps, by country, can be found in Appendix (Table 3).

4.3.1.1 SES Achievement Gap

We used parental education as a measure of socioeconomic status and compared students with parents with university education (ISCED 5A) against parents with less than university education. If the educational attainment of the parents differed, we used the highest educational attainment reached between both parents—i.e., one parent having university level education is enough to be considered in the highest category. We marked parental education as missing if there was no information about both parents. We opted for a single proxy of SES instead of a complex index, such as the ESCS reported in PISA, for the sake of simplicity and consistency with the other categories that also use a single indicator. Table 1 shows the proportion of parents with university education. There is a high heterogeneity between countries on socioeconomic levels. This proportion ranges from 7% in Vietnam to 73% in Denmark.

4.3.1.2 Immigration Achievement Gap

We operationalize the immigration background by comparing students whose parents were both born abroad with students with one or no parent born abroad. We categorize the first group as “immigrant” and the second as “native”, aware that this is a simplified category of a more complex phenomenon. We marked this variable as missing if there was no information about both parents. Immigration background has a high heterogeneity between countries, ranging from slightly over 0% in several countries to as high as 63% in Macao (see Table 1). Six countries (China, Korea, Peru, Poland, Romania, and Vietnam) have fewer than 30 cases with immigrant backgrounds. The efficiency of the estimation of achievement gaps based on immigration is reduced. However, excluding these cases does not affect the results of this study (see Results below).

We calculate the “raw” association between immigration and achievement scores. An alternative would be to estimate the achievement gap, controlling first for student SES. However, we want to highlight how immigration has different connotations between countries, as shown in Fig. 1. In addition, the association between immigration background and SES tends to be small (even non-significant) in several countries, and in different directions, as shown in Appendix (Table 3). While many countries (e.g., Western European countries) show a positive association between being a native student and having parents with university education, in many others (e.g., South American and Middle-East countries) the correlation is negative. Moreover, the between-countries correlation of the correlation of University-Native with Native-immigrant achievement gap is only r = 0.21 (p < 0.1). Therefore, we consider it appropriate to study the immigrant achievement gap fully detached from its interaction with the student SES.

Fig. 1
figure 1

SES, Immigrant, and Gender achievement gaps. Confidence intervals at 95% confidence level. Y axis are on different scale between each plot. Available as table format in Appendix (Table 3)

4.3.1.3 Gender Achievement Gap

To measure gender, we use the variable available on the PISA student dataset. The proportion of girls is mostly balanced across countries, ranging from 47 to 53% (see Table 1).

4.3.2 Country-Level Covariates

To validate the empirical differentiability of the three types of inequality, we use some key correlates of student achievement and social inequality in achievement commonly used in previous studies. This section is not intended to bring substantive conclusions, but to complement the previous analyses; if the regression models differ across the different types of social inequality, it brings some evidence on how this discussion have consequences on substantive educational research too. Some information is derived from the PISA school principal questionnaire, which is also representative of each country’s school system, while other variables are derived from external sources.

4.3.2.1 GDP per Capita

To indicate a country’s economic wealth, we used gross domestic product (GDP) per capita. This information is based on the World Bank database (World Bank, 2022), and we used the latest information for each country or region (up to 2018).

4.3.2.2 Growth Mindset

To capture cultural differences across countries, we used the variable growth mindset, available on the PISA 2018 student dataset. ‘Growth mindset’ refers to the belief that someone’s ability and intelligence can be developed over time (OECD, 2019c). Within each country, we averaged the percentage of students that strongly disagree or disagree with the statement “Your intelligence is something about you that you can’t change very much”.

4.3.2.3 Between-school Tracking Age

This variable indicates at what age (based on the modal age for the corresponding grade) students are placed into different school tracks. Different tracks typically have different curricula, and the transition from a comprehensive to a tracked school system constitutes an important event in students’ educational careers. We followed the information indicated in Strello et al., (2021), complemented by our own elaboration based on UNESCO-IBE’s World Data on Education (UNESCO-IBE, 2012).

4.3.2.4 Selectiveness

Besides tracking, we also included two indicators of the degree of selectiveness within educational systems: the importance for school admission of (1) Students’ record of academic performance (including placement tests) and (2) Residence in a particular area. For each of these we calculate the percentage per country of school principals that declare they Always (vs. Sometimes or Never) consider these factors in school admissions. A larger percentage of the first item is an indicator of a more selective system, while a larger percentage of the latter item is an indicator of a less selective system.

4.3.2.5 Grade Repetition

We included the percentage of students that had repeated a grade in their school course, using information on the PISA 2018 student questionnaire aggregated to the country level.

5 Results

5.1 Social Achievement Gaps Across Countries

As a preliminary step, we describe the social achievement gaps by social category. SES achievement gaps consist of the mean difference between high SES and low SES in standardized mathematics achievement scores. Figure 1a shows that the vast majority of countries present a positive and significant SES achievement gap. The only exceptions are the Philippines and Kazakhstan with a negative gap; and Lebanon, Baku (AZ), Brunei Darussalam, and Albania with a non-significant gap. Among those with significant positive achievement gaps, there is a high variability on the magnitude of these gaps; Norway and Indonesia have an SES achievement gap of 0.14 SD, while Belarus and Vietnam have SES achievement gaps of 0.66 SD and 0.74 SD, respectively.

The immigration achievement gap is calculated as the mean score difference between native students (without an immigrant background) and students with an immigrant background. Immigration achievement gaps are present in most countries on where there is a positive gap (i.e., natives perform better than immigrants), although several countries present a negative gap (i.e., immigrants perform better than natives), and many others where the differences are non-significant (see Fig. 1b). The range of the magnitude of the immigration achievement gaps is also larger than for SES achievement gaps, from a negative gap of -0.8 SD in United Arab Emirates to around 1.50 SD in Indonesia. Moreover, the unbalanced shares of natives and immigrant subsamples imply large confidence intervals in some countries, so the estimations of gaps in this category are less efficient than on SES and gender.

The gender achievement gap is calculated as the mean score difference between boys and girls. A positive gap indicates a higher mean score for boys than girls. One contrast with both previous measures is the smaller range of achievement gaps overall, from a − 0.24 SD gap in Qatar to a 0.24 SD gap in Colombia (see Fig. 1c). While most countries present positive gaps (boys achieve better mathematics scores than girls), there are negative gaps in many countries, with girls achieving better mathematics scores than boys.

Before we explore the association between the three types of social inequality with the full sample of 76 countries, we take a closer look at individual countries. In Turkey, the SES achievement gap is very high, whereas the immigration and gender gaps are small compared to the other countries. In Italy, the gaps for SES, immigration, and gender are low, medium, and high, respectively. Such patterns suggest that social inequality must be understood multidimensionally, since certain types of inequality are typically higher than others within the same country. Accordingly, a single type of social inequality is insufficient to conclude that social inequality in performance is generally low or high in a country.

5.2 Correlation of SES, Immigration, and Gender Achievement Gaps Across Countries

The correlational analyses, including all 76 countries, confirm the need to distinguish between SES, immigration, and gender achievement gaps. The correlation between SES and immigration achievement gaps is r = − 0.13 (non-significant [n.s.]), between SES and gender gaps it is r = 0.24 (p < 0.05), and between immigration and gender gaps it is r = 0.18 (n.s.). Figure 2 plots the associations between the three performance gaps, with no evidence of non-linear relationships.

Fig. 2
figure 2figure 2

Correlation between achievement gaps. Horizontal and vertical lines represent the between-countries mean of axis Y and axis X achievement gaps respectively. Solid line represents the correlation using the full sample of countries (N = 76)

In countries with a small number of immigrants, the gap between immigrant and native students is measured with lower reliability (see the large confidence intervals for some countries in Fig. 2b). To address this issue, we restricted the sample to the n = 53 countries with more than 100 immigrant students.Footnote 2 The results of the correlation analyses with the restricted sample are qualitatively the same as with the total sample. The correlation between SES and native-immigrant achievement gap remains insignificant and the correlation estimate is even lower (r = 0.03; n.s.). The correlation between immigrant achievement gap and gender achievement gap is 0.31 (p < 0.05). There is a small correlation when removing countries with less precise estimations of the immigrant achievement gap; in this sample there is a small tendency of countries with advantages for native students over immigrants to also show advantages for boys over girls. The correlation between the SES achievement gap and gender achievement gap is lower and is statistically insignificant (r = 0.19; n.s.). The analyses in the following section use the full sample of countries.

5.3 Nomological Networks

In the previous section, we presented evidence that the three achievement gaps—by SES, immigration background, and gender—are largely uncorrelated. In this section, as validity analyses that complement the previous section, we explore how the different achievement gaps are associated with different country-level features. If institutional features are associated differently with each achievement gap, this would provide further evidence of the need to differentiate between these three types of inequality when analyzing social inequality. We do not aim to bring substantive conclusions, that would require more theoretical development and a more complex analysis design.

Table 2 shows the results from three regression analyses, where we regressed the three measures of social inequalities on the set of country-level institutional features of education systems. For easier interpretation of the results, we present standardized regression estimates. Models estimated variable-by-variable are available in Appendix, Tables 4, 5, 6. The comparison reveals that institutional characteristics better explain variation in the gender achievement gap (explaining 48% of the international variation) than in the immigrant achievement gap (32%) and the SES achievement gap (22%).

Table 2 OLS Models on Achievement Gaps

The main finding of the comparison of regression parameters is that institutional characteristics are differentially associated with different social achievement gaps, providing further evidence that a holistic evaluation of social inequality requires a consideration of different gaps. For example, the economic power of countries, measured as GDP per capita, is associated negatively with the immigrant achievement gap and the gender gap, but it is not associated with the SES achievement gap. The growth mindset cultural indicator is associated with the SES achievement gap and the gender achievement gap, but not with the immigration gap.

Regarding selectivity, using residence as a criteria for selection is negatively associated with SES and gender achievement gaps. Counterintuitively, selecting by performance is only associated with a reduction in the immigration gap. A later tracking is significantly negatively associated only with the immigration gap.

Education systems with a higher percentage of repeating students tend to show a lower SES gap and a higher gender gap.

6 Discussion

In this study, we aimed to explore the degree to which there is one umbrella concept of social inequality, or whether there are substantially different concepts of social inequalities. We explored the correlations between three different social inequalities in achievement: SES, immigration status, and gender. We also compared how different education system-level covariates are associated with each achievement gap.

We highlight several points. First, at least one category of achievement gaps can be observed in every country. Second, while SES gaps were observed in all but four countries (as well as their direction), the size and direction of the immigration and gender gaps vary across countries. In most countries, natives and boys have better mean performance in mathematics than immigrants and girls, but there are several countries where immigrants and girls have an advantage. The variation across countries in the achievement gap by immigration is clearly higher than in the SES and, especially, the gender achievement gap. Also, the share of immigrants is very low in many countries, making it both empirically difficult to study (due the lower efficiency of the estimations) and a less prominent problem in some regions of the world. These findings suggest that, while SES inequality appears to be an almost global phenomenon, immigration and gender are associated with educational disadvantages differently across different countries and regions. Based on these findings, we conclude that the institutional context and social practices in different countries play a role in shaping social inequality. In the following section, we support this interpretation with additional findings.

Second, there is hardly any correlation between the three achievement gaps. This means that one education system can be egalitarian in some category, but profoundly unequal in another. To properly assess how unequal or egalitarian education systems are, policy-makers, researchers, and other stakeholders need to consider and address different indicators of social inequality.

Third, this lack of correlation is also related to how we study these inequalities. Using the same sample of countries and the same covariates, we showed that each achievement gap is associated with a different set of institutional features. Researchers who aim to study the impact of institutional characteristics on social inequality from a holistic perspective are advised to consider different forms of social inequality. Conclusions from a study on one gap cannot be generalized to other gaps.

6.1 Limitations and Future Research

This article has some limitations. One set of limitations relate to the measures we considered in the present study. We have only considered three key social categories here—SES, immigration, and gender—but there are other important categories (such as religiosity and ethnicity). These categories are often not highlighted in international assessments, and more comprehensive data is required to explore the gaps associated with these categories. Another limitation concerns the indicators used to measure SES, immigration, and gender. For the sake of simplicity, we considered only one indicator per category; nevertheless we recognize that these indicators have limitations, as there are more ways of operationalizing both immigration and SES. Also, we have not explored intersectionality among the three categories. For example, boys with a migration background may be a particularly disadvantaged group. Such analyses are beyond the scope of this paper but appear important for further research.

It is important to mention that these results only refer to mathematics achievement. There are other cases where achievement gaps could be of different magnitude or different direction. For example, looking at the latest international reports of PIRLS (Mullis et al., 2017) and PISA (OECD, 2019a), girls score significantly higher than boys in most countries, while in no country boys score better than girls. We focused on the achievement gap in mathematics as it illustrated the best the differences between achievement gaps.

Another set of limitations relates to the analysis of the institutional covariates, and natural limitations to the samples in some countries. The analysis of the covariates is based on cross-sectional data, and for this reason we do not make causal inferences. However, such analyses of nomological networks provide useful evidence for the distinction of social inequalities. Furthermore, the results involving immigration gaps are particularly affected by small subsamples of immigrants in certain countries where immigration is uncommon, lowering the measurement efficiency. Lastly, while PISA samples hundreds of thousands of students, the number of countries remains a natural limitation in any cross-national research.

6.2 Conclusion

In conclusion, mind the gap, but consider what gap you are looking at, as not all gaps are equal; depending on the social category, the results are very different. Ranking countries in terms of just one social inequality category provides a limited picture, at best. SES inequality is mostly a global problem, but immigration is more relevant in some regions than others, while gender gaps follow opposite direction between countries. This has direct consequences on the evaluation of education systems, and on research.