Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Argumentation-Based Collaborative Inquiry in Science Through Representational Work: Impact on Primary Students’ Representational Fluency

  • Published:
Research in Science Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study explored the impact of argumentation-promoting collaborative inquiry and representational work in science on primary students’ representational fluency. Two hundred sixty-six year 6 students received instruction on natural disasters with a focus on collaborative inquiry. Students in the Comparison condition received only this instruction. Students in the Explanation condition were also instructed with a focus on explanations using representations. Students in the Argumentation condition received similar instruction to the Comparison and Explanation conditions but were also instructed with a focus on argumentation using representations. Conceptual understanding and representational competencies (interpreting, explaining and constructing representations) were measured prior to and immediately following the instruction. A small group collaborative representational task was video recorded at the end of the instruction and coded for modes of knowledge-building discourse; knowledge-sharing and knowledge-construction. Higher measures of conceptual understanding, representational competencies and knowledge-construction discourse were taken together as representational fluency. Students in all conditions showed significant improvement in conceptual understanding, interpreting representations and explaining representations. Students in the Comparison and Argumentation conditions also showed significantly improved scores in constructing representations. When compared to the other conditions, the Explanation group had the highest scores in conceptual understanding and also interpreting and explaining representations. While the Argumentation group had the highest scores for constructing representations, their scores for conceptual understanding as well as interpreting and explaining representations were also high. There was no difference between the groups in knowledge-sharing discourse; however, the Argumentation group displayed the highest incidence of knowledge-construction discourse. The paper discusses how a collaborative inquiry instructional focus on explanation-building using representations fosters representational competencies, while a collaborative inquiry instructional focus on argumentation and explanation using representations promotes representational fluency.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ainsworth, S. (2006). DeFT: a conceptual framework for considering learning with multiple representations. Learning and Instruction, 16, 183–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, S. (2011, August 31). Understanding and transforming multi-representational learning. Keynote speech at the EARLI 2011 conference. Resource document. http://www.earli2011.org/nqcontent.cfm?a_id=489. Accessed 6 November 2013.

  • Australian Curriculum, Assessment and Reporting Authority. (2014). The Australian curriculum: science. Resource document. http://www.australiancurriculum.edu.au/science/curriculum/f-10?layout=1. Accessed 3 June, 2014.

  • Bereiter, C., & Scardmalia, M. (1993). Surpassing ourselves: an inquiry into the nature and implications of expertise. Chicago: Open Court.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berland, K. L., & Reiser, B. J. (2009). Making sense of argumentation and explanation. Science & Education, 93, 26–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchard, M. R., Southerland, S. A., Osborne, J. W., Sampson, V. D., Annetta, L. A., & Granger, E. M. (2010). Is inquiry possible in light of accountability: a quantitative comparison of the relative effectiveness of guided inquiry and verification laboratory instruction. Science & Education, 84, 577–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braaten, M., & Windschitl, M. (2011). Working toward a stronger conceptualization of scientific explanation for science education. Science & Education, 95(4), 639–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, R. W. (2006). Enhancing science teaching and student learning: a BSCS perspective. Boosting science learning: what it will take. ACER Research Conference. Review of Educational Research, 64, 1–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. B., & Sampson, V. D. (2007). Personally seeded discussions to scaffold online argumentation. International Journal of Science Education, 29(3), 253–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. (2011). Representational competence: a commentary on the Greeno analysis of classroom practice. In Theories of learning and studies of instructional practice (pp. 105–111). Springer New York.

  • DiSessa, A. A. (2004). Metarepresentation: native competence and targets for instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 22(3), 293–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duschl, R. A., & Osborne, J. (2002). Supporting and promoting argumentation discourse in science education. Studies in Science Education, 38, 39–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillies, G., Nichols, K., & Kahn, A. (2014). The effect of scientific representations on primary students’ development of scientific discourse and conceptual understanding during cooperative contemporary inquiry-science. Cambridge Journal of Education. doi:10.1080/0305764X.2014.988681.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hand, B., & Choi, A. (2010). Examining the impact of student use of multiple modal representations in constructing arguments in organic chemistry laboratory classes. Research in Science Education, 40(1), 29–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinze, S. R., Rapp, D. N., Williamson, V. M., Shultz, M. J., Deslongchamps, G., & Williamson, K. C. (2013). Beyond ball-and-stick: students’ processing of novel STEM visualizations. Learning and Instruction, 26, 12–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubber, P., Tytler, R., & Haslam, F. (2010). Teaching and learning about force with a representational focus: pedagogy and teacher change. Research in Science Education, 40, 5–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jimenez-Aleixandre, M. P., & Erduran, S. (2007). Argumentation in science education: an overview. In S. Erduran & M. P. Jimenez-Aleixandre (Eds.), Argumentation in science education (pp. 3–27): Springer.

  • Kockelman, P. (2007). The relation between meaning, power, and knowledge. Current Anthropology, 48(3), 375–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kozma, R., & Russell, J. (2005). Students becoming chemists: developing representational competence. In J. K. Gilbert (Ed.), Visualization in science education, models and modelling in science education (Vol. 1, pp. 121–145). Netherlands: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lemke, J. L. (2004). The literacies of science. Resource document http://jaylemke.squarespace.com/storage/Literacies-of-science-2004.pdf Accessed 20 June 2007.

  • Mason, L., & Santi, M. (1994). Argumentation structure and metacognition in constructing shared knowledge at school. New Orleans: A paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathai, S., & Ramadas, J. (2009). Visuals and visualisation of human body systems. International Journal of Science Education, 31(3), 439–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, N., Dawes, L., Wegerif, R., & Sams, C. (2004). Reasoning as a scientist: ways of helping children to use language to learn science. British Educational Research Journal, 30, 359–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, W. S. (2002). Understanding learning in a postmodern world: reconsidering the Perry scheme of intellectual and ethical development. In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: the psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 17–36). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, K., Hanan, J., & Ranasinghe, M. (2013a). Transforming the social practices of learning with representations: a study on disciplinary discourse. Research in Science Education, 43(1), 179–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, K., Ranasinghe, M., & Hanan, J. (2013b). Translating between representations in a social context: a study of undergraduate science students’ representational fluency. Instructional Science, 41(4), 699–728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nistal, A. A., Van Dooren, W., Clarebout, G., Elen, J., & Verschaffel, L. (2009). Conceptualising, investigating and stimulating representational flexibility in mathematical problem solving and learning: a critical review. ZDM, 41, 627–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osborne, J., & Patterson, A. (2011). Scientific argument and explanation: a necessary distinction? Science & Education, 95(4), 627–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osborne, J., Erduran, S., & Simon, S. (2004). Enhancing the quality of argumentation in science classrooms. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41(10), 994–1020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pea, R. D. (1994). Seeing what we build together: distributed multimedia learning environments for transformative communities. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 3, 285–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rau, M. A., Aleven, V., & Rummel, N. (2013). Interleaved practice in multi-dimensional learning tasks: which dimension should we interleave? Learning and Instruction, 23, 98–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, A. R. (2003). Argument and authority in the visual representations of science. Technical Communication Quarterly, 12(3), 183–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, S., Erduran, S., & Osborne, J. (2006). Learning to teach argumentation: research and development in the science classroom. International Journal of Science Education, 28(2–3), 235–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tytler, R., Prain, V., & Peterson, S. (2007). Representational issues in students learning about evaporation. Research in Science Education, 37, 313–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Aalst, J. (2009). Distinguishing knowledge-sharing, knowledge construction, and knowledge creation discourses. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 4, 259–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waldrip, B. & Prain, V. (2013). Teachers’ initial response to a representational focus. In R. Tytler, V. Prain, P. Hubber, & B. Waldrip (Eds.), Constructing representations to learn in Science, pp 15–30. Sense Publisher.

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by the Australian Research Council. We would like to thank Dr. Michael Boyle and Dr. Muditha Ranasinghe for their technical support in the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kim Nichols.

Appendix

Appendix

Pre/post-test for conceptual understanding and representational competencies (interpreting, explaining, creating representations)

figure afigure afigure a

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nichols, K., Gillies, R. & Hedberg, J. Argumentation-Based Collaborative Inquiry in Science Through Representational Work: Impact on Primary Students’ Representational Fluency. Res Sci Educ 46, 343–364 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-014-9456-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-014-9456-4

Keywords

Navigation