Introduction

With increasing age, the susceptibility to age-associated cognitive impairment increases [1]. Concerningly, the demographic profile of the world population is shifting towards older age with improved life expectancy. As of 2020, older Australians (aged ≥ 65 years) comprised 16% of the total Australian population and is projected to increase to 20.7% by 2066 [2]. This unprecedented growth of population ageing presents significant challenges relating to healthcare, independence, social, and community interaction among the elderly.

Ageing is associated with cognitive impairment ranging from mild impairment to dementia (severe impairment). Such impairment is a major cause of dependency and disability in the elderly. The age at which cognitive abilities decline is subject to debate [3,4,5,6]. However, longitudinal data has shown cognitive decline is evident at all ages between 45 and 70 years, with an accelerated decline in the oldest age groups [7]. Amongst older Australians (aged ≥ 65 years), the estimated prevalence of cognitive impairment ranges from 7.7 to 33.3% [8, 9]. Globally, the prevalence of cognitive impairment in those aged ≥ 50 years, ranges from 5.1 to 41.0%, with a median prevalence of 19.0% [10]. It is estimated that the prevalence of severe cognitive impairment will be 82 million in 2030, and will rise to 152 million by 2050 [11, 12].

There is well-established literature on the vital role that cognitive ability occupies in an individual’s daily functioning. Cognitive ability is defined as “the skills involved in performing the tasks associated with perception, learning, memory, understanding, awareness, reasoning, judgment, intuition, and language” [13]. The degree of cognitive impairment may range from mild deficits in memory, language, executive functioning, or visuospatial capabilities [14] to clinically significant deficits associated with other pathologies, such as dementia and Alzheimer’s disease [15, 16]. Given the complex multitude of influences on cognitive impairment, a multi-dimensional measure of health-related outcomes is needed to assess health and well-being.

HRQoL is a multi-dimensional construct that encompasses physical, mental, emotional, and social functioning [17]. It provides a broad summary of overall health status by incorporating elements that have been shown to affect health. Self-reported health outcomes can be measured accurately using generic non-preference and preference-based quality of life instruments, allowing for meaningful comparisons between healthy and clinical populations [18]. However, it is argued that condition-specific preference-based measurements may be more successful than a generic preference-based measure of HRQoL because they are more responsive to a particular health condition [19].

The association between cognitive impairment and HRQoL has been previously investigated, yielding conflicting findings. Notably, a number of these studies were undertaken in specific age groups and settings. Cognitive impairment has been associated with lower HRQoL in the Chinese (utilised EQ-5D) [20], Swedish (utilised EQ-5D) [21], and Turkish (utilised CDC HRQOL-4) [22] elderly. Other studies have shown similar findings in older adults with ailments, such as Alzheimer’s disease (utilised Quality of Life–Alzheimer’s Disease Scale) [23], dementia (utilised Qualidem) [24], and neurological disease [25]. Conversely, other evidence suggests that HRQoL is not impacted by cognitive impairment in nursing home residents (utilised Nursing Home Vision-Targeted Health-Related Quality of Life Questionnaire, VF-14, and the SF-36) [26], dementia patients [27] or institutionalized older Canadians (utilised EQ-5D-3L) [28]. One study of older Belgian adults found that lower HRQoL (using Alzheimer's Disease Related Quality of Life) was associated with dementia, rather than mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [29]. These inconsistent findings may be attributed to the subjective nature of HRQoL, instruments used, statistical methods and settings. Thus, a reliable comparison of findings across studies is challenging. Further, research on the complex interaction between degrees of cognitive impairment and HRQoL and how it evolves over time is needed across a broader spectrum of older age. This calls for further longitudinal research to elucidate how cognitive impairment impacts HRQoL over time.

The unprecedented proportional population growth of the elderly, in conjunction with the negative association between cognitive impairment and HRQoL, will pose significant logistical and financial healthcare challenges in the future. It is thus paramount that further research be conducted into developing medical treatments and funding preventative public health measures to address the cognitive decline. The current study will examine the association between cognitive impairment and HRQoL among older Australians (≥ 50 years old) using longitudinal data from the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics (HILDA) survey. Understanding the association between cognitive impairment and HRQoL may assist in identifying effective measures to support healthy ageing. It may also serve to optimize how resources are distributed into conserving and/or relieving cognitive impairment symptoms and subsequently, improving and maintaining HRQoL amongst the elderly.

Methods

Data and sample selection

This study sourced data from the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) survey. The survey collects annual data from a nationally representative cohort of Australian adults and has been running since 2001. It follows over 17,000 Australian adults over the course of their lifetimes and collects information on a variety of aspects of daily life, including but not limited to: household and family dynamics, labour and income statistics, education levels, and health outcomes. A more thorough guide for understanding HILDA data and documentation can be found elsewhere [30].

The data sourced for this study were extracted from wave 12 (2012) and wave 16 (2016), as these were the only waves where the survey collected information on cognitive impairment. Wave 12 was considered the baseline and wave 16 was considered the follow-up. This study was limited to older Australians, so only observations from those aged 50 years or above were included. Participants with missing data on the exposure variables (cognitive function test outcomes) and the outcome variables (HRQoL outcomes) were excluded. This resulted in a dataset of 10,737 person-year observations from 6,892 unique individuals. The sample selection, including exclusion criteria and breakdown of missing observations, is outlined in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Participants flow into the analytic sample and missing data

Dependent variables

The HILDA survey records HRQoL data through RAND Corporation’s 36-Item Short Form Survey (SF-36). This questionnaire consists of 36 generic and easily administered questions that span 4 physical dimensions and 4 mental dimensions of health [31]. Participant responses are scored from 0 to 100 to represent the worst and best health status for each specific dimension of health. The values of the 8 dimensions of health are then crystallized into two summary component scores: the physical component summary (PCS) score and the mental component summary (MCS) score. The PCS and MCS scores were calculated using the recommended scoring algorithm for Australians [32] and standardized using a linear z-score transformation with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation (SD) of 10. The final PCS and MCS scores in this analysis ranged from 6.04 to 72.14 and 4.36 to 72.79, respectively.

Notably, while the SF-36 is a fairly comprehensive measure of health status, it does not consider utility, which is the measure of preference that an individual imparts onto a particular health state. It equally values each health dimension regardless of how any individual HILDA participant may prioritize aspects of their health. To rectify this, HILDA also incorporates the SF-6D, a derivative of the SF-36 that generate utilities and thus is more econometrically valuable [33]. The SF-6D ranges from 0.29 to 1, where 1 represents full health and 0.29 indicates worst health state.

Predicting variable

Cognitive ability amongst participants is reported by HILDA through the use of previously established cognitive function tests; they are easily administered and thus are compatible with the face-to-face survey design employed by HILDA. This study uses the Backwards Digit Span test (BDS) and the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) as the relevant markers for cognitive function. These particular tests have also seen previous use in identifying cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis patients [34, 35] and acutely hospitalized patients [36]. The BDS is a test wherein the participant is given a string of digits and is asked to repeat them backwards [37]. The BDS assesses working memory and is scored from 0 to 8. The SDMT is a test where the participant pairs specific numbers with random geometric figures [38]. The SDMT assesses central processing and is scored from 0 to 110.

For this study, previously developed criteria were used to determine the threshold for cognitive impairment: participants who score ≥ 1.0 standard deviations (SD) below the mean on either the BDS or SDMT or both are considered to have MCI, and participants who score ≥ 1.5 SD below the mean for both tests are considered to have severe cognitive impairment [39]. This resulted in the cut-off for the BDS being ≤ 3, and the cut-off for SDMT being ≤ 30, such that any participant scoring at or below the cut-off for either test is considered mildly cognitively impaired. Any participant scoring ≤ 2 on the BDS and ≤ 24 on the SDMT tests will be considered to have severe cognitive impairment.

Control variables

A set of socio-demographic and health-related characteristics, along with health-related behaviours, were considered as potential confounders for this study. The variables include age, gender, relationship status, highest education level completed, household annual disposable income, labour force status, Indigenous status, region of residence, Body Mass Index (BMI), disability, smoking status, alcohol consumption, and physical activity. A categorical breakdown of these variables is found in Table 1.

Table 1 Control variables list and description

Analytic strategy

We constructed an unbalanced longitudinal data set consisting of 10,737 person-year observations from 6,892 unique individuals. The subsequent statistical analysis reported the descriptive statistics using mean and standard deviation (SD) for continuous variables, and percentages for categorical variables. The dependent variables of this study are continuous and we fit a longitudinal random-effects regression model to explain the relationship between cognitive impairment and HRQoL. The use of random-effects regression will help to identify the between-person differences in the association between cognitive impairment and HRQoL. The functional form of the panel random-effects regression is as follows.

$${\text{HRQoL}}_{it} \, = \,\alpha \, + \,\beta_{1} C_{it} \, + \,\beta X^{\prime}_{it} \, + \,\mu_{i} \, + \,\varepsilon_{it}$$
(1)

\(\mathrm{HRQoL}\) denotes the outcome variables: two summary measures (PCS and MCS), and health utility index (SF-6D) derived from the SF-36 questionnaire. C indicates the main variable of interests (cognitive impairment). \(X^{\prime}_{it}\) refers to the vector of time invariant and time-varying control variables. \(\alpha\) is the model’s grand intercept, \(\beta\) is the model parameter of interests to be estimated, and β′ indicates vector of co-efficient. \(\upmu\) represents individual-specific component that are constant over time and \(\upvarepsilon\)is the time and person-specific error term that is presumed to have no correlation with the regressors. The subscripts i and t in Eq. 1 represent individual and time, respectively.

All models are adjusted by age, gender, relationship status, highest education level completed, household annual disposable income, labour force status, Indigenous status, region of residence, BMI, disability status, smoking status, alcohol consumption, and physical activity. A p-value < 0.05 was considered significant, with p-values of < 0.01 and < 0.001 also reported for greater evidence of significance. The statistical analysis was performed using Stata version 17.0 (Stata SE 17, College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC, USA).

Results

Descriptive analyses

Table 2 summarizes the study population’s socio-demographic, health-related characteristics, and health-related behaviours in the baseline wave (2012), final wave (2016), and pooled waves (2012 to 2016). The pooled data showed that 41% of participants were over 65 years old, slightly more than half were female (53%), almost two-thirds were in a relationship (65%), almost a quarter had completed a university degree (24%), slightly more than half were unemployed or retired (52%), nearly 2% identified Indigenous, 36% resided in a regional or remote area, 29% were obese, 42% have some form of disability, 13% were current smokers, 81% were current drinkers, and one-third of the respondent performed the recommended level of physical activity (33%).

Table 2 Distribution of the analytic sample (socio-demographic and health-related characteristics): Baseline, final wave, and pooled data

Table 3 summarizes the distribution of the exposure variable (BDS and SDMT scores, cognitive impairment level) and outcome variables (SF-36 domain scores, PCS score, MCS score, SF-6D score) across the baseline, final, and pooled across all waves. The outcome measures for the pooled sample are as follows: the mean PCS score was 44.63 (SD = 11.30), the mean MCS score was 50.51 (SD = 10.10), the mean SF-6D score was 0.74 (SD = 0.13). The distribution for the exposure measures in the pooled samples are as follows: the mean BDS score was 4.83 (SD = 1.40), the mean SDMT score was 42.63 (SD = 11.88), 89% were not cognitively impaired, 10.16% were moderately cognitively impaired, 0.72% were severely cognitively impaired.

Table 3 Distribution of subjective health scores, test scores, and cognitive impairment: baseline, final wave and pooled data

The distribution of the primary outcome measures for the study population is shown in Fig. 2. The PCS scores ranged from 6.0 to 72.1, and the MCS scores ranged from 4.4 to 72.8. Majority of participants had PCS and MCS scores between 40 and 60, with a smaller percentage scoring below 40 and above 60, indicating a right-skewed distribution. The SF-6D utility scores are right skewed similarly to the PCS and MCS scores. The SF-6D scores range from 0.30 to 1, with majority of participants scoring between 0.75 and 1.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Distribution of PCS and MCS scores and SF-6D utility values

Figure 3 shows the trend of PCS, MCS, and SF-6D scores of the study population over the 4-year observation period between 2012 and 2016, stratified by level of cognitive impairment. The figure shows that the PCS (Panel A), and SF-6D (Panel C) scores for those who were moderately or severely cognitively impaired were much lower than those with no cognitive impairment in all waves. For instance, the mean PCS scores of adults with no cognitive impairment, moderate cognitive impairment and severe cognitive impairment are 45.51, 37.78, and 35.28, respectively in the final wave (wave 16). Figure 3 also demonstrates that the decline in SF-6D is more pronounced in those who are moderately (0.670) and severely (0.666) cognitively impaired compared to those who were not cognitively impaired (0.745) in wave 16. We observed that the MCS scores for those who were moderately and severely cognitively impaired were lower than those with no cognitive impairment (Panel B). The mean MCS scores of adults with no cognitive impairment, moderate cognitive impairment and severe cognitive impairment are 50.56, 47.80, and 48.25, respectively in the final wave (wave 16).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean PCS, MCS, and SF-6D score by cognitive impairment

Regression models

Table 4 demonstrates the relationships between different levels of cognitive impairment and the HRQoL outcome measures (PCS, MCS, SF-6D) for this study. The regression coefficients of the cognitive impairment concerning the PCS, MCS, and SF-6D are displayed in models 1 to 3. Model 1 shows that PCS scores of those who were severely cognitively impaired scored over 3 unit points (β = − 3.560) lower than normal, while PCS scores of those with moderate cognitive impairment scored almost 2 unit points (β = − 1.765) lower than normal. Model 2 shows that MCS scores of those who were severely cognitively impaired scored over 1 unit point (β = − 1.759) lower than normal, but could not significantly demonstrate this. Conversely, Model 2 demonstrates a significant decrease in MCS scores compared to normal for those who are moderately cognitively impaired, showing a decrease of almost 2 unit points (β = − 1.612) lower than normal. Finally, model 3 shows that SF-6D utility scores of those who were severely cognitively impaired scored over 0.03 unit points (β = − 0.034) lower than normal, while SF-6D utility scores of those with moderate cognitive impairment scored over 0.02 unit points (β = − 0.024) lower than normal.

Table 4 The association between cognitive impairment and the SF–36 component summary scores and SF–6D utility score, a longitudinal random-effects analysis

Discussion

This study examined the association between cognitive impairment with HRQoL in a sample of older Australians, using data from the HILDA survey. The findings indicated that majority of older Australians do not have cognitive impairment (89%). However, 10% and 1% had moderate and severe cognitive impairment, respectively. Similar levels of cognitive impairment amongst older Australians (aged ≥ 65 years) have been documented elsewhere [8, 9].

We measured HRQoL by the generic non-preference based measure (SF-36) and generic preference based measure (SF-6D). Generic tools have been criticised for not being sensitive for cognition. However, our results indicate that possibly SF-36 is a suitable tool for measuring change in quality of life related to cognitive status. The study showed strong evidence of an association between moderate and severe cognitive impairment with lower HRQoL concerning physical function (PCS) and the SF-6D. With regards to mental function (MCS), there was a strong association between moderate cognitive impairment and lower HRQoL after adjusting for confounding factors. A similar association between cognitive impairment and low HRQoL has been observed in older community-based participants from Australia [40], the United States [40], Japan [41], China [20] and Sweden [21]. Cognitive impairment has been associated with lower ratings of physical and mental components of HRQoL [20, 21]. Amongst community-dwelling older people, a 10-unit higher PCS and MCS score was associated with a 12% and 6% decreased risk of cognitive impairment, respectively [40]. Alternatively, no association was found between cognitive impairment and HRQoL in a community-dwelling sample of older adults [28]. However, this was attributed to the use of EuroQOL (EQ-5D) to measure HRQoL which does not contain a domain related to cognition, and thus may not be well associated with the cognitive impairment screening measure (i.e., Montreal Cognitive Assessment) used in the study [42, 43]. This issue has been previously debated [44]. Furthermore, as demonstrated in this study, severe cognitive impairment was associated with lower HRQoL scores compared to mild cognitive impairment in other studies [21, 29]. The concordance in community-based findings across various samples and this study’s findings add weight to the association between cognitive impairment and HRQoL.

Additionally, this study revealed differing strengths in associations between the mental and physical components of HRQoL with cognitive impairment. MCS and PCS scores assess unique aspects of self-reported HRQoL, which may account for differing associations with cognitive impairment. The current study illustrated that cognitive impairment was negatively associated with PCS and MCS, albeit the negative association between severe cognitive impairment and lower MCS scores was not statistically significant. In contrast, previous research on healthy older adults have shown differing results on the mental and physical components of HRQoL. A cross-sectional study on healthy older adults (aged 55 and older) showed a negative association between the physical component of HRQoL with age but a positive association between the mental component of HRQoL with age [45]. This is in line with theoretical views that older adults may maintain mental well-being despite objective health losses given that they possess self-regulatory mechanisms [46].

A plausible reason for the association between cognitive impairment and low HRQoL is the perception of poor health. The deterioration of HRQoL amongst the cognitively impaired elderly has been primarily attributed to loss of autonomy, which affects the ability to complete basic daily tasks independently [47, 48]. Cognitively impaired individuals may experience frustrations from the inability to complete basic daily tasks independently [48]. Therefore, individuals may experience heightened depression, anxiety, and dysfunctional social interactions [49], which may further deteriorate HRQoL. Prior studies have highlighted that older adult with cognitive impairment has a higher likelihood of reporting problems in HRQoL dimensions such as pain/discomfort, and anxiety/depression [20, 50]. Thus, it has been argued that an awareness of cognitive impairment may lead to distress as a reaction to declining cognition in elderly adults [50]. Independent of the severity of cognitive impairment, older adults aware of a mild cognitive impairment diagnosis and its prognosis had lower HRQoL, and more difficulties with daily functioning than those who were unaware [51]. Conversely, if individuals with cognitive impairment do not perceive their functioning as impaired, they may report a non-deteriorated HRQoL. Given that individuals assess their HRQoL by comparing their expectations and experience, perceived functional limitations may lower HRQoL [52]. Therefore, promoting practical coping strategies could improve HRQoL among cognitively impaired older adults. However, as cognitive impairment is associated with functional decline and multiple morbidities [53], it may curtail compensatory mechanisms amongst cognitively impaired elderly to maintain good mental well-being in contrast to their healthier, community-dwelling counterparts.

This study adds to the existing evidence that cognitive impairment is associated with low HRQoL in older adults. The findings highlight the importance of sustaining the cognitive ability to improve HRQoL in healthy ageing, particularly for the physical component of HRQoL. This presents a public health opportunity for policymakers to design and implement strategies that will maintain cognitive functioning or relieve symptoms associated with cognitive decline. It is hoped that such efforts may meaningfully improve HRQoL and potentially prevent individuals from declining further into severe cognitive impairment seen in dementia.

As cognitive decline is associated with functional impairments, this may pose difficulties in fulfilling basic needs, thus leading to poor physical and mental health. Specifically, assisting cognitively impaired older adults with feelings of loneliness, pain and improving the ability to undertake basic activities of daily living may have important implications for mental and physical HRQoL [54,55,56]. Therefore, promoting purposeful care to address a broad range of modifiable risk factors and encouraging protective factors may be key. These include social engagement, cognitive and physical activity and may serve as an effective strategy to maintain HRQoL with advancing age [57,58,59]. Besides, there is evidence that a patient empowerment model that considers the patient as the prime member of the health team and care managers who provide services to patients suggested by physicians in the primary health care system is beneficial for improving health outcomes for patients with heart failure and diabetes [60]. Hence, introducing a patient empowerment model might be helpful to reduce cognitive impairment among older Australians. Moreover, the literature has highlighted the importance of preventative measures in sustaining cognitive function. Recent evidence illustrated that 21.7% of MCI cases that deteriorated in dementia may have been preventable by targeting diet (8.7%), diabetes (1.5%) and neuropsychiatric symptoms (11.5%) [61]. Furthermore, specific lifestyle factors such as engaging in social and artistic activities during midlife and late were protective against cognitive impairment at ages 85–89 [62]. This evidence reinforces the need for further research to broadly understand health status across multiple domains, and formulate preventative measures against cognitive deterioration. Thus, by understanding the overall impact of cognitive impairment on HRQoL, cognitive measures may be utilised as a pragmatic tool to identify those who may benefit from such preventative strategies.

A key contribution of this study is the suggested cut-off scores to define cognitive impairment. To the authors’ knowledge, there is limited literature on appropriate cut-off scores that define cognitive impairment using the BDS and SDMT in older adults. BDS cut-off scores ≤ 4 have been used as a threshold to define cognitive impairment in older adults [36, 63, 64]. Previous research has suggested SDMT cut-off scores between 24 and 40 to define cognitive impairment. However, such research was based on highly specific groups, particularly, those with multiple sclerosis [34, 65,66,67]. The generated cut-off scores used in the current study generally align with previous literature discussed previously. Nevertheless, the current study suggested a SDMT cut-off score of ≤ 30, which lies in the range suggested by previous studies as discussed. However, the BDS cut-off score of ≤ 3 was more conservative than previous literature, and thus may be less sensitive in capturing milder cognitive impairment. Nevertheless, it is hoped that the suggested cut-off scores for cognitive impairment in the current study may contribute to the limited body of literature on optimal cut-off scores for defining cognitive impairment in the elderly. Much of the previous literature has focused on highly specific samples, yielding inconsistent findings on the association between cognitive impairment and HRQoL. This may be attributed to the heterogeneity in study samples, settings, and methodology. Furthermore, to the authors’ knowledge, majority of studies assessing the association between cognitive impairment and HRQoL have been cross-sectional in nature. Thus, no causative association could be firmly established. By undertaking this longitudinal study in a nationally representative cohort of older Australians, findings on the impact of cognitive impairment on HRQoL may be more generalisable to the normal ageing process and may be applied to future research and policy endeavours.

Strength and weaknesses

A major strength of this study was its large, population-based longitudinal nature, covering a wide spectrum of older age (50 years and older). This study utilised 10,737 person-year observations from 6892 unique individuals, using two waves of the HILDA survey to examine the association between cognitive impairment and HRQoL amongst older Australians. Further, a variety of factors were controlled for, and findings remained robust with adjustment. However, given the design of the study, unmeasured and unknown confounders could exist and impact the results.

The cognitive impairment measures (SDMT and BDS) are also validated and have good utility in reflecting core constructs of cognitive ageing and impairment. The use of validated tools facilitates the comparison of this study’s findings with previous research. A notable limitation of this study was the data collection methods for HRQoL. As HRQoL was self-reported, there is a risk of social desirability bias, leading to an inflation of HRQoL scores. Secondly, there is no consensus on clear cut-off scores for the SDMT and BDS scales to define cognitive impairment. Therefore, these measures are not diagnostic of cognitive impairment and may not accurately capture a full spectrum of cognitive impairment that is clinically significant. Thirdly, chronic conditions and pharmacological treatment are two crucial confounding variables in the relationship between cognitive impairment and HRQoL. Due to the unavailability of chronic conditions and pharmacologic data, the authors can’t include these confounding factors in the multivariate regression. Therefore, the estimated co-efficient could be underestimated or overestimated.

Conclusion

This study examines the association between cognitive impairment and HRQoL in older Australian adults. We found that those deemed mildly and severely cognitively impaired had lower PCS, and SF-6D scores than those with no cognitive impairment. Our findings might be helpful for performing future economic evaluation aiming to reduce cognitive impairment as the results revealed the disutility associated with moderate and severe cognitive impairment. Given that this study found a strong association between mild cognitive impairment and the physical component, but not the mental component of HRQoL, further research may be needed to understand how these components may be differentially affected by cognitive impairment across a spectrum of old age. Such understanding may be leveraged to promote targeted interventions that prevent or alleviate symptoms of poor HRQoL in those with cognitive impairment. Along similar lines, future studies could systematically evaluate specific elements of cognitive functioning to better understand their relative impact on HRQoL. This may assist in identifying preventative strategies to improve specific cognitive functions to yield a greater degree of improvement in HRQoL as adults enter older age.