Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Televised Oral Arguments and Judicial Legitimacy: An Initial Assessment

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

This article has been updated

Abstract

What happens to the perceived legitimacy of appellate courts when they allow cameras into their courtrooms? We implemented two experiments that exposed people to real video clips from two courts. In the first experiment we varied the modality (video or audio), contentiousness (neutral or contentious), and camera angle (static or dynamic) of exchanges between an attorney and judge and then measured people’s views toward judicial legitimacy. We found that static angles do not appear to influence legitimacy but using dynamic angles might have a limited effect. Watching a neutral exchange might increase judicial legitimacy—compared to listening to that exchange—but watching a contentious exchange might decrease it. In a second experiment we examined whether the presence of judicial symbols interacts with these effects. Evidence here is suggestive that these symbols could mitigate the negative effect of exposure to contentious content. Our results, though initial and limited in a number of ways, underscore both the complicated nature of cameras in the courtroom as well as the strong need for additional studies on a topic of great importance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Change history

  • 23 April 2023

    The orcid of coauthor Justin Wedeking has been updated in the article.

Notes

  1. A number of other studies find a connection between legitimacy and things like partisan or ideological agreement with decisions (Bartels & Johnston, 2013, 2020; Zilis, 2018; Christenson & Glick, 2015; Nicholson & Hansford, 2014).

  2. To be sure, cameras involve more issues than legitimacy alone. Some have argued that cameras could enhance transparency or accountability. Here, we focus on legitimacy because it has received the most scholarly attention.

  3. Some worry that cameras would lead attorneys to grandstand (Kennedy, 1996). While that outcome is possible, Jansen et al. (2018) finds that people actually engage in “good behavior” in the presence of cameras.

  4. An alternative approach would be to focus on the practical change that could take place in a world of televised arguments. Under this view, the format shift would create media coverage in cases that currently does not take place due to the unengaging nature of audio. We account for this possibility in our experimental design by including a control group who did not see or hear any oral argument content at all.

  5. Crigler et al. (1994) found that subjects were most emotionally aroused in the audiovisual condition. And emotion has been associated with decreases in court legitimacy (Armaly, 2018a).

  6. Two subsequent studies build on these findings. Nielsen et al. (2020) show that symbols increase acquiescence for the Australian High Court’s rulings. Armaly (2018b) shows that symbols, when paired with a legal argument about the importance of filling the vacancy created by the unexpected death of Justice Antonin Scalia, resulted in an increase in institutional support for the Supreme Court.

  7. We fielded the survey after the Supreme Court announced it would hold teleconferenced oral arguments (April 13) but before it provided the specific details of how it would operate such proceedings (April 30).

  8. In all of our stimuli, both the attorneys and judges are white males, which holds constant race and gender. Varying both of these in future studies would be useful given existing work on, for example, legitimacy and race (Gibson & Nelson, 2018) as well as on gender and oral argument (Patton & Smith, 2017).

  9. See https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/contentious and https://www.dictionary.com/browse/contentious.

  10. Because we used real world materials, we could not manipulate only the tone or manner in which the exchanges took place while holding constant the actual words being spoken. Thus, there are differences in the stimuli aside from the tone and behavior of the justice. See the Supplementary Materials for additional discussion and details of these differences.

  11. As we note above, most appellate courts already record (and often livestream) audio of their proceedings, which means, as a policy matter, audio is a natural baseline (Morton & Williams, 2008). We still include a control group that lacks any stimuli at all (Gaines et al., 2007) as it allows us to evaluate how a change to video might impact individuals who would be newly exposed to oral argument content (presumably due to increased media coverage of the proceedings).

  12. As both of these states are Midwestern, this means we also held constant possible differences in the accent of the speakers—justices and attorneys alike. This is noteworthy since accents from some regions—particularly the north (e.g., New York) and the south—are perceived less favorably than others (Alford & Strother, 1990; Amira et al., 2018). Note, additionally, that none of the Minnesota speakers sounded like someone from the movie Fargo. Not that there’s anything wrong with that, ya know.

  13. As with any survey experiment, there was variation in the seriousness with which respondents completed the assigned tasks (Berinsky et al., 2014). The results we describe below include all respondents in the analysis. In the Supplementary Materials we present an alternative approach where we allow the effects to vary by level of engagement with our experiment. As one would expect, the effects are present among those who were engaged.

  14. Because we did not specifically identify either our courts or our judges, we needed to modify our question wording from those used in typical studies of the U.S. Supreme Court. We also crafted these statements to align with recent work arguing for a more “applied” approach to measuring legitimacy (Badas, 2019).

  15. This approach is substantively equivalent to simply conducting a series of t-tests to evaluate differences in our dependent variable. Additional results in the Supplementary Materials show that these results are unchanged if we add controls for respondent demographics. We also show that these results are substantively identical if we instead estimate an ordinal least squares linear regression model.

  16. Again, there are marginal differences between the control group and those who saw the contentious dynamic exchange (\(p = 0.08\), one-tailed). People who saw the contentious exchange were marginally less supportive of the court than people who saw nothing.

  17. We examined the effect of changing from neutral to contentious audio content since there are no camera angle differences in these materials. If it was video style (alone) causing our results, then the neutral-contentious content difference should be equal between the states. Increased contentiousness has a statistically significant increase in legitimacy for the Indiana audio content (\(p = 0.003\)) but has a slight yet statistically insignificant decrease for the Minnesota audio content (\(p=0.61\)). A Wald test of the difference between those differences (i.e., Indiana’s increase versus Minnesota’s null) allows us to reject the null that the two are equal (\(p = 0.02\)). The videos therefore may have differences which lead to the results we observe.

  18. These results include all respondents who completed our experiment. See the Supplementary Materials for an analogous treatment of shirkers that we discuss above for the first experiment.

  19. Recall that our original experiment provided some evidence of an effect within the dynamic video condition. Specifically, a neutral exchange led to higher legitimacy scores than a contentious exchange (i.e., the left half of Fig. 4). Though our primary purpose in executing this second experiment was to assess the effect of symbols, we note that, within this independent sample, we failed to recover the same effect when comparing subjects who were in the non-symbols condition (i.e., the first and third points plotted in Fig. 5).

References

  • Alford, R. L., & Strother, J. B. (1990). Attitudes of native and nonnative speakers toward selected regional accents of U.S. English. TESOL Quarterly, 24(3), 479–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amira, K., Cooper, A., Knotts, H., & Wofford, C. (2018). Linguistic profiling in education: How accent bias denies equal educational opportunities to students of color. American Politics Research, 46(6), 1065–1093.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Armaly, M. T. (2018a). Extra-judicial actor induced change in Supreme Court legitimacy. Political Research Quarterly, 71(3), 600–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Armaly, M. T. (2018b). Politicized nominations and public attitudes toward the Supreme Court in the polarization era. Justice System Journal, 39(3), 193–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Austin, P. C., & Hux, J. E. (2002). A brief note on overlapping confidence intervals. Journal of Vascular Surgery, 36(1), 194–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Badas, A. (2019). The applied legitimacy index: A new approach to measuring judicial legitimacy. Social Science Quarterly, 100(5), 1848–1861.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, B. L., & Johnston, C. D. (2013). On the ideological foundations of Supreme Court legitimacy in the American public. American Journal of Political Science, 57(1), 184–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, B. L., & Johnston, C. D. (2020). Curbing the Court: Why the public constrains judicial independence. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Berinsky, A. J., Margolis, M. F., & Sances, M. W. (2014). Separating the shirkers from the workers? Making sure respondents pay attention on self-administered surveys. American Journal of Political Science, 58(3), 739–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryan, A. C., & Ringsmuth, E. M. (2016). Jeremiad or weapon of words? The strategic power of emotive language in Supreme Court dissents. Journal of Law and Courts, 4(1), 159–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caldeira, G. A., & Gibson, J. L. (1992). The etiology of public support for the Supreme Court. American Journal of Political Science, 36(3), 635–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christenson, D. P., & Glick, D. M. (2015). Chief justice Roberts’s health care decision disrobed: The microfoundations of the Supreme Court’s legitimacy. American Journal of Political Science, 59(2), 403–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coppock, A., & McClellan, O. A. (2019). Validating the demographic, political, psychological, and experimental results obtained from a new source of online survey respondents. Research and Politics, 6(1), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crigler, A. N., Just, M., & Neuman, W. R. (1994). Interpreting visual versus audio messages in television news. Journal of Communication, 44(4), 132–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • C-SPAN. (2011). Cameras in the court: Learn about the justice’s views on the issue of opening the court to cameras, based on their public statements. Retrieved November, 10, 2011, from http://www.c-span.org/The-Courts/Cameras-in-The-Court/

  • Cummins, R. G. (2009). The effects of subjective camera and fanship on viewers’ experience of presence and perception of play in sports telecasts. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 37(4), 374–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, R. G., Keene, J. R., & Nutting, B. H. (2012). The impact of subjective camera in sports on arousal and enjoyment. Mass Communication and Society, 15(1), 74–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2003). The power of television images: The first Kennedy–Nixon debate revisited. Journal of Politics, 65(2), 559–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fang, A. H., & Huber, G. A. (2019). Perceptions of deservingness and the politicization of social insurance: Evidence from disability insurance in the United States. American Politics Research, 48, 543–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frick, R. W. (1995). Accepting the null hypothesis. Memory and Cognition, 23, 132–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaines, B. J., Kuklinski, J. H., & Quirk, P. J. (2007). The logic of the survey experiment reexamined. Political Analysis, 15(1), 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GAO. (2016). U.S. Supreme Court: Policies and perspectives on video and audio coverage of appellate court proceedings. United States Government Accountability Office Report to Congressional Requesters. GAO.

  • Gibson, J. L. (2015). Legitimacy is for losers: The interconnections of institutional legitimacy, performance evaluations, and the symbols of judicial authority. In B. Bornstein & A. Tomkins (Eds.), Motivating cooperation and compliance with authority (pp. 81–116). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. L., & Caldeira, G. A. (2009a). Citizens, courts, and confirmations: Positivity theory and the judgments of the American people. Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. L., & Caldeira, G. A. (2009b). Knowing the Supreme Court? A reconsideration of public ignorance of the High Court. Journal of Politics, 71(2), 429–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. L., & Caldeira, G. A. (2011). Has legal realism damaged the legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court? Law and Society Review, 45(1), 195–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. L., & Caldeira, G. A. (2012). Campaign support, conflicts of interest, and judicial impartiality: Can recusals rescue the legitimacy of courts? Journal of Politics, 74(1), 18–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. L., Caldeira, G. A., & Baird, V. A. (1998). On the legitimacy of national high courts. American Political Science Review, 92(2), 343–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. L., Lodge, M., & Woodson, B. (2014). Losing, but accepting: Legitimacy, positivity theory, and the symbols of judicial authority. Law and Society Review, 48(4), 837–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. L., & Nelson, M. J. (2015). Is the U.S. Supreme Court’s legitimacy grounded in performance satisfaction and ideology? American Journal of Political Science, 59(1), 162–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. L., & Nelson, M. J. (2016). Change in institutional support for the U.S. Supreme Court: Is the court’s legitimacy imperiled by the decisions it makes? Public Opinion Quarterly, 80(3), 622–641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. L., & Nelson, M. J. (2018). Black and Blue: How African Americans judge the U.S. legal system. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jansen, A. M., Giebels, E., van Rompay, T. J., & Junger, M. (2018). The influence of the presentation of camera surveillance on cheating and pro-social behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(1937), 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, A. M., O’Connor, S. D., & Breyer, S. G. (2006). The role of the judiciary: Panel discussion with United States Supreme Court justices. Berkeley Journal of International Law, 25(1), 71–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, J. A. (1996). Testimony before House Committee on Appropriations.

  • Krewson, C. N. (2019). Save this honorable court: Shaping public perceptions of the Supreme Court off the bench. Political Research Quarterly, 72(3), 686–699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kromphardt, C. D., & Bolton, J. P. (2022). Ready for their close-up? Ideological cues and strategic televising in the Ninth Circuit Court of Appeals. Justice System Journal, 43(3), 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, A., Zhou, S., Schwartz, N., Bolls, P. D., & Potter, R. F. (2000). The effects of edits on arousal, attention, and memory for television messages: When an edit is an edit can an edit be too much? Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 44(1), 94–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombard, M., & Ditton, T. (1997). At the heart of it all: The concept of presence. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 3(2), JCMC321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, R. B., & Williams, K. C. (2008). Experimentation in political science. In The Oxford handbook of political methodology (pp. 339–356). Oxford University Press.

  • Mutz, D. C. (2011). Population-based survey experiments. Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mutz, D. C., & Reeves, B. (2005). The new videomalaise: Effects of televised incivility on political trust. American Political Science Review, 99(1), 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, M. J., & Gibson, J. L. (2020). Measuring subjective ideological disagreement with the US Supreme Court. Journal of Law and Courts, 8(1), 75–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, M. J., & Tucker, P. D. (2021). The stability and durability of the US Supreme Court’s legitimacy. Journal of Politics, 83(2), 767–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, S. P., & Hansford, T. G. (2014). Partisans in robes: Party cues and public acceptance of Supreme Court decisions. American Journal of Political Science, 58(3), 620–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, I., Robinson, Z., & Smyth, R. (2020). Keep your (horse) hair on? Experimental evidence on the effect of exposure to legitimising symbols on diffuse support for the High Court. Federal Law Review. https://doi.org/10.1177/0067205X20927818

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patton, D., & Smith, J. L. (2017). Lawyer, interrupted: Gender bias in oral arguments at the US Supreme Court. Journal of Law and Courts, 5(2), 337–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, T. E., Boomgaarden, H. G., Swert, K. D., & de Vresse, C. H. (2018). Video killed the news article? Comparing multimodal framing effects in news videos and articles. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 62(4), 578–596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, R. F., Detenber, B. H., Cuthbert, B. N., Schwartz, D. D., & Reiss, J. E. (2003). Attention to television: Alpha power and its relationship to image motion and emotional content. Media Psychology, 5(3), 283–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slotnick, E. E., & Segal, J. A. (1998). Television news and the Supreme Court: All the news that’s fit to air? Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Souter, J. D. (1996). Testimony before House Committee on Appropriations.

  • Sundar, S. S., Molina, M. D., & Cho, E. (2021). Seeing is believing: Is video modality more powerful in spreading fake news via online messaging apps? Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 26(6), 310–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R. (1989). The psychology of procedural justice: A test of the group-value model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(5), 830.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R. (2021). Why people obey the law. Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R., & Rasinski, K. (1991). Procedural justice, institutional legitimacy, and the acceptance of unpopular US Supreme Court decisions: A reply to Gibson. Law and Society Review, 25(3), 621–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, R. (2019). Cameras in the Supreme Court? Not anytime soon. USA Today.

  • Yadav, A., Phillips, M. M., Lundeberg, M. A., Koehler, M. J., Hilden, K., & Dirkin, K. H. (2011). If a picture is worth a thousand words is video worth a million? Differences in affective and cognitive processing of video and text cases. Journal of Computing in Higher Education, 23, 15–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zilis, M. (2015). The limits of legitimacy: Dissenting opinions, media coverage, and public responses to Supreme Court decisions. University of Michigan Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Zilis, M. A. (2018). Minority groups and judicial legitimacy: Group affect and the incentives for judicial responsiveness. Political Research Quarterly, 71(2), 270–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zilis, M. A. (2021). The rights paradox: How group attitudes shape US Supreme Court legitimacy. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

No funding was received for conducting this study. Additionally, all authors certify that they have no affiliations with or involvement in any organization or entity with any financial interest or non-financial interest in the subject matter or materials discussed in this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ryan C. Black.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

We thank Matt Cota and Jonathan King for feedback on earlier drafts. Replication files for the paper are available on Harvard Dataverse (https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/IUANCE).

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

(PDF 439 kb)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Black, R.C., Johnson, T.R., Owens, R.J. et al. Televised Oral Arguments and Judicial Legitimacy: An Initial Assessment. Polit Behav (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-022-09848-5

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-022-09848-5

Keywords

Navigation