Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Freediving neurophenomenology and skilled action: an investigation of brain, body, and behavior through breath

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper I investigate the neurophenomenology of freediving (NoF) and the Skilled Intentionality Framework (SIF), using these two components to mutually inform each other in order to better understand cognition in skilled action. First, this paper provides a novel neurophenomenological exposition of the practice of freediving. It combines quantitative neurophysiological data with qualitative phenomenological reports in order to understand the neural and bodily mechanisms that correlate with the phenomenology of freediving. The NoF data suggests that freediving induces a unique neurophysiological state. This unique neurophysiological state forms the basis for a peculiar and exceptional experiential state, which is phenomenologically characterized by a heightened sense of presence, heightened perception, lack of reflective awareness, lack of anticipation in decision-making, and restricted emotional range. Second, this paper synthesizes the NoF data and the SIF conceptual framework of cognition in skilled action in order to investigate how the two can mutually inform one another. This synthesis provides 1) a unified and cohesive understanding of the NoF data; 2) elucidation and clarification of three key features generalizable to SIF’s metastable zones; 3) refinement of the role of anticipation in SIF, with the focus shifting instead towards task-specific constraint of action-readiness; and 4) an investigation of the breath, an understudied dynamical oscillator of brain, body, and behavior, which provides an empirical mechanism to support SIF’s theoretical assumption of the dynamical self-organization required in skilled action. Looking more broadly, this neurophenomenological investigation of freediving elucidates a novel case study which can provide rich perspectives and fertile material for further scientific, phenomenological, theoretical, and philosophical investigations in ecological psychology, expertise, reflection, enactivism, and cognition more generally.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data and material

A list of interview questions asked of all study participants is available as Online Resource 1. An appendix of key interview excerpts, anonymized and sorted thematically, is available as Online Resource 2. Full interview transcripts are available upon request. Please send all inquiries and requests to s.luecke@berkeley.edu.

Code availability

Not applicable.

Notes

  1. Ideally, a rigorous neurophenomenological approach would employ synchronous collection of qualitative and quantitative datasets, rather than relying on one or more pre-existing datasets, so as to increase the robustness of correlations discovered. However, for the case of freediving, synchronously collecting these two datasets in an ecologically valid setting is near impossible due to current technological constraints: sophisticated neuroimaging devices cannot yet be taken underwater into the freediving environment. However, a wearable, waterproof, and pressure-proof near-infrared spectroscopy device – which can collect neurophysiological measurements such as cerebral haemodynamic responses, heart rate, cerebral and arterial blood oxygen responses, and temperature – is currently under development (McKnight et al., 2021). Preliminary results show its efficacy in measuring the neurophysiology of human freedivers in an ecologically valid field setting. Future work should utilize this device, in concert with phenomenological interviews, to increase the rigor and robustness of NoF.

  2. For methodology, see Høffding (2014), Høffding and Martiny (2016), and Ravn and Høffding (2017).

  3. As participation in this study relied on voluntary response sampling, it is important to consider the effects of non-response bias and how they might affect the data collected. I cannot here discuss these considerations in due detail, but point instead to Cheung et al. (2017), where such a discussion is produced.

  4. It is important to note that all study participants engaged only in modern Western forms of freediving. While the cultural specificity of this sample allows for greater ease of comparison across participants, it is thereby not representative of other cultural variations in freediving practices. The results of the current study are thus limited in scope, and are only applicable to modern Western freediving. Future investigations should explore other freediving cultures in order to determine the extent to which the neurophysiology and phenomenology that follows obtains across different freediving cultures and contexts.

  5. I cannot defend this assumption here, but instead point to Allen‐Collinson (2009) or Graneheim, Lindgren and Lundman (2017), where such a defense is produced.

  6. Online Resources 2.1 to 2.10 refer to key excerpts from the interview data, anonymized and sorted thematically.

  7. While freediving does induce a unique neurophysiological state, unable to be realistically reached otherwise, more research is needed to determine whether it also induces a unique experiential state. Current data suggests that freediving does induce a unique experiential state; however, similar experiential states may obtain in other practices with similar characteristics. Such practices could include intense meditative or mindfulness practices (Gamma & Metzinger, 2021; Travis & Pearson, 2000), or other extreme, water-based, and/or life-threatening practices, such as free solo climbing or big wave surfing (Ilundáin-Agurruza, 2015). More research is needed to discern whether the phenomenology that follows obtains uniquely for freediving.

  8. Future work should compare freediving versus advanced meditative states in more detail, in order to understand the nuanced similarities and differences in both neurophysiology and phenomenology between the two practices.

  9. Low-threshold pressure sensors critical to gentle touch and proprioception.

  10. A recent study by Karalis and Sirota (2022) may provide initial corroboration of some of these findings. The study shows that breathing acts as a ‘perennial oscillatory pacemaker’ of the brain, entraining and synchronizing both local and global neural circuit dynamics. The breath “acts as a functional oscillatory scaffold and provides a unifying global temporal coordination of neuronal firing and network dynamics,” which allows for the segregation and integration of information across distributed neuronal networks (Karalis & Sirota, 2022, p. 11). This breathing-induced oscillatory pacemaker activity may be implicated in memory consolidation, integration of exteroceptive and interoceptive inputs, fear behavior, and other cognitive processes.

  11. The case of freediving adds further convolutions to the Piezo2/ICP mechanism which need to be investigated. For example, how does the high external pressure of deep underwater environments affect ICP? The effects of hyperbaric conditions on ICP are still unknown and require further research (Mehrpour et al., 2014). What effects do prolonged breath-holds have on ICP? Progressive increased CO2 levels (hypercapnia), characteristic of prolonged breath-holds, can increase ICP due to cerebral vasodilation (Asgari et al., 2011). However, at depth, freedivers sometimes experience elevated O2 levels (hyperoxia), due to the increased pressure facilitating blood-oxygen uptake into the tissues (Bosco et al., 2018) – and hyperbaric oxygen is known to decrease ICP in mammals in certain situations (Miller et al., 1970). Freedivers may thus experience amplified ICP changes due to the volitional breathe-up practice, altered ICP changes due to hyperbaric conditions, increased ICP due to hypercapnia, and decreased ICP due to hyperoxia at depth, all within a single dive. There is simply not enough information available regarding the effects of breath-holds nor hyperbaric conditions on ICP, but these are important areas for future research in order to understand the proposed respiration-entrained mechanisms that may alter freedivers’ brain activity, behavior, and experience.

  12. I cannot here take a particular stance on the reflection literature (eg. Dreyfus, 2014; McDowell, 2007; Toner et al., 2016; Sutton et al., 2011; Høffding, 2014), due to a lack of adequate space. While the peculiarities of the current investigation are certainly relevant to this body of literature, and should be explored in further work, the purpose of the present subsection is to explore and portray freedivers’ phenomenological reports and the relevant neurophysiological data.

  13. This phenomenological description of decision-making in freediving seems, upon first glance, to be at odds with the concept of action automaticity, and its often inverse relationship to both the level of action flexibility and adaptability, and the level of complexity and difficulty of task conditions (see eg. Christensen et al., 2016; Ilundáin-Agurruza, 2015). Future work should explore the concept of automaticity further to determine how closely freedivers’ conceptions of automaticity align with theoretical notions of automaticity, and what the implications of this phenomenology might be for the relevant theoretical frameworks.

  14. Due to space constraints, the implications to be discussed in the present paper will focus primarily on the concept of anticipation as it is presented in the SIF literature. However, the peculiar experience (or apparent lack thereof) of anticipation in freediving should be explored further, by way of more detailed engagement with a broader literature on cognition in skilled action. Future work should apply NoF to other theoretical investigations which critically engage the concept of anticipation (eg. Christensen & Bicknell, 2019; Farrow & Abernethy, 2015).

  15. For any readers who are intrigued by the phenomenology of freediving and wish to hear more about the experience from the perspective of a world renowned freediving champion, I can recommend William Trubridge’s TEDx Talk on the matter, called “This is why I freedive: A Journey into the Deep,” where he beautifully and poetically describes what it feels like to freedive: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=-MZetpFw7qY

  16. While a full review of the relevant theoretical literature in relation to the NoF data is certainly worthwhile, it is not possible in the scope of the current paper. Future work should therefore use NoF to investigate other relevant theoretical frameworks, such as Christensen et al.’s ‘Mesh’ framework (2016).

  17. SIF authors occasionally write that anticipation need not always be explicitly experienced. In the absence of explicit goals or plans, relevant actions arise through “the skilled animal’s anticipatory dynamics, understood in terms of self-organizing states of action readiness” (Bruineberg et al., 2019, p. 5234). In these cases, anticipation takes on a different, more technical meaning, being understood as synonymous to SIF’s assumed theoretical process of action-readiness self-organization. However, this second definition of anticipation, unlike the first, fails to answer the key question of narrowing. It defines the general process of narrowing that SIF assumes must occur in all cases of skilled action (self-organization), but does not provide an answer to the question of what specifically is guiding that process in any given case of skilled action. As we can see, ‘anticipation’ becomes a slippery term: the technical conception (which refines the question of narrowing through describing the presumed general process of self-organization) has colloquial connotations that allow for conflation with more explicit conceptions of anticipation (which provide the answer to what specifically is doing the narrowing). More precision is needed within the SIF framework to avoid conflation between these two different conceptions of anticipation. For present purposes, we focus on explicit conceptions of anticipation, since we are interested in the answer to what specifically is doing the narrowing.

  18. This maintenance of viability conditions relates to Di Paolo et al.’s notion of agent normativity (2017).

  19. On neither its explicit nor its technical conception, since 1) explicit anticipation is not necessary for skilled action, and 2) technical anticipation, while necessary due to the fact that it defines the general process which is assumed by SIF to be driving all cases of skilled action, does not address the key question of what specifically is driving this assumed general process.

  20. Such as task difficulty, interdependence of task features, and whether or not a task is presupposed.

References

  • Aktas, G., Kollmeier, J. M., Joseph, A. A., Merboldt, K.-D., Ludwig, H.-C., Gärtner, J., Frahm, J., & Dreha-Kulaczewski, S. (2019). Spinal CSF flow in response to forced thoracic and abdominal respiration. Fluids and Barriers of the CNS, 16(1), 10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allen-Collinson, J. (2009). Sporting embodiment: Sports studies and the (continuing) promise of phenomenology. Qualitative Research in Sport and Exercise, 1(3), 279–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Annen, J., Panda, R., Martial, C., Piarulli, A., Nery, G., Sanz, L. R., Valdivia-Valdivia, J. M., Ledoux, D., Gosseries, O., & Laureys, S. (2021). Mapping the functional brain state of a world champion freediver in static dry apnea. Brain Structure and Function, 226(8), 2675–2688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asgari, S., Bergsneider, M., Hamilton, R., Vespa, P., & Hu, X. (2011). Consistent changes in intracranial pressure waveform morphology induced by acute hypercapnic cerebral vasodilatation. Neurocritical Care, 15(1), 55–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barry, R. J., Johnstone, S. J., & Clarke, A. R. (2003). A review of electrophysiology in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: II. Event-related potentials. Clinical Neurophysiology: Official Journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology, 114(2), 184–198.

  • Bilo, G., Revera, M., Bussotti, M., Bonacina, D., Styczkiewicz, K., Caldara, G., Giglio, A., Faini, A., Giuliano, A., Lombardi, C., Kawecka-Jaszcz, K., Mancia, G., Agostoni, P., & Parati, G. (2012). Effects of slow deep breathing at high altitude on oxygen saturation, pulmonary and systemic hemodynamics. PloS One, 7(11), e49074.

  • Bosco, G., Rizzato, A., Martani, L., Schiavo, S., Talamonti, E., Garetto, G., Paganini, M., Camporesi, E. M., & Moon, R. E. (2018). Arterial Blood Gas Analysis in Breath-Hold Divers at Depth. Frontiers in Physiology, 9, 1558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruineberg, J., Chemero, A., & Rietveld, E. (2019). General ecological information supports engagement with affordances for “higher”cognition. Synthese, 196(12), 5231–5251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruineberg, J., & Rietveld, E. (2014). Self-organization, free energy minimization, and optimal grip on a field of affordances. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruineberg, J., Seifert, L., Rietveld, E., & Kiverstein, J. (2021). Metastable attunement and real-life skilled behavior. Synthese, 199(5), 12819–12842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buzsaki, G. (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Casaburi, R., Spitzer, S., Haskell, R., & Wasserman, K. (1989). Effect of altering heart rate on oxygen uptake at exercise onset. Chest, 95(1), 6–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, K. L., Peter, M., Smit, C., de Vries, H., & Pieterse, M. E. (2017). The impact of non-response bias due to sampling in public health studies: A comparison of voluntary versus mandatory recruitment in a Dutch national survey on adolescent health. BMC Public Health, 17(1), 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, W., Sutton, J., & McIlwain, D. J. F. (2016). Cognition in Skilled Action: Meshed Control and the Varieties of Skill Experience: Cognition in Skilled Action. Mind & Language, 31(1), 37–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, W., & Bicknell, K. (2019). Affordances and the anticipatory control of action. In Handbook of embodied cognition and sport psychology (pp. 601–621). MIT Press.

  • Colombetti, G. (2011) Varieties of Pre-Reflective Self-Awareness: Foreground and Background Bodily Feelings in Emotion Experience. Inquiry: a journal of medical care organization, provision and financing, 54(3), 293–313.

  • Colombetti, G. (2014). The Feeling Body: Affective Science Meets the Enactive Mind. MIT Press.

  • Daubenmier, J., Sze, J., Kerr, C. E., Kemeny, M. E., & Mehling, W. (2013). Follow your breath: respiratory interoceptive accuracy in experienced meditators. Psychophysiology, 50(8), 777–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denton, D. (2006). The primordial emotions: The dawning of consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Di Paolo, E., Buhrmann, T., & Barandiaran, X. (2017) Sensorimotor Life: An enactive proposal. Oxford University Press.

  • Dotov, D., & Chemero, A. (2014). Breaking the Perception-Action Cycle: Experimental Phenomenology of Non-Sense and its Implications for Theories of Perception and Movement Science. In M. Cappuccio & T. Froese (Eds.), Enactive Cognition at the Edge of Sense-Making: Making Sense of Non-Sense (pp. 37–60). Palgrave Macmillan UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downey, G. (2022). Not breathing together: The collaborative development of expert apnoea. Collaborative Embodied Performance: Ecologies of Skill, 93.

  • Dreyfus, H. L. (2014). Skillful Coping: Essays on the phenomenology of everyday perception and action. OUP Oxford.

  • Dworkin, S. L. (2012). Sample size policy for qualitative studies using in-depth interviews. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 1319–1320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrow, D., & Abernethy, B. (2015). Expert anticipation and pattern perception. Routledge handbook of sport expertise, 9–21.

  • Fenn, W. O. (1928). The carbon dioxide dissociation curve of nerve and muscle. American Journal of Physiology-Legacy Content, 85(2), 207–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flyvbjerg, B. (2006). Five Misunderstandings About Case-Study Research. Qualitative Inquiry: QI, 12(2), 219–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, W. J. (1987). Simulation of chaotic EEG patterns with a dynamic model of the olfactory system. Biological Cybernetics, 56(2–3), 139–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1986). The Emotions. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (2007). What might emotions be? Comments on the Comments. Social Sciences Information. Information Sur Les Sciences Sociales, 46(3), 433–443.

  • Frijda, N. H., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Rietveld, E. (2014). Impulsive action: Emotional impulses and their control. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friston, K., Kilner, J., & Harrison, L. (2006). A free energy principle for the brain. Journal of Physiology, Paris, 100(1–3), 70–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2012). What Is Phenomenology? In Phenomenology (pp. 7–18). Springer.

  • Gamma, A., & Metzinger, T. (2021). The Minimal Phenomenal Experience questionnaire (MPE-92M): Towards a phenomenological profile of “pure awareness” experiences in meditators. Plos one, 16(7), e0253694.

  • Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. Hilldale, USA, 1(2).

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA, US. Houghton, Mifflin and Company.

  • Gooden, B. A. (1994). Mechanism of the human diving response. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science: THe Official Journal of the Pavlovian Society, 29(1), 6–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graneheim, U. H., Lindgren, B. M., & Lundman, B. (2017). Methodological challenges in qualitative content analysis: A discussion paper. Nurse Education Today, 56, 29–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grosmaitre, X., Santarelli, L. C., Tan, J., Luo, M., & Ma, M. (2007). Dual functions of mammalian olfactory sensory neurons as odor detectors and mechanical sensors. Nature Neuroscience, 10(3), 348–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haugeland, J. (1998) Mind embodied and embedded. In: Having thought: Essays in the metaphysics of mind, ed. Haugeland, J., pp. 207–40. Harvard University Press.

  • Høffding, S. (2014). What is Skilled Coping?: Experts on Expertise. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 21(9–10), 49–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Høffding, S., & Martiny, K. (2016). Framing a phenomenological interview: What, why and how. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 539–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hristovski, R., Davids, K. W., & Araujo, D. (2009). Information for regulating action in sport: Metastability and emergence of tactical solutions under ecological constraints. In M. Raab, D. Araujo, & H. Ripoll (Eds.), Perspectives on cognition and action in sport (pp. 43–57). Nova Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ilardo, M. A., Moltke, I., Korneliussen, T. S., Cheng, J., Stern, A. J., Racimo, F., de Barros Damgaard, P., Sikora, M., Seguin-Orlando, A., Rasmussen, S., van den Munckhof, I. C. L., ter Horst, R., Joosten, L. A. B., Netea, M. G., Salingkat, S., Nielsen, R., & Willerslev, E. (2018). Physiological and genetic adaptations to diving in sea nomads. Cell, 173(3), 569–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ilundáin-Agurruza, J. (2015). From clumsy failure to skillful fluency: A phenomenological analysis of and Eastern solution to sport’s choking effect. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 14(2), 397–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, J., Roy, S., Liu, Y., Cao, Y., Fletcher, M., Lu, L., Boughter, J. D., Grün, S., & Heck, D. H. (2014). Whisker barrel cortex delta oscillations and gamma power in the awake mouse are linked to respiration. Nature Communications, 5, 3572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karalis, N., & Sirota, A. (2022). Breathing coordinates cortico-hippocampal dynamics in mice during offline states. Nature Communications, 13(1), 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S. (2012). Multistability and metastability: understanding dynamic coordination in the brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 367(1591), 906–918.

  • Ko, J. Y., Jones, G. A., Heo, M. S., Kang, Y. S., & Kang, S. H. (2010). A fifty-year production and economic assessment of common property-based management of marine living common resources: A case study for the women divers communities in Jeju, South Korea. Marine Policy, 34(3), 624–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luck, S. J., Hillyard, S. A., Mouloua, M., & Hawkins, H. L. (1996). Mechanisms of visual–spatial attention: Resource allocation or uncertainty reduction? Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 22(3), 725–737.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDowell, J. (2007). Response to Dreyfus. Inquiry: A Journal of Medical Care Organization, Provision and Financing, 50(4), 366–370.

  • McKnight, J. C., Mulder, E., Ruesch, A., Kainerstorfer, J. M., Wu, J., Hakimi, N., Balfour, S., Bronkhorst, M., Horschig, J. M., Pernett, F., Sato, K., Hastie, G. D., Tyack, P., & Schagatay, E. (2021). When the human brain goes diving: Using near-infrared spectroscopy to measure cerebral and systemic cardiovascular responses to deep, breath-hold diving in elite freedivers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 376(1831), 20200349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mehrpour, M., Shams-Hosseini, N. S., & Rezaali, S. (2014). Effect of scuba-diving on optic nerve and sheath diameters. Medical Journal of the Islamic Republic of Iran, 28, 89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (2003). Nature: Course Notes from the College de France (trans. By R. Vallier). Evanston (IL): Northwestern University Press.

  • Miller, J. D., Ledingham, I. M., & Jennett, W. B. (1970). Effects of hyperbaric oxygen on intracranial pressure and cerebral blood flow in experimental cerebral oedema. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 33(6), 745–755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mohri, M., Torii, R., Nagaya, K., Shiraki, K., Elsner, R., Takeuchi, H., Park, Y. S., & Hong, S. K. (1995). Diving patterns of ama divers of Hegura Island, Japan. Undersea & Hyperbaric Medicine: Journal of the Undersea and Hyperbaric Medical Society, Inc, 22(2), 137–143.

  • Olivares, F. A., Vargas, E., Fuentes, C., Martínez-Pernía, D., & Canales-Johnson, A. (2015). Neurophenomenology revisited: Second-person methods for the study of human consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perl, O., Ravia, A., Rubinson, M., Eisen, A., Soroka, T., Mor, N., Secundo, L., & Sobel, N. (2019). Human non-olfactory cognition phase-locked with inhalation. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(5), 501–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Philippot, P., Chapelle, G., & Blairy, S. (2002). Respiratory feedback in the generation of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 16(5), 605–627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quadt, L., Critchley, H. D., & Garfinkel, S. N. (2018). The neurobiology of interoception in health and disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1428(1), 112–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravn, S., & Høffding, S. (2017). The promise of “sporting bodies” in phenomenological thinking–how exceptional cases of practice can contribute to develop foundational phenomenological concepts. Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health, 9(1), 56–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radak, Z., Zhao, Z., Koltai, E., Ohno, H., & Atalay, M. (2013). Oxygen Consumption and Usage During Physical Exercise: The Balance Between Oxidative Stress and ROS-Dependent Adaptive Signaling. Antioxidants & Redox Signaling, 18(10), 1208–1246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, E., Denys, D., & Van Westen, M. (2018). Ecological-Enactive Cognition as engaging with a field of relevant affordances. In The Oxford handbook of 4E cognition (p. 41). Oxford University Press.

  • Rietveld, E., & Kiverstein, J. (2014). A Rich Landscape of Affordances. Ecological Psychology: A Publication of the International Society for Ecological Psychology, 26(4), 325–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarantino, A., & Griffiths, P. (2009). Emotions in the wild: The situated perspective on emotion. Cambridge Handbook Of Situated Cognition, 347–353.

  • Schagatay, E. (2011). Predicting performance in competitive apnea diving. Part III: Depth. Diving and Hyperbaric Medicine, 41, 216–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schagatay, E., Lodin-Sundström, A., & Abrahamsson, E. (2011). Underwater working times in two groups of traditional apnea divers in Asia: The Ama and the Bajau. Diving and Hyperbaric Medicine, 41(1), 27–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmicking, D. (2010). A Toolbox of Phenomenological Methods. In D. Schmicking & S. Gallagher (Eds.), Handbook of Phenomenology and Cognitive Science (pp. 35–55). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Seifert, L., Wattebled, L., Herault, R., Poizat, G., Adé, D., Gal-Petitfaux, N., & Davids, K. (2014). Neurobiological degeneracy and affordance perception support functionalintra-individual variability of inter-limb coordination during ice climbing. PloS one, 9(2), e89865

  • Shargel, D., & Prinz, J. J. (2018). An enactivist theory of emotional content. The Ontology of Emotion, 110–129.

  • Smith, J. A., Flowers, P., & Larkin, M. (2009). Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis: Theory, Method and Research. SAGE.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stacey, N., Steenbergen, D. J., Clifton, J., & Acciaioli, G. (2018). Understanding social wellbeing and values of small-scale fisheries amongst the Sama-Bajau of archipelagic Southeast Asia. In Social wellbeing and the values of small-scale fisheries (pp. 97–123). Springer, Cham.

  • Sutton, J., McIlwain, D., Christensen, W., & Geeves, A. (2011). Applying Intelligence to the Reflexes: Embodied Skills and Habits between Dreyfus and Descartes. Journal of the British Society for Phenomenology, 42(1), 78–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toner, J., Montero, B. G., & Moran, A. (2016) Reflective and prereflective bodily awareness in skilled action. Psychology of Consciousness.

  • Travis, F., & Pearson, C. (2000). Pure consciousness: Distinct phenomenological and physiological correlates of “consciousness itself.” International Journal of Neuroscience, 100(1–4), 77–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tschacher, W., & Haken, H. (2007). Intentionality in non-equilibrium systems? The functional aspects of self-organized pattern formation. New Ideas in Psychology, 25(1), 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Usrey, W. M., & Kastner, S. (2020). Functions of the Visual Thalamus in Selective Attention. The Cognitive Neurosciences, 367.

  • van Dijk, L., & Rietveld, E. (2018). Situated anticipation. Synthese.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Dijk, L., & Withagen, R. (2016). Temporalizing agency: Moving beyond on- and offline cognition. Theory & Psychology, 26(1), 5–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J. (1996). Neurophenomenology: A methodological remedy for the hard problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 3(4), 330–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F., Lachaux, J. P., Rodriguez, E., & Martinerie, J. (2001). The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2(4), 229–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varga, S., & Heck, D. H. (2017). Rhythms of the body, rhythms of the brain: Respiration, neural oscillations, and embodied cognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 56, 77–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, J., & Hamill, O. P. (2020). Piezo2, a pressure sensitive channel is expressed in select neurons of the mouse brain: a putative mechanism for synchronizing neural networks by transducing intracranial pressure pulses. In bioRxiv (p. 2020.03.24.006452).

  • Withagen, R., Araújo, D., & de Poel, H. J. (2017). Inviting affordances and agency. New Ideas in Psychology, 45, 11–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woods, S. W., Charney, D. S., Goodman, W. K., & Heninger, G. R. (1988). Carbon Dioxide-Induced Anxiety: Behavioral, Physiologic, and Biochemical Effects of Carbon Dioxide in Patients With Panic Disorders and Healthy Subjects. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45(1), 43–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yasuma, F., & Hayano, J.-I. (2004). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Why does the heartbeat synchronize with respiratory rhythm? Chest, 125(2), 683–690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaccaro, A., Piarulli, A., Laurino, M., Garbella, E., Menicucci, D., Neri, B., & Gemignani, A. (2018). How Breath-Control Can Change Your Life: A Systematic Review on Psycho-Physiological Correlates of Slow Breathing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. (2008) Subjectivity and Selfhood: Investigating the First-Person Perspective. MIT Press.

  • Zelano, C., Jiang, H., Zhou, G., Arora, N., Schuele, S., Rosenow, J., & Gottfried, J. A. (2016). Nasal Respiration Entrains Human Limbic Oscillations and Modulates Cognitive Function. The Journal of Neuroscience: THe Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 36(49), 12448–12467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank all the freedivers who took the time out of their busy days to share with me their unbelievably fascinating and powerful stories. Thank you to Dave Ward, Erik Rietveld, Susanne Ravn, and Andreas Roepstorff, whose feedback, insights, and revisions provided critical direction. Thank you to three anonymous reviewers for their very detailed and helpful suggestions for ways to improve the manuscript and make it more relevant and accessible to a broader audience of readers. Thank you to Simon Høffding, whose inputs, comments, and revisions provided clarity, methodology, and motivation from conceptualization to publication of this project. Thank you to my supervisor Alistair Isaac for his wonderful guidance and encouragement throughout the course of this project. A final thank you to Venita De Souza and Matthew Emslie-Smith, whose constant support was crucial to my ability to complete this project amidst a global pandemic.

Funding

The author did not receive support from any organization for the submitted work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Suraiya Luecke conceptualized the project, conducted the literature review, designed the phenomenological interview questions and structure, recruited participants, conducted the phenomenological interviews, transcribed the interviews, analyzed the data, wrote the original draft of the manuscript, and edited the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Suraiya Luecke.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval

This study was approved on 25th June 2020 by the School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences Research Ethics Committee of The University of Edinburgh (358–1920/5).

Consent to participate

All participants gave their written informed consent to participate in this study and were able to withdraw at any point during the process.

Consent for publication

All participants gave their written informed consent for publication of any data corresponding to this study and were able to withdraw consent at any point during the process.

Conflicts of interest

The author has no conflicts of interest to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 8 KB)

Supplementary file2 (DOCX 45 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Luecke, S. Freediving neurophenomenology and skilled action: an investigation of brain, body, and behavior through breath. Phenom Cogn Sci 22, 761–797 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-022-09808-8

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-022-09808-8

Keywords

Navigation