Skip to main content
Log in

Narratives, culture, and folk psychology

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper, I aim to determine to what extent contemporary cross-cultural and developmental research can shed light on the role that narrative practices might play in the development of folk psychology. In particular, I focus on the role of narrative practices in the development of false belief understanding, which has been regarded as a milestone in the development of folk psychology. Second, I aim to discuss possible cognitive procedures that may underlie successful performance in false belief tasks. Methodologically, I distinguish between two kinds of narrative practices: ‘mentalistic narrative practice’ (which involves an explicit reference to another person’s mental states), and ‘behavioral-contextual narrative practice’ (which involves an explicit reference to the (normative) behavior of another person in a specific socio-situational context). Whereas the former is more prevalent in Western cultures than in Eastern cultures, the latter is predominantly used by members of Eastern cultures. Mentalistic narrative practices correlate with cultural divergences in the development of false belief understanding throughout ontogeny but do not seem to play the key role. The analysis shows that (i) conceptual change and the acquisition of mental state terms is essential for passing the false belief task, and that (ii) theory is likely to be the cognitive mechanism involved here such as proposed by Theory Theory. However, Hutto’s Narrative Practice Hypothesis trumps over Theory Theory to account for the varieties and ambiguities people typically meet when understanding each other in everyday life.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. There are also other versions of TT and ST, which defend another notion of ‘folk psychology’ than it is used here. These versions are not discussed in the present investigation.

  2. The role of inhibitory processes has been emphazised not only by simulation theorists but also nativist theory theorists (see e.g., Carruthers 2013).

References

  • Alberti, K. (2010). Japanische märchen. Bremen: Europäischer Hochschulverlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, K. (2012). Do apes read minds? London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. (2011). Mindreaders. the cognitive basis of “theory of mind”. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 1311–1320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1989). Young children’s attribution of action to beliefs and desires. Child Development, 60, 946–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., et al. (1992). Functional analysis of the contents of maternal speech to infants of 5 and 13 months in four cultures: Argentina, France, Japan, and the United States. Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 593–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carnes, P. (1992). The Japanese face of Aesop: Hoshi Shin’ichi and modern fable tradition. Journal of Folklore Research, 29(1), 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpendale, J., & Lewis, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind: the development of children’s social understanding withing social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(1), 79–96.

  • Carruthers, P. (2013). Mindreading in infancy. Mind and Language, 28(2), 141–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Courtin, C., & Melot, A. M. (1998). Development of theories of mind in deaf children. In M. Marschark (Ed.), Psychological Perspectives on Deafness (pp. 79–102). Malwah, NJ.: Erlbaum.

  • Doan, S. N., & Wang, Q. (2010). Maternal discussions of mental states and behaviors: relations to emotion situation knowledge in European American and immigrant Chinese children. Child Development, 81(5), 1490–1503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fabricius, W. V., Boyer, T., Weimer, A. A., & Carroll, K. (2010). True or false: do five-year-olds understand belief? Developmental Psychology, 46, 1402–1416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiebich, A. (in press). Mindreading with ease? Fluency and belief reasoning in 4- to 5-year-olds. Synthese. doi: 10.1007/s11229-013-0301-5.

  • Gallagher, S., & Hutto, D. (2008). Understanding others through primary interaction and narrative practice. In J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 17–38). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Garfield, J., Peterson, C., & Perry, T. (2001). Social cognition, language acquisition, and the development of the theory of mind. Mind and Language, 16(5), 494–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. (2006). Simulating minds: the philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff (1998). Words, Thoughts, and Theories. Massachusetts Institute of Technology: MIT Press.

  • Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (1992). Why the child’s theory of mind really is a theory. Mind and Language, 7(1), 145–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Jaffee, S. R., Happé, F., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2005). Origins of individual differences in theory of mind: from nature to nurture? Child Development, 76(2), 356–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D. (2007). The narrative practice hypothesis: origins and applications of folk psychology. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement, 60, 43–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D. (2008a). The narrative practice hypothesis: clarifications and implications. Philosophical Explorations, 11(3), 175–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D. (2008b). Folk psychological narratives: the sociocultural basis of understanding reasons. Cambridge MA: Bradford Books MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (1996). Cognitive factory and family structure associated with theory of mind development in young children. Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 70–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 358, 345–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: the origins of ‘theory of mind’. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, C., Freedman, N. H., Kryiakidou, C., Maridaki-Kassotaki, K., & Berridge, D. M. (1996). Social influences on false belief access: specific sibling influences or general apprenticeship. Child Development, 67, 2930–2947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, D., Wellman, H. M., Tardiff, T., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2008). Theory of mind development in Chinese children: a meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across cultures and languages. Developmental Psychology, 44, 523–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malle, B. F., Knobe, J. M., & Nelson, S. E. (2007). Actor-observer asymmetries in explanations of behavior: new answers to an old question. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(4), 491–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAlister, A., & Peterson, C. C. (2012). Siblings, theory of mind, and executive functioning in children aged 3–6 years: new longitudinal evidence. Child Development, 84(4), 1442–1458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–646.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949–971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naito, M., & Koyama, K. (2006). The development of false- belief understanding in Japanese children: delay and difference? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 290–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (2009). Narrative practices and folk psychology: a perspective from developmental psychology. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16(6–8), 69–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1995). Teaching theory of mind: a new approach to social skills training for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25(4), 415–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., Ruffman, T., & Leekam, S. (1994). Theory of mind is contagious: you catch it from your sibs. Child Development, 65, 1228–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversation and the theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology, Child Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 36, 459–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravenscroft, I. (2010). Folk Psychology as a Theory. Standford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/folkpsych-theory/220). New York: Academic Press.

  • Ruffman, T., Perner, J., Naito, M., Parkin, L., & Clements, W. (1998). Older (but not younger) siblings facilitate false belief understanding. Developmental Psychology, 34, 161–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73(3), 734–751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C., & Mackintosh, E. (2007). Mind what mother says: narrative input and theory of mind in typical children and those on autism spectrum. Child Development, 78(3), 839–858.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., Cyphers, L., Sueko, T., & Ogino, M. (1992). Language and play at one year: a comparison of toddlers and mothers in the United States and Japan. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15(1), 19–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tardif, T., & Wellman, H. M. (2000). Acquisition of mental state language in mandarin- and canronese-speaking children. Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 25–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development, 77(2), 465–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2008). Stepping stones to others’ minds: maternal talk relates to child mental state language and emotion understanding at 15, 24, and 33 months. Child Development, 79(2), 284–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M., & Carlson, S. M. (1997). The relation between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind. Child Development, 68(3), 436–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaccari, C., & Marschark, M. (1997). Communication between parents and deaf children: implications for social-emotional development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 793–801.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Q. (2001). “Did you have fun? American and Chinese mother-child conversations about shared emotional experiences. Cognitive Development, 16, 693–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woolfe, T., Want, S. C., & Siegal, M. (2002). Signposts to development: theory of mind in deaf children. Child Development, 73(3), 768–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xuegang, S., & Guoyun, C. (2005). Chinese fairy-tales. Victoria: Penguin Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, A. Z. (2007). The nature of belief. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14, 61–82.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to Dan Hutto for helpful comments on a previous version of this paper. Moreover, I would like to thank Tomoo Ueda for inspiring discussions on narrative practices that are prevalent in Japan. This research has been supported by the Volkswagen Foundation project The Social Mind.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anika Fiebich.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fiebich, A. Narratives, culture, and folk psychology. Phenom Cogn Sci 15, 135–149 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9378-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9378-7

Keywords

Navigation