Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Prosaccade and Antisaccade Paradigms in Persons with Alzheimer’s Disease: A Meta-Analytic Review

  • Review
  • Published:
Neuropsychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Persons with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) are at high Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) risk but the development of sensitive measures to assess subtle cognitive decline in this population poses a major challenge for clinicians and researchers. Eye movement monitoring is a non-invasive, sensitive way to assess subtle cognitive processes in clinical populations. We conducted a critical review and a meta-analysis of the literature on pro and antisaccade paradigm in AD/MCI. The meta-analysis included 20 studies, all of which used the prosaccade paradigm and 13 of which studied the antisaccade paradigm as well. Our meta-analysis showed that AD but not MCI patients showed longer prosaccade latencies when compared to controls. While antisaccade latencies did not differentiate between patients from controls, antisaccade error rate were significantly increased among patients in comparison to controls in over 87% of the studies. These findings highlight antisaccade error rate as a reliable tool to distinguish inhibition abilities between AD/MCI and healthy older persons.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abel, L. A., Unverzagt, F., & Yee, R. D. (2002). Effects of stimulus predictability and interstimulus gap on saccades in Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 13(4), 235–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alichniewicz, K. K., Brunner, F., Klünemann, H. H., & Greenlee, M. W. (2013). Neural correlates of saccadic inhibition in healthy elderly and patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 467.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Alzheimer’s Association. (2015). 2015 Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 11(3), 332–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, T. J., & MacAskill, M. R. (2013). Eye movements in patients with neurodegenerative disorders. Nature Reviews Neurology, 9(2), 74–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bélanger, S., Belleville, S., & Gauthier, S. (2010). Inhibition impairments in Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment and healthy aging: Effect of congruency proportion in a Stroop task. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 581–590.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biscaldi, M., Fischer, B., & Hartnegg, K. (2000). Voluntary saccadic control in dyslexia. Perception, 29(5), 509–521.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Boxer, A. L., Garbutt, S., Rankin, K. P., Hellmuth, J., Neuhaus, J., Miller, B. L., et al. (2006). Medial versus lateral frontal lobe contributions to voluntary saccade control as revealed by the study of patients with frontal lobe degeneration. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(23), 6354–6363.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Boxer, A. L., Garbutt, S., Seeley, W. W., Jafari, A., Heuer, H. W., Mirsky, J., et al. (2012). Saccade abnormalities in autopsy-confirmed frontotemporal lobar degeneration and Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology, 69(4), 509–517.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bundesen, C. (1990). A theory of visual attention. Psychological Review, 97(4), 523.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bylsma, F. W., Rasmusson, D. X., Rebok, G. W., Keyl, P. M., Tune, L., & Brandt, J. (1995). Changes in visual fixation and saccadic eye movements in Alzheimer's disease. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 19(1), 33–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, T. J., Devereaux, A., Higham, S., & Kelly, C. (2015). The disengagement of visual attention in Alzheimer's disease: A longitudinal eye-tracking study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 7, 118. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00118.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, T. J., Higham, S., Mayes, J., Dale, M., Shaunak, S., & Lekwuwa, G. (2013). The role of working memory and attentional disengagement on inhibitory control: effects of aging and Alzheimer's disease. Age, 35(5), 1637–1650.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, T. J., Higham, S., Renvoize, T., Patel, J., Dale, M., Suriya, A., et al. (2005). Inhibitory control of saccadic eye movements and cognitive impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. Biological Psychiatry, 57(9), 1052–1060.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crutcher, M. D., Calhoun-Haney, R., Manzanares, C. M., Lah, J. J., Levey, A. I., & Zola, S. M. (2009). Eye tracking during a visual paired comparison task as a predictor of early dementia. American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease and Other Dementias, 24(3), 258–266.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Currie, J., Ramsden, B., McArthur, C., & Maruff, P. (1991). Validation of a clinical antisaccadic eye movement test in the assessment of dementia. Archives of Neurology, 48(6), 644–648.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (2003). Delayed saccades, but not delayed manual aiming movements, require visual attention shifts. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1004(1), 289–296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eenshuistra, R. M., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & van der Molen, M. W. (2004). Age-related changes in antisaccade task performance: Inhibitory control or working-memory engagement? Brain and Cognition, 56(2), 177–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, W. A., & Sharpe, J. A. (1986). Saccadic eye movement dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease. Annals of Neurology, 20(4), 464–471.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garbutt, S., Matlin, A., Hellmuth, J., Schenk, A. K., Johnson, J. K., Rosen, H., et al. (2008). Oculomotor function in frontotemporal lobar degeneration, related disorders and Alzheimer's disease. Brain, 131(5), 1268–1281.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gurnani, A. S., & Gavett, B. E. (2017). The differential effects of Alzheimer's Disease and lewy body pathology on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. [journal article]. Neuropsychology Review, 27(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-016-9334-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hannula, D. E., Althoff, R. R., Warren, D. E., Riggs, L., Cohen, N. J., & Ryan, J. D. (2010). Worth a glance: Using eye movements to investigate the cognitive neuroscience of memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4(166), 52–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannula, D. E., Ryan, J. D., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2007). Rapid onset relational memory effects are evident in eye movement behavior, but not in hippocampal amnesia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(10), 1690–1705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hershey, L. A., Whicker, L., Abel, L. A., Dell'Osso, L., Traccis, S., & Grossniklaus, D. (1983). Saccadic latency measurements in dementia. Archives of Neurology, 40(9), 592–593.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer, H. W., Mirsky, J. B., Kong, E. L., Dickerson, B. C., Miller, B. L., Kramer, J. H., et al. (2013). Antisaccade task reflects cortical involvement in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology, 81(14), 1235–1243.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, J. P., & Green, S. (2011). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions (Vol. 4). UK: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutton, S. B., & Ettinger, U. (2006). The antisaccade task as a research tool in psychopathology: A critical review. Psychophysiology, 43(3), 302–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jadad, A. R., Moore, R. A., Carroll, D., Jenkinson, C., Reynolds, D. J. M., Gavaghan, D. J., et al. (1996). Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: is blinding necessary? Controlled Clinical Trials, 17(1), 1–12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jak, A. J., Preis, S. R., Beiser, A. S., Seshadri, S., Wolf, P. A., Bondi, M. W., et al. (2016). Neuropsychological Criteria for Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia Risk in the Framingham Heart Study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1-7.

  • Kalesnykas, R., & Hallett, P. (1994). Retinal eccentricity and the latency of eye saccades. Vision Research, 34(4), 517–531.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, L. D., Pratt, J., Levine, B., & Black, S. E. (2010). Antisaccades: A probe into the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in Alzheimer's disease. A critical review. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 19(3), 781–793.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, L. D., Pratt, J., Levine, B., & Black, S. E. (2012). Executive deficits detected in mild Alzheimer's disease using the antisaccade task. Brain and Behavior, 2(1), 15–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kent, B., Hvoslef-Eide, M., Saksida, L., & Bussey, T. (2016). The representational–hierarchical view of pattern separation: Not just hippocampus, not just space, not just memory? Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 129, 99–106.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lagun, D., Manzanares, C., Zola, S. M., Buffalo, E. A., & Agichtein, E. (2011). Detecting cognitive impairment by eye movement analysis using automatic classification algorithms. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 201(1), 196–203.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leigh, R., & Kennard, C. (2004). Using saccades as a research tool in the clinical neurosciences. Brain, 127(3), 460–477.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mewborn, C. M., Lindbergh, C. A., & Stephen Miller, L. (2017). Cognitive Interventions for cognitively healthy, mildly impaired, and mixed samples of older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized-controlled trials. Neuropsychology Review. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-11017-19350-11068.

  • Michie, S., Abraham, C., Whittington, C., McAteer, J., & Gupta, S. (2009). Effective techniques in healthy eating and physical activity interventions: a meta-regression. Health Psychology, 28(6), 690–701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mielke, R., Kessler, J., Fink, G., Herholz, K., & Heiss, W.-D. (1995). Dysfunction of visual cortex contributes to disturbed processing of visual information in Alzheimer's disease. International Journal of Neuroscience, 82(1–2), 1–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1), 167–202.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (1999). Oculomotor evidence for neocortical systems but not cerebellar dysfunction in autism. Neurology, 52(5), 917–917.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, A. J., & Shiri-Feshki, M. (2009). Rate of progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia–meta-analysis of 41 robust inception cohort studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 119(4), 252–265.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Annals of Internal Medicine, 151(4), 264–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Systematic Reviews, 4(1), 1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Molitor, R. J., Ko, P. C., & Ally, B. A. (2015). Eye movements in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 44(1), 1–12.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mosimann, U. P., Müri, R. M., Burn, D. J., Felblinger, J., O'Brien, J. T., & McKeith, I. G. (2005). Saccadic eye movement changes in Parkinson's disease dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Brain, 128(6), 1267–1276.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moss, R. A. (2016). A theory on the singular function of the hippocampus: Facilitating the binding of new circuits of cortical columns. AIMS Neuroscience, 3(3), 264–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakashima, Y., Morita, K., Ishii, Y., Shouji, Y., & Uchimura, N. (2010). Characteristics of exploratory eye movements in elderly people: possibility of early diagnosis of dementia. Psychogeriatrics, 10(3), 124–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Driscoll, G. A., Dépatie, L., Holahan, A.-L. V., Savion-Lemieux, T., Barr, R. G., Jolicoeur, C., et al. (2005). Executive functions and methylphenidate response in subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1452–1460.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Okonkwo, O. C., Wadley, V. G., Ball, K., Vance, D. E., & Crowe, M. (2008). Dissociations in visual attention deficits among persons with mild cognitive impairment. Aging Neuropsychol C, 15(4), 492–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peltsch, A., Hemraj, A., Garcia, A., & Munoz, D. (2011). Age-related trends in saccade characteristics among the elderly. Neurobiology of Aging, 32(4), 669–679.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peltsch, A., Hemraj, A., Garcia, A., & Munoz, D. P. (2014). Saccade deficits in amnestic mild cognitive impairment resemble mild Alzheimer's disease. European Journal of Neuroscience, 39(11), 2000–2013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256(3), 183–194.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, R. C., & Bennett, D. (2005). Mild cognitive impairment: is it Alzheimer's disease or not? Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 7(3), 241–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., & Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology, 56(3), 303–308.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, J., Abrams, R. A., & Chasteen, A. L. (1997). Initiation and inhibition of saccadic eye movements in younger and older adults: An analysis of the gap effect. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 52(2), P103-P107.

  • Pratt, J., Dodd, M., & Welsh, T. (2006). Growing older does not always mean moving slower: examining aging and the saccadic motor system. Journal of Motor Behavior, 38(5), 373–382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research (Vol. 6): Sage.

  • Rösler, A., Mapstone, M. E., Hays, A. K., Mesulam, M., Rademaker, A., Gitelman, D. R., et al. (2000). Alterations of visual search strategy in Alzheimer's disease and aging. Neuropsychology, 14(3), 398–408.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rüb, U., Del Tredici, K., Schultz, C., Büttner-Ennever, J., & Braak, H. (2001). The premotor region essential for rapid vertical eye movements shows early involvement in Alzheimer's disease-related cytoskeletal pathology. Vision Research, 41(16), 2149–2156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, J. D., Althoff, R. R., Whitlow, S., & Cohen, N. J. (2000). Amnesia is a deficit in relational memory. Psychological Science, 11(6), 454–461.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sánchez-Meca, J., & Marín-Martínez, F. (1997). Homogeneity tests in meta-analysis: A Monte Carlo comparison of statistical power and Type I error. Quality and Quantity, 31(4), 385–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Santana, R., Mendiburu, A., & Lozano, J. A. (2015). Multi-view classification of psychiatric conditions based on saccades. Applied Soft Computing, 31, 308–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saslow, M. (1967). Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. JOSA, 57(8), 1024–1029.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Scinto, L. F., Daffner, K. R., Castro, L., Weintraub, S., Vavrik, M., & Mesulam, M. M. (1994). Impairment of spatially directed attention in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease as measured by eye movements. Archives of Neurology, 51(7), 682–688.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, S. C., & Giovannetti, T. (2015). The potential utility of eye movements in the detection and characterization of everyday functional difficulties in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology Review, 25(2), 199–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shafiq-Antonacci, R., Maruff, P., Masters, C., & Currie, J. (2003). Spectrum of saccade system function in Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology, 60(9), 1272–1278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shakespeare, T., Yong, K., Kaski, D., Schott, J. M., & Crutch, S. (2014). Abnormalities of fixation, saccade, and pursuit in posterior cortical atrophy compared to typical AD. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 10(4), P199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silverberg, N. B., Ryan, L. M., Carrillo, M. C., Sperling, R., Petersen, R. C., Posner, H. B., et al. (2011). Assessment of cognition in early dementia. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7(3), e60–e76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 1657–1661.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, R. A., Aisen, P. S., Beckett, L. A., Bennett, D. A., Craft, S., Fagan, A. M., et al. (2011). Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7(3), 280–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, R. A., Dickerson, B. C., Pihlajamaki, M., Vannini, P., LaViolette, P. S., Vitolo, O. V., et al. (2010). Functional alterations in memory networks in early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuromolecular Medicine, 12(1), 27–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Thulborn, K. R., Martin, C., & Voyvodic, J. T. (2000). Functional MR imaging using a visually guided saccade paradigm for comparing activation patterns in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease and in cognitively able elderly volunteers. AJNR, 21(3), 524–531.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tzekov, R., & Mullan, M. (2014). Vision function abnormalities in Alzheimer disease. Survey of Ophthalmology, 59(4), 414–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verheij, S., Muilwijk, D., Pel, J. J., van der Cammen, T. J., Mattace-Raso, F. U., & van der Steen, J. (2012). Visuomotor impairment in early-stage Alzheimer's disease: changes in relative timing of eye and hand movements. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 30(1), 131–143.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vos, S. J., Verhey, F., Frölich, L., Kornhuber, J., Wiltfang, J., Maier, W., et al. (2015). Prevalence and prognosis of Alzheimer’s disease at the mild cognitive impairment stage. Brain, 138, 1327–1338.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, T. L., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 775–808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, Q., Wang, T., Su, N., Liu, Y., Xiao, S., & Kapoula, Z. (2011). Long latency and high variability in accuracy-speed of prosaccades in Alzheimer’s disease at mild to moderate stage. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra, 1(1), 318–329.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, Q., Wang, T., Su, N., Xiao, S., & Kapoula, Z. (2013). Specific saccade deficits in patients with Alzheimer’s disease at mild to moderate stage and in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Age, 35(4), 1287–1298.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, L.-K., Ryan, J. D., Cowell, R. A., & Barense, M. D. (2013). Recognition memory impairments caused by false recognition of novel objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1384–1397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yun, J.-Y., Lee, D. Y., Seo, E. H., Choo, I. H., Park, S. Y., Kim, S. G., et al. (2011). Neural correlates of stroop performance in Alzheimer’s disease: a FDG-PET study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra, 1(1), 190–201.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Naomi Kahana Levy.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

All authors report no disclosures or conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kahana Levy, N., Lavidor, M. & Vakil, E. Prosaccade and Antisaccade Paradigms in Persons with Alzheimer’s Disease: A Meta-Analytic Review. Neuropsychol Rev 28, 16–31 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-017-9362-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-017-9362-4

Keywords

Navigation