Skip to main content
Log in

On the apparent unbindability of overt third-person pronouns in Japanese

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Contrary to the widely held view that Japanese overt third-person pronouns such as kare and kanozyo cannot function as bound variables, it has been sporadically reported in the literature that there are cases in which they can receive a bound-variable interpretation. The present paper attempts to provide an account of why Japanese third-person pronouns can be construed as bound variables only in a subset of the contexts in which bound pronouns in English can occur. I argue that Japanese overt third-person pronouns should be analyzed as epithets, claiming that they can function as bound variables only when Condition B and the so-called anti-logophoricity constraint are simultaneously satisfied. I also claim that the apparent insensitivity of the referential use of kare/kanozyo to the anti-logophoricity constraint is attributed to the fact that Japanese (but not English) allows a structure in which a null pronoun is juxtaposed with an appositive epithet phrase.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Abbreviations used in this paper are as follows: ACC = accusative case, ASP = aspect, COMP = complementizer, COP = copula, DAT = dative case, GEN = genitive case, HON = honorific marker, NOM = nominative case, PART = particle, PAST = past tense, Q = question particle, TOP = topic.

  2. The underlying idea here is that as opposed to variable binding, which is regulated by Binding Conditions, coreference is governed by an economy condition called Rule I (Grodzinsky and Reinhart 1993; Reinhart 1983), which blocks accidental coreference if the coreference interpretation is equivalent to what would be obtained by binding.

  3. It should be noted that not all researchers agree that quantificational binding requires c-command. See Barker (2012).

  4. Since the issue of whether A′-binding of epithets is possible is orthogonal to the purposes of this paper, I will not address it here. For epithets in resumptive contexts, see, among others, Aoun and Choueiri (2000), Aoun et al. (2001), and Safir (2004:63). Also, the jury is still out as to how sentences like Every boy 1 ’s mother thinks that the bastard 1 is crazy should be analyzed. See Kayne (1994:23–24) and Reinhart (1983).

  5. Given that coreference (as opposed to variable binding) is governed by Rule I rather than Binding Principles (Grodzinsky and Reinhart 1993), it is more accurate to state that epithets are subject to (i) either Condition B or Rule I and (ii) the anti-logophoricity constraint. For brevity’s sake, we ignore Rule I here.

  6. This characterization of anti-logophoric pronouns is only approximate, but for the purposes of this paper, it will suffice.

  7. Haïk credits the example to Haj Ross.

  8. That said, the precise nature of Condition C is a matter of debate. See Schlenker (2005).

  9. The use of the feminine form kanozyo is also avoided when referring to someone socially superior to the speaker. For other characteristics of Japanese third-person pronouns, see Hinds (1975, 1978).

  10. While the evaluative connotations of kare/kanozyo seem to be very subtle for some speakers (especially those in the younger generation), all the speakers I consulted confirmed, regardless of age, that they would avoid using kare/kanozyo to refer to their superiors. I take this to indicate that kare and kanozyo come with certain expressive content associated with the speaker’s evaluation.

  11. A few potential counterexamples are found in Hoji (1991: e.g., (31a, b)). It might be worth mentioning that all those examples involve verbs of saying rather than verbs of thinking. Note also that Hoji acknowledges that there is variation among speakers with respect to the acceptability of those data.

  12. Patel-Grosz (2012) takes the fact that epithets can be bound by a quantificational antecedent as evidence for postulating a null pronominal anchor. As we will see, however, this claim runs counter to the well-established observation that the host of an appositive phrase must be a referring expression. Cf. Potts (2005:173ff).

  13. Note that here the evaluation of the epithet is due to the discourse speaker’s perspective (not Taro’s).

  14. I intentionally avoid using those epithets that involve (incorporated) demonstratives (e.g., aitu ‘that guy’) because the anaphoric use of Japanese demonstratives is known to be subject to some idiosyncratic constraints. See, among others, Kuno (1973) for discussion on the anaphoric use of demonstratives.

  15. Also, the domain of quantification must be restricted to male individuals in the case of kare and to female individuals in the case of kanozyo.

References

  • Aoun, Joseph, and Lina Choueiri. 2000. Epithets. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 18: 1–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aoun, Joseph, Lina Choueiri, and Norbert Hornstein. 2001. Resumption, movement, and derivational economy. Linguistic Inquiry 32: 371–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aoun, Joseph, and Norbert Hornstein. 1991. Bound and referential pronouns. In Logical structure and linguistics structure: cross-linguistic perspectives, eds. C.-T. James Huang and Robert May, 1–23. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, Chris. 2012. Quantificational binding does not require c-command. Linguistic Inquiry 43: 614–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, George Nicholas. 1975. The logophoric pronoun in Ewe: its role in discourse. Journal of West African Languages 10: 141–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, Chris, and Paul Martin Postal. 2012. Imposters: a study of pronominal agreement. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Déchaine, Rose-Marie, and Martina Wiltschko. 2002. Decomposing pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry 33: 409–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dubinsky, Stanley, and Robert Hamilton. 1998. Epithets as antilogophoric pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry 29: 685–693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbourne, Paul. 2005. Situations and individuals. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emonds, Joseph. 1979. Appositive relatives have no properties. Linguistic Inquiry 10: 211–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grodzinsky, Yosef, and Tanya Reinhart. 1993. The innateness of binding and coreference. Linguistic Inquiry 24: 69–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagège, Claude. 1974. Les pronoms logophoriques. Bulletin de la Société de Linguistique de Paris 69: 287–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haïk, Isabelle. 1984. Indirect binding. Linguistic Inquiry 15: 185–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hara, Takaaki. 2002. Bound variable interpretation and the degree of accessibility. In Student Organization of Linguistics in Europe (ConSOLE) IX, eds. Marjo van Koppen, Erica Thrift, Erik Jan van der Torre, and Malte Zimmermann, 82–95. Leiden: SOLE.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinds, John. 1975. Third person pronouns in Japanese. In Language in Japanese society, ed. Fred C. C. Peng, 129–157. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinds, John. 1978. Anaphora in Japanese conversation. In Anaphora in discourse, ed. John Hinds, 136–179. Alberta: Linguistic Research Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoji, Hajime. 1990. Theories of anaphora and aspects of Japanese syntax. Unpublished ms., Los Angeles: University of Southern California.

  • Hoji, Hajime. 1991. Kare. In Interdisciplinary approaches to language: essays in honor of S.-Y. Kuroda, eds. Carol J. Georgopoulos and Roberta Ishihara, 287–304. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hoji, Hajime. 1995. Demonstrative binding and Principle B. In North East Linguistic Society (NELS) 25, ed. Jill N. Beckman, 255–271. Amherst: GLSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoji, Hajime, Satoshi Kinsui, Yukinori Takubo, and Ayumi Ueyama. 2000. Demonstratives, bound variables, and reconstruction effects. In Generative Linguistics in the Old Word (GLOW) in ASIA II, 141–158. Nagoya: Nansan University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hornstein, Norbert, and Amy Weinberg. 1990. The necessity of LF. The Linguistic Review 7: 129–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, Ray. 1972. Semantic interpretation in generative grammar. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katada, Fusa. 1991. The LF representation of anaphors. Linguistic Inquiry 22: 287–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard. 1994. The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitagawa, Chisato. 1981. Anaphora in Japanese: kare and zibun. In Coyote papers 2, eds. Ann K. Farmer and Chisato Kitagawa, 61–75. Tucson: University of Arizona.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuno, Susumu. 1973. The structure of the Japanese language. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lasnik, Howard. 1976. Remarks on coreference. Linguistic Analysis 2: 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lasnik, Howard. 1989. On the necessity of binding conditions. In Essays on anaphora, ed. Howard Lasnik, 149–167. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lasnik, Howard. 2001. Subjects, objects, and the EPP. In Objects and other subjects: grammatical functions, functional categories, and configurationality, eds. William D. Davies and Stanley Dubinsky, 103–121. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lasnik, Howard, and Mamoru Saito. 1991. On the subject of infinitives. In Chicago Linguistic Society (CLS) 27, eds. Lise Dobrin, Lynn Nichols, and Rosa Rodriduez, 324–343. Chicago: Chicago: Linguistic Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montalbetti, Mario. 1984. After binding: on the interpretation of pronouns. PhD diss., MIT, Cambridge, MA.

  • Noguchi, Tohru. 1997. Two types of pronouns and variable binding. Language 73: 770–797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patel-Grosz, Pritty. 2012. Anti-locality at the interfaces. PhD diss., MIT, Cambridge, MA.

  • Pica, Pierre. 1994. Condition C and epistemic contexts: a case study of epithets and anti-logophoric pronouns in French. In Explorations in generative grammar: a festschrift for Dong-Whee Yang, eds. Young-Sun Kim, Byung-Choon Lee, Kyoung-Jae Lee, Kyun-Kwon Yang, and Jong-Kuri Yoon, 544–570. Seoul: Hankuk Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postal, Paul Martin. 1972. Pronominal epithets and similar items. Foundations of Language 9: 246–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postal, Paul Martin. 1974. On raising: an inquiry into one rule of English grammar and its theoretical implications. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potts, Christopher. 2005. The logic of conventional implicatures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinhart, Tanya. 1983. Anaphora and semantic interpretation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, John Robert. 1967. Constraints on variables in syntax. PhD diss., MIT, Cambridge, MA.

  • Safir, Ken. 2004. The syntax of (in) dependence. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saito, Mamoru, and Hajime Hoji. 1983. Weak crossover and move α in Japanese. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 1: 245–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlenker, Philippe. 2005. Minimize restrictors! (Notes on definite descriptions, Condition C and epithets). In Sinn und Bedeutung 9, eds. Emar Maier, Corien Bary, and Janneke Huitink, 385–416. Nijmegen: NCS.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thanks first and foremost to Dominique Sportiche for his feedback on an earlier version of this paper. I am also indebted to Vincent Homer, Nina Hyams, and Hilda Koopman for their input. Thanks also to Marcel den Dikken as well as three anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments and suggestions. All remaining errors are my own.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jun Yashima.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yashima, J. On the apparent unbindability of overt third-person pronouns in Japanese. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 33, 1421–1438 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-014-9269-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-014-9269-1

Keywords

Navigation