Skip to main content
Log in

A motivation-enhancing treatment to sustain goal engagement during life course transitions

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although theory-driven control striving treatments may sustain motivation for individuals navigating life course transitions, their efficacy during these challenging junctures remains unexamined. In a pre-post randomized field study (n = 316), a novel control striving treatment based on Heckhausen et al.’s (Psychol Rev 117:32–60, 2010) motivational theory of life-span development was administered to young adults making the landmark transition to university. For students who faced obstacles to goal attainment, the motivation-enhancing selective secondary control (SSC) striving treatment (vs. no-treatment) increased performance by 8 % in a two-semester course (74.85 % vs. 66.68 %). Consistent with theory, the SSC treatment-performance linkage was mediated by selective secondary and selective primary control in a hypothesized causal sequence. Findings advance the literature by showing control striving treatments can improve performance for some young adults in transition by promoting adaptive changes in theoretically-derived psychological process variables.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The present literature review focuses on control striving from the perspective of the MTLD and therefore does not address motivation treatments that: involve motivation theories other than Heckhausen et al.’s (2010); are not control strategy based, for example, attributional retraining (e.g., Perry and Hamm, in press), value enhancement (e.g., Hulleman and Harackiewicz 2009; Harackiewicz et al. 2012), intention implementation (e.g., Duckworth et al. 2011), goal setting (e.g., Morisano et al. 2010), or social belonging (e.g., Walton and Cohen 2011); do not concern motivation or performance (e.g., psychotherapy); or, focus on very young or old populations (e.g., Chapin and Dyck 1976; Gitlin et al. 2006a, b). Reviews of the broader motivation treatment literature are provided elsewhere (see Elliot et al., in press; Karabenick and Urdan 2014).

  2. Comprehensive meta-analyses by Robbins et al. (2004) and Richardson et al. (2012) showed HSGs (rs = .40–.41) and PAC (rs = .31–.59) were the strongest traditional and psychosocial correlates of university performance and therefore represent two of the most influential academic risk factors in university achievement settings. This implies students with a “double-jeopardy” low HSG-low PAC risk profile are prone to academic failure and may suffer extreme motivation deficits during the school-to-university transition. A one-time SSC treatment administered in a group setting is unlikely to remedy these substantial deficits. Low HSG-low PAC students may require more intensive intervention programs involving multicomponent treatments tailored to their specific needs and administered on multiple occasions.

  3. Although PAC should relate to student grades, it may not correlate with HSGs considering that the PAC items were context-specific and referred to students’ university experiences (see Perry et al. 2001). This implies that PAC should positively relate to university performance but may not relate to past HSGs given (a) shifts in perceptions of control during the major school-to-university transition and (b) the significant differences between high school and university achievement settings (see Perry 2003; Perry et al. 2001, 2005a).

  4. SSC treatment effects were consistent when accounting for autoregressive effects of T1 secondary control and initial test performance. For only low HSG-high PAC students, the SSC treatment (vs. no-treatment) increased (a) Time 3 secondary control when controlling for Time 1 secondary control (partially standardized β = .52, p = .021) and (b) final course grades when controlling for initial test performance [partially standardized β = .36, p = .036].

  5. To test Heckhausen et al.’s (2010) proposition that SSC should promote perceived control, we conducted a supplemental Treatment x HSG x PAC regression analysis with Time 3 PAC as the outcome measure when controlling for age and gender. The three-way interaction was significant (β = −.12, p = .009, CIs = −0.207 to −0.030), and simple–simple slope analyses indicated that the SSC treatment (vs. no-treatment) increased Time 3 PAC for individuals with low HSGs and high Time 1 PAC (partially standardized β = .49, p = .014, CIs = 0.097 to 0.873). The SSC treatment also increased Time 3 PAC for those with high HSGs and low Time 1 PAC (partially standardized β = .64, p = .002, CIs = 0.234 to 1.040). No treatment effects were observed for students with the remaining two combinations of HSG and PAC. Results were consistent when employing a traditional subgroups approach: The SSC treatment (vs. no-treatment) only increased Time 3 PAC for students with low HSGs and high initial PAC [t(238) = 2.31, p = .022, Ms = 4.66 vs. 4.38, M diff  = 0.28, d = 0.61] and for students with high HSGs and low initial PAC [t(238) = 1.98, p = .048, Ms = 4.13 vs. 3.87, M diff  = 0.25, d = 0.54].

  6. Although treatment conditions and experimental sessions were homogenous on the pre-treatment (baseline) variables, supplemental multi-level analyses were conducted to account for potential group effects by nesting students within (a) course tutorial sections and (b) experimental sessions based on recommendations by Tabachnick and Fidell (2013). SSC treatment effects on each of the outcome measures (secondary control, PAC, final grades) were consistent in both sets of multi-level analyses.

  7. Predicted values were consistent with the supposition that low HSG-low PAC students may be most susceptible to the detrimental effects of difficult transitions and possibly cannot be assisted by motivation treatments: Irrespective of treatment condition, these young adults reported the lowest selective secondary control (ZPRED = −.33, −.42), the lowest PAC (ZPRED = −.74, −.67), and achieved the lowest final grades (ZPRED = −.69, −.58).

References

  • American College Health Association. (2012). American College Health Association national college health assessment II: Reference group executive summary fall 2012. Hanover, MD: American College Health Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B. M. (2010). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapin, M., & Dyck, D. (1976). Persistence in children’s reading behavior as a function of N length and attributional retraining. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 511–515. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.85.5.511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chipperfield, J. G., Hamm, J. M., Perry, R. P., & Ruthig, J. C. (in press). Perceived control. In M. D. Robinson & M. Eid (Eds.), The happy mind: Cognitive contributions to well-being. New York: Springer.

  • Chipperfield, J. G., & Perry, R. P. (2006). Primary- and secondary-control strategies in later life: Predicting hospital outcomes in men and women. Health Psychology, 25, 226–236. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.25.2.226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chipperfield, J. G., Perry, R. P., Bailis, D. S., Ruthig, J. C., & Chuchmach, L. P. (2007). Gender differences in use of primary and secondary control strategies in older adults with major health problems. Psychology and Health, 22, 83–105. doi:10.1080/14768320500537563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chipperfield, J. G., Perry, R. P., & Menec, V. H. (1999). Primary and secondary control-enhancing strategies: Implications for health in later life. Journal of Aging and Health, 11, 517–539. doi:10.1177/089826439901100403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Apfel, N., & Brzustoski, P. (2009). Recursive processes in self-affirmation: Intervening to close the minority achievement gap. Science, 324, 400–403. doi:10.1126/science.1170769.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duckworth, A. L., Grant, H., Loew, B., Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2011). Self-regulation strategies improve self-discipline in adolescents: Benefits of mental contrasting and implantation intentions. Educational Psychology, 31, 17–26. doi:10.1080/01443410.2010.506003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., Dweck, C. S., & Yeager, D. S. (in press). Handbook of Competence and Motivation (2nd Edition): Theory and Applications. New York: Guilford Press.

  • Gitlin, L. N., Hauck, W. W., Winter, L., Dennis, M. P., & Schulz, R. (2006a). Effect of an in-home occupational and physical therapy intervention on reducing mortality in functionally vulnerable older people: Preliminary findings. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 54, 950–955. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2006.00733.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gitlin, L. N., Winter, L., Dennis, M. P., Corcoran, M., Schinfeld, S., & Hauck, W. W. (2006b). A randomized trial of a multicomponent home intervention to reduce functional difficulties in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 54, 809–816. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2006.00703.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haase, C. M., Heckhausen, J., & Köller, O. (2008). Goal engagement during the school-work transition: Beneficial for all, particularly for girls. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 18, 671–698. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2008.00576.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haase, C. M., Heckhausen, J., & Silbereisen, R. K. (2012). The interplay of occupational motivation and well-being during the transition from university to work. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1739–1751. doi:10.1037/a0026641.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, N. C., Chipperfield, J. G., Heckhausen, J., & Perry, R. P. (2010). Control striving in older adults with serious health problems: A 9-year longitudinal study of survival, health, and well-being. Psychology and Aging, 25, 432–445. doi:10.1037/a0019278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, N. C., Perry, R. P., Ruthig, J. C., Hladkyj, S., & Chipperfield, J. G. (2006). Primary and secondary control in achievement settings: A longitudinal field study of academic motivation, emotions, and performance. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 1430–1470. doi:10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00067.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamm, J. M., Perry, R. P., Chipperfield, J. G., Stewart, T. L., & Heckhausen, J. (2015). Motivation-focused thinking: Buffering against stress-related physical symptoms and depressive symptomology. Psychology & Health, 30, 1326–1345. doi:10.1080/08870446.2015.1050394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamm, J. M., Perry, R. P., Clifton, R. A., Chipperfield, J. G., & Boese, G. D. (2014). Attributional Retraining: A motivation treatment with differential psychosocial and performance benefits for failure prone individuals in competitive achievement settings. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 36, 221–237. doi:10.1080/01973533.2014.890623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamm, J. M., Stewart, T. L., Perry, R. P., Clifton, R. A., Chipperfield, J. G., & Heckhausen, J. (2013). Sustaining primary control striving for achievement goals during challenging developmental transitions: The role of secondary control strategies. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 35, 286–297. doi:10.1080/01973533.2013.785404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Rozek, C. S., Hulleman, C. S., & Hyde, J. S. (2012). Helping parents to motivation adolescents in mathematics and science: An experimental test of a utility value intervention. Psychological Science, 23, 899–906. doi:10.1177/0956797611435530.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haynes, T. L., Heckhausen, J., Chipperfield, J. G., Perry, R. P., & Newall, N. E. (2009a). Primary and secondary control strategies: Implications for health and well-being among older adults. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 165–197. doi:10.1521/jscp.2009.28.2.165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haynes, T. L., Perry, R. P., Stupnisky, R. H., & Daniels, L. M. (2009b). A review of attributional retraining treatments: Fostering engagement and persistence in vulnerable college students. In J. Smart (Ed.), Higher education: Handbook of theory and research (Vol. 24, pp. 227–272). The Netherlands: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-1-4020-9628-0_6.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Haynes, T. L., Ruthig, J. C., Perry, R. P., Stupnisky, R. H., & Hall, N. C. (2006). Reducing the academic risks of over-optimism: The longitudinal effects of attributional retraining on cognition and achievement. Research in Higher Education, 47, 755–779. doi:10.1007/978-1-4020-9628-0_6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haynes Stewart, T. L., Clifton, R. A., Daniels, L. M., Perry, R. P., Chipperfield, J. G., & Ruthig, J. C. (2011). Attributional retraining: Reducing the likelihood of failure. Social Psychology of Education, 14, 75–92. doi:10.1007/s11218-010-9130-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (1997). Developmental regulation across adulthood: Primary and secondary control of age-related changes. Developmental Psychology, 33, 176–187. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 102, 284–304. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Fleeson, W. (2001). Developmental regulation before and after a developmental deadline: The sample case of “biological clock” for childbearing. Psychology and Aging, 16, 400–413. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.16.3.400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A motivational theory of life-span development. Psychological Review, 117, 32–60. doi:10.1037/a0017668.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hulleman, C. S., & Baron, K. E. (in press). Motivation interventions in education: Bridging theory, research, and practice. L. Corno & E. M. Anderman (Eds.), Handbook of Educational Psychology (3rd Ed.). New York: Routledge, Taylor and Francis.

  • Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2009). Promoting interest and performance in high school science classes. Science, 326, 1410–1412. doi:10.1126/science.1177067.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, I. (2008). Enrollment, persistence, and graduation of in-state students at a public research university: Does high school matter? Research in Higher Education, 49, 776–793. doi:10.1007/s11162-008-9105-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karabenick, S. A., & Urdan, T. C. (Eds.). (2014). Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 18). Bingley, UK: Emerald Publishing. doi:10.1108/S0749-742320140000018000.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathiasen, R. E. (1984). Predicting college academic achievement: A research review. College Student Journal, 18, 380–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morisano, D., Hirsh, J. B., Peterson, J. B., Pihl, R. O., & Shore, B. M. (2010). Setting, elaborating, and reflecting on personal goals improves academic performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 255–264. doi:10.1037/a0018478.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mouw, J. T., & Khanna, R. K. (1993). Prediction of academic success: A review of the literature and some recommendations. College Student Journal, 27, 328–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., Romero, C., Smith, E. N., Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2015). Mind-set interventions are scalable treatments for academic underachievement. Psychological Science, 26, 784–793. doi:10.1177/0956797615571017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2009). Achievement goals and achievement emotions: Testing a model of their joint relations with academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 115–135. doi:10.1037/a0013383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., & Perry, R. P. (2010). Boredom in achievement settings. Exploring control-value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 531–549. doi:10.1037/a0019243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Perry, R. P., Kramer, K., Hochstadt, M., & Molfenter, S. (2004). Beyond test anxiety: Development and validation of the test emotions questionnaire (TEQ). Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 17(3), 287–316. doi:10.1080/10615800412331303847.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, R. P. (1991). Perceived control in college students: Implications for instruction in higher education. In J. Smart (Ed.), Higher education: Handbook of theory and research (Vol. 7, pp. 1–56). New York: Agathon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, R. P. (2003). Perceived (academic) control and causal thinking in achievement settings: Markers and mediators. Canadian Psychologist, 44, 312–331. doi:10.1037/h0086956.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, R. P., Chipperfield, J. G., Hladkyj, S., Pekrun, R., & Hamm, J. M. (2014). Attribution-based treatment interventions in some achievement settings. In S. Karabenick & T. Urdan (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 18). Bingley, UK: Emerald Publishing. doi:10.1108/S0749-742320140000018000.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, R. P., Hall, N. C., & Ruthig, J. C. (2005a). Perceived (academic) control and scholastic attainment in higher education. In J. C. Smart (Ed.), Higher education: Handbook of theory and research (Vol. 20, pp. 363–436). The Netherlands: Springer. doi:10.1007/1-4020-3279-X_7.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, R. P., & Hamm, J. M. (in press). An attribution perspective on competence and motivation: Theory and treatment interventions. In A. Elliot, C. Dweck, & D. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (2nd Edition): Theory and applications. New York: Guilford Press.

  • Perry, R. P., Hladkyj, S., Pekrun, R. H., Clifton, R. A., & Chipperfield, J. G. (2005b). Perceived academic control and failure in college students: A three-year study of scholastic attainment. Research in Higher Education, 46, 535–569. doi:10.1007/s11162-005-3364-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, R. P., Hladkyj, S., Pekrun, R. H., & Pelletier, S. T. (2001). Academic control and action control in the academic achievement of students: A longitudinal field study of self-regulation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 776–789. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.93.4.776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, R. P., & Penner, K. S. (1990). Enhancing academic achievement in college students through attributional retraining and instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 262–271. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.2.262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, R. P., Stupnisky, R. H., Hall, N. C., Chipperfield, J. G., & Weiner, B. (2010). Bad starts and better finishes: Attributional retraining and initial performance in competitive achievement settings. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29, 668–700. doi:10.1521/jscp.2010.29.6.668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poulin, M. J., & Heckhausen, J. (2007). Stressful events compromise control strivings during a major life transition. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 300–311. doi:10.1007/s11031-007-9077-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptomatic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 353–387. doi:10.1037/a0026838.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, S. B., Lauver, K., Le, H., Davis, D., Langley, R., & Carlstrom, A. (2004). Do psychosocial and study skill factors predict college out- comes? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 261–288. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.2.261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, S. B., Oh, I. S., Le, H., & Button, C. (2009). Intervention effects on college performance and retention as mediated by motivational, emotional, and social control factors: Integrated meta-analytic path analyses. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 1163–1184. doi:10.1037/a0015738.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosseel, R. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 1–36. doi:10.18637/jss.v048.i02.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruthig, J. C., Haynes, T. L., Perry, R. P., & Chipperfield, J. G. (2007). Academic optimistic bias: Implications for college student performance and well-being. Social Psychology of Education, 10(1), 115–137. doi:10.1007/s11218-006-9002-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruthig, J. G., Haynes, T. L., Stupnisky, R. H., & Perry, R. P. (2009). Perceived academic control: Mediating the effects of optimism and social support on college students’ psychological health. Social Psychology of Education, 12, 233–249. doi:10.1007/s11218-008-9079-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, T. D., & Dillow, S. A. (2013). Digest of education statistics 2012 (NCES 2014-015). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stupnisky, R. H., Renaud, R. D., Daniels, L. M., Haynes, T. L., & Perry, R. P. (2008). The interrelation of first-year college students’ critical thinking disposition, perceived academic control, and academic achievement. Research in Higher Education, 49(6), 513–530. doi:10.1007/s11162-008-9093-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stupnisky, R. H., Renaud, R. D., Perry, R. P., Ruthig, J. C., Haynes, T. L., & Clifton, R. A. (2007). Comparing self-esteem and perceived control as predictors of first-year college students’ academic achievement. Social Psychology of Education, 10, 303–330. doi:10.1007/s11218-007-9020-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walton, G. M. (2014). The new science of wise psychological interventions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 73–82. doi:10.1177/0963721413512856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2011). A brief social-belonging intervention improves academic and health outcomes of minority students. Science, 331, 1447–1451. doi:10.1126/science.1198364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548–573. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.92.4.548.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (2012). An attribution theory of motivation. In P. Van Lange, A. Kruglanski, & T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeager, D. S., Henderson, M. D., Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., D’Mello, S., Spitzer, B. J., et al. (2014). Boring but important: A self-transcendent purpose for learning fosters academic self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 559–580. doi:10.1037/a0037637.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

This study was based on doctoral dissertation research conducted by the first author. The work was supported by Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) Doctoral and Postdoctoral Fellowships to the first author, a SSHRC Insight Grant [435-2012-1143] and a Royal Society of Canada and Alexander von Humboldt Research Grant to the second author, and SSHRC Insight Grants [410-2010-2049; 435-2016-0970] to the third author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jeremy M. Hamm.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hamm, J.M., Perry, R.P., Chipperfield, J.G. et al. A motivation-enhancing treatment to sustain goal engagement during life course transitions. Motiv Emot 40, 814–829 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9576-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9576-4

Keywords

Navigation