Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Role of music tempo in choosing from large and small choice sets: insights from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous research has concluded that music tempo either increases the total amount of momentary mental resources to benefit decision-making or compulsively employs working memory and impairs decision-making. Two experiments, including a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment and a laboratory experiment, converged on the conclusion that the role of music tempo when making a decision varies with the size of choice set. At the neural level, under a larger choice set, slower music resulted in stronger activation in the superior frontal gyrus (SFG), which has stronger neural coactivation with the hippocampus. In contrast, under the smaller choice set, faster music resulted in stronger activation in the middle frontal gyrus (MFG), which has stronger neural coactivation with the SFG, paracingulate gyrus (PCG), and lateral occipital cortex (LOC). Behaviorally, participants had more positive internal states under slower music than under faster music in a larger choice set. In comparison, faster music resulted in more positive internal states in a smaller choice set. This study is among the first to examine the joint effect of music tempo and choice set size by offering neural imaging evidence with fMRI techniques.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data supporting this study’s findings are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Notes

  1. We did not include a condition without music because we aimed to explore the effect of music tempo on the decision-making process under different choice sets rather than providing an optimal tempo interval. The same basic principles apply to both studies.

  2. In this regard, a high-definition printer was used to print the visual stimuli on A4 paper. Moreover, subjects viewed the visual stimuli on the paper attached to the head coil of the magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner (viewing distance 138 mm). All the images in different choice sets were scaled down to a size of 3.5 cm × 3.5 cm and randomly placed in the middle of the paper. These locations were arranged in six rows and four columns. At the same time, to diminish the global visual difference (brightness, color, and image density) across the sets, a scrambled image was displayed in the remaining locations.

  3. During fMRI scanning, binaural auditory stimuli were presented using custom-built magnet-compatible earphones. The most important feature of this system was high-quality acoustic stimulus delivery during which scanner noise was attenuated. During scanning, the loudness level that participants experienced was approximately 88 dB. After the attenuation of the listening device, auditory stimuli were approximately 68 dB at the ears. During a pretest scan, all the participants individually determined their optimal listening level.

  4. We provided them the option to not make a choice to avoid the negative emotions caused by a forced choice; however, none of the subjects opted not to choose an item.

References

  • Abi-Dargham, A., Mawlawi, O., Lombardo, I., Gil, R., Martinez, D., Huang, Y., Hwang, D. R., Keilp, J., Kochan, L., Van Heertum, R., Gorman, J. M., & Laruelle, M. (2002). Prefrontal Dopamine D1 receptors and working memory in schizophrenia. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(9), 3708–3719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banbury, S. P., Macken, W. J., Tremblay, S., & Jones, D. M. (2001). Auditory distraction and short-term memory: Phenomena and practical implications. Human Factors, 43(1), 12–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barbey, A. K., Koenigs, M., & Grafman, J. (2013). Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex, 49(5), 1195–1205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, B. R. (2010). Competing in tough times: Business lessons from LL bean, Trader Joe’s, Costco, and other world-class retailers (Paperback). FT Press.

  • Bernadine, C., Nanda, N., Vijayalakshmi, R., Thilaga, M., Naga, D., & Nabaraj, D. (2013). Breaking the camel’s back: Can cognitive overload be quantified in the human brain? Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 97, 21–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumenfeld, R. S., & Ranganath, C. (2006). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex promotes long-term memory formation through its role in working memory organization. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(3), 916–925.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boisgueheneuc, FDu., Levy, R., Volle, E., Seassau, M., Duffau, H., Kinkingnehun, S., Samson, Y., Zhang, S., & Dubois, B. (2006). Functions of the left superior frontal gyrus in humans: A lesion study. Brain, 129(12), 3315–3328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, G. C. (1990). Music, mood, and marketing. Journal of Marketing, 54(4), 94–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cao, C. C., & Reimann, M. (2020). Data triangulation in consumer neuroscience: Integrating functional neuroimaging with meta-analyses, psychometrics, and behavioral data. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 550204

  • Chai, W. J., Abd Hamid, A. I., & Abdullah, J. M. (2018). Working memory from the psychological and neurosciences perspectives: A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chandrasekhar, P. V. S., Capra, C. M., Moore, S., Noussair, C., & Berns, G. S. (2008). Neurobiological regret and rejoice functions for aversive outcomes. NeuroImage, 39(3), 1472–1484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chernev, A. (2003). When more is less and less is more: The role of ideal point availability and assortment in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(2), 170–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chernev, A., Böckenholt, U., & Goodman, J. (2015). Choice overload: A conceptual review and meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(2), 333–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, I., Lee, J.-Y., & Lee, S.-H. (2018). Bottom-up and top-down modulation of multisensory integration. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 52, 115–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colle, H. A., & Welsh, A. (1976). Acoustic masking in primary memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15(1), 17–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Courtney, S. M., Petit, L., Maisog, J. M., Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (1998). An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex. Science, 279(5355), 1347–1351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crespo-Facorro, B., Kim, J.-J., Andreasen, N. C., Spinks, R., O’Leary, D. S., Bockholt, H. J., Harris, G., & Magnotta, V. A. (2000). Cerebral cortex: a topographic segmentation method using magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 100(2), 97–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Day, R., Lin, C., Huang, W., & Chuang, S. (2009). Effects of music tempo and task difficulty on multi-attribute decision-making: An eye-tracking approach. Computers in Human Behavior, 25(1), 130–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diehl, K., & Poynor, C. (2010). Great expectations?! Assortment size, expectations, and satisfaction. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(2), 312–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dobbins, I. G., Foley, H., Schacter, D. L., & Wagner, A. D. (2002). Executive control during episodic retrieval: Multiple prefrontal processes subserve source memory. Neuron, 35(5), 989–996.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eroglu, S. A., Machleit, K. A., & Chebat, J. C. (2005). The interaction of retail density and music tempo: Effects on shopper responses. Psychology and Marketing, 22(7), 577–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Etkin, J., & Mogilner, C. (2016). Does variety among activities increase happiness? Journal of Consumer Research, 43(2), 210–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fasolo, B., Hertwig, R., Huber, M., & Ludwig, M. (2009). Size, entropy, and density: What is the difference that makes the difference between small and large real-world assortments? Psychology & Marketing, 26(3), 254–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furnham, A., & Bradley, A. (1997). Music while you work: The differential distraction of background music on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11(5), 445–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furnham, A., & Strbac, L. (2002). Music is as distracting as noise: The differential distraction of background music and noise on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Ergonomics, 45(3), 203–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gard, A. M., Waller, R., Swartz, J. R., Shaw, D. S., Forbes, E. E., & Hyde, L. W. (2018). Amygdala functional connectivity during socioemotional processing prospectively predicts increases in internalizing symptoms in a sample of low-income, urban, young men. NeuroImage, 178, 562–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gasquoine, P. G. (2013). Localization of function in anterior cingulate cortex: From psychosurgery to functional neuroimaging. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(3), 340–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grill-Spector, K., Kourtzi, Z., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition. Vision Research, 41(10–11), 1409–1422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermann, A., Bieber, A., Keck, T., Vaitl, D., & Stark, R. (2014). Brain structural basis of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(9), 1435–1442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hildebrand, D., Rubin, D., Hadi, R., & Kramer, T. (2020). Flavor fatigue: Cognitive depletion influences consumer enjoyment of complex flavors. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 31(1), 103–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. H., Holyoak, K. J., Nisbett, R. E., & Thagard, P. R. (1986). Induction: Processes of inference, learning, and discovery. MIT press.

  • Husain, G., Thompson, W. F., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2002). Effects of musical tempo and mode on arousal, mood, and spatial abilities. Music Perception, 20(2), 151–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Israel, A., Lahav, E., & Ziv, N. (2019). Stop the music? The effect of music on risky financial decisions: An experimental study. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 24, 100231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S. S., Wells, R. E., & Schwartz, B. (2006). Doing better but feeling worse—Looking for the “best’’’ job undermines satisfaction”. Psychological Science, 17(2), 143–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, D. (1999). The cognitive psychology of auditory distraction: The 1997 BPS Broadbent Lecture. British Journal of Psychology, 90(2), 167–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Prentice Hall.

  • Kellaris, J. J., & Kent, R. J. (1994). An exploratory investigation of responses elicited by music varying in tempo, tonality, and texture. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 2(4), 381–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K., & Zauberman, G. (2019). The effect of music tempo on consumer impatience in intertemporal decisions. European Journal of Marketing, 53(3), 504–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klingberg, T. (2009). The overflowing brain: Information overload and the limits of working memory. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koelsch, S. (2020). A coordinate-based meta-analysis of music-evoked emotions. NeuroImage, 223, 117350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krishna, A. (2012). An integrative review of sensory marketing: Engaging the senses to affect perception, judgment and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(3), 332–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ku, Y., Bodner, M., & Zhou, Y. D. (2015). Prefrontal cortex and sensory cortices during working memory: quantity and quality. Neuroscience Bulletin, 31, 175–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Axelrod, B., Yun, L. S., Holmes, A., & Schwartz, G. E. (1998). Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness: Evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(4), 525–535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann, J. A., & Seufert, T. (2017). The influence of background music on learning inthe light of different theoretical perspectives and the role of working memory capacity. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 1902.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linden, D. E. J. (2007). The working memory networks of the human brain. The Neuroscientist, 13(3), 257–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Z., Li, L., Zheng, L., Xu, M., Zhou, F. A., & Guo, X. (2017). Attentional deployment impacts neural response to regret. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 41374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Y., Liu, G., Wei, D., Li, Q., Yuan, G., Wu, S., Wang, G., & Zhao, X. (2018). Effects of musical tempo on musicians’ and non-musicians’ emotional experience when listening to music. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Z., Cheng, X., Liu, S., Zhang, Z., Li, S., Li, L., & Guo, X. (2020). Attentional deployment training impacts neural responses to subsequent regret. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 157(September), 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logie, R. H. (2003). Spatial and visual working memory: A mental workspace. Psychology of Learning and Motivation - Advances in Research and Theory, 42, 37–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luers, P., Schloeffel, M., & Prüssner, J. C. (2020). Working memory performance under stress. Experimental Psychology, 67(2), 132–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A., & Burdette, J. H. (2003). An automated methodfor neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. NeuroImage, 19(3), 1233–1239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mansouri, F. A., Acevedo, N., Illipparampil, R., Fehring, D. J., Fitzgerald, P. B., & Jaberzadeh, S. (2017). Interactive effects of music and prefrontal cortex stimulation in modulating response inhibition. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, K., & Tanaka, K. (2004). Conflict and cognitive control. Science, 303(5660), 969–970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2020). Multimedia learning. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, E. A. (2007). The amygdala, reward and emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(11), 489–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W., Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Petrides, M. (2000). The role of the mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in working memory. Experimental Brain Research, 133(1), 44–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollatos, O., Schandry, R., Auer, D. P., & Kaufmann, C. (2007). Brain structures mediating cardiovascular arousal and interoceptive awareness. Brain Research, 1141(1), 178–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reutskaja, E., Lindner, A., Nagel, R., Andersen, R. A., & Camerer, C. F. (2018). Choice overload reduces neural signatures of choice set value in dorsal striatum and anterior cingulate cortex. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(12), 925–935.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rey, G. D. (2012). A review of research and a meta-analysis of the seductive detail effect. Educational Research Review, 7(3), 216–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, M. P., Strange, B. A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Encoding of emotional memories depends on amygdala and hippocampus and their interactions. Nature Neuroscience, 7(3), 278–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rottschy, C., Langner, R., Dogan, I., Reetz, K., Laird, A. R., Schulz, J. B., Fox, P. T., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2012). Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 60(1), 830–846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salamé, P., & Baddeley, A. (1982). Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech: Implications for the structure of working memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21(2), 150–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salame, P., & Baddeley, A. (1989). Effects of background music on phonological short-term memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 41(1), 107–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schweppe, J., & Knigge, J. (2020). Irrelevant music: How suprasegmental changes of a melody’s tempo and mode affect the disruptive potential of music on serial recall. Memory and Cognition, 48(6), 982–993.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 1657–1661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, K., & Koh, H. E. (2017). Hooked on a feeling: The effect of music tempo on attitudes and the mediating role of consumers’ affective responses. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 16(6), 550–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Su, W., Guo, J., Zhang, Y., Zhou, J., Chen, N., Zhou, M., Li, R., Chen, H., & He, L. (2018). A longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study of working memory in patients following a transient ischemic attack: A preliminary study. Neuroscience Bulletin, 34(6), 963–971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, M. (2022). How many products does Walmart sell? (Mind Blowing). Querysprout. https://querysprout.com/how-many-products-does-walmart-sell/

  • Tomasi, D., Caparelli, E. C., Chang, L., & Ernst, T. (2005). fMRI-acoustic noise alters brain activation during working memory tasks. NeuroImage, 27(2), 377–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wysiadecki, G., Mazurek, A., Walocha, J., Majos, A., Tubbs, R. S., Iwanaga, J., Żytkowski, A., & Radek, M. (2021). Revisiting the morphology and classification of the paracingulate gyrus with commentaries on ambiguous cases. Brain Sciences, 11(7), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, Y., Gao, H., Tong, L., Li, Z. L., Wang, L., Zhang, C., Yang, Q., & Yan, B. (2019). Emotion regulation of hippocampus using real-time fMRI neurofeedback in healthy human. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13(July), 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This work was supported by a grant from the Chinese National Science Foundation (grant number 72172131).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jun Ye.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval

The fMRI study involving human participants was reviewed and approved by the Second Affiliated Hospital, Medical College of Shantou University.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 7715 KB)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhang, K., Liu, H. & Ye, J. Role of music tempo in choosing from large and small choice sets: insights from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Mark Lett 34, 633–652 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-023-09672-9

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-023-09672-9

Keywords

Navigation