Skip to main content
Log in

Stereoscopy in Static Scientific Imagery in an Informal Education Setting: Does It Matter?

  • Published:
Journal of Science Education and Technology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Stereoscopic technology (3D) is rapidly becoming ubiquitous across research, entertainment and informal educational settings. Children of today may grow up never knowing a time when movies, television and video games were not available stereoscopically. Despite this rapid expansion, the field’s understanding of the impact of stereoscopic visualizations on learning is rather limited. Much of the excitement of stereoscopic technology could be due to a novelty effect, which will wear off over time. This study controlled for the novelty factor using a variety of techniques. On the floor of an urban science center, 261 children were shown 12 photographs and visualizations of highly spatial scientific objects and scenes. The images were randomly shown in either traditional (2D) format or in stereoscopic format. The children were asked two questions of each image—one about a spatial property of the image and one about a real-world application of that property. At the end of the test, the child was asked to draw from memory the last image they saw. Results showed no overall significant difference in response to the questions associated with 2D or 3D images. However, children who saw the final slide only in 3D drew more complex representations of the slide than those who did not. Results are discussed through the lenses of cognitive load theory and the effect of novelty on engagement.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aitsiselmi Y, Holliman NS (2009) Using mental rotation to evaluate the benefits of stereoscopic displays. In: IS&T/SPIE electronic imaging. International Society for Optics and Photonics, pp 72370Q–72370Q

  • Apley A, Streitburger K, Scala J (2008) Dinosaurs alive: film summative report submitted to Maryland Science Center. RMC Research Corporation, Portsmouth

    Google Scholar 

  • Barfield W, Rosenberg C (1995) Judgments of azimuth and elevation as a function of monoscopic and binocular depth cues using a perspective display. Human Factors J Human Factors Ergonom Soc 37(1):173–181

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bodner GM, Guay RB (1997) The Purdue visualization of rotations test. Chem Educ 2(4):1–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bombeke K, Van Looy J, Szmalec A, Duyck W (2013) Leaving the third dimension: no measurable evidence for cognitive aftereffects of stereoscopic 3D movies. J Soc Inform Display 21(4):159–166

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown S (2013) From novelty to normal: 3DTV as special effect. Crit Stud Telev Int J Telev Stud 8(3):33–46

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bunzeck N, Düzel E (2006) Absolute coding of stimulus novelty in the human substantia nigra/VTA. Neuron 51(3):369–379

  • Cid XC, Lopez RE (2010) The impact of stereo display on student understanding of phases of the moon. Astron Educ Rev 9(1):010105

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cliburn D, Krantz J (2008) Towards an effective low-cost virtual reality display system for education. J Comput Sci Coll 23(3):147–153

    Google Scholar 

  • Cruz-Neira C, Sandi DJ, DeFanti TA (1993) Surround-screen projection-based virtual reality: the design and implementation of the CAVE. Commun ACM 35(6):64–72

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Winter JCF, Wieringa PA, Dankelman J, Mulder M, Van Paassen MM, De Groot S (2007) Driving simulator fidelity and training effectiveness. In: Proceedings of the 26th European annual conference on human decision making and manual control, Lyngby, Denmark

  • Drascic D (1991) Skill acquisition and task performance in teleoperation using monoscopic and stereoscopic video remote viewing. In: Proceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society annual meeting, vol 35, No. 19. SAGE, pp 1367–1371

  • Dukes P, Bruton D (2008) A Geowall with physics and astronomy applications. Phys Teach 46(3):180–183

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fluke CJ, Bourke PD (2005) Astronomy visualisation in reflection. The Planetarian 34:10–15

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraser J, Heimlich JE, Jacobsen J, Yocco V, Sickler J, Kisiel J, Stahl J (2012) Giant screen film and science learning in museums. Mus Manag Curatorship 27(2):179–195

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurevitch L, Ross M (2013) Stereoscopic media: scholarship beyond booms and busts. Public 24(47):83–93

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen J, Barnett M, MaKinster J, Keating T (2004) The impact of three-dimensional computational modeling on student understanding of astronomical concepts: a quantitative analysis. Int J Sci Educ 26(11):1365–1575

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holford DG, Kempa RF (1970) The effectiveness of stereoscopic viewing in the learning of spatial relationships in structural chemistry. J Res Sci Teach 7(3):265–270

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsu J, Pizlo Z, Babbs CF, Chelberg DM, Delp EJ III (1994) Design of studies to test the effectiveness of stereo imaging truth or dare: is stereo viewing really better? In: IS&T/SPIE 1994 international symposium on electronic imaging: science and technology. International Society for Optics and Photonics, pp 211–222

  • Isik-Ercan Z, Kim B, Nowak J (2012) Can 3D visualization assist in young children’s understanding of Sun–Earth–Moon system? Int J Knowl Soc Res 3(4):12–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keebler JR (2011) Effects of 3D stereoscopy, visuospatial working memory, and perceptions of simulation experience on the memorization of confusable objects. Doctoral dissertation, University of Central Florida Orlando, FL

  • Kennedy C (1936) The development and use of stereo photography for educational purposes. SMPTE Motion Imaging J 26(1):3–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim WS, Ellis SR, Tyler ME, Hannaford B, Stark LW (1987) Quantitative evaluation of perspective and stereoscopic displays in three-axis manual tracking tasks. IEEE Trans Syst Man Cybern 17(1):61–72

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschner PA, Ayres P, Chandler P (2011) Contemporary cognitive load theory research: the good, the bad and the ugly. Comput Hum Behav 27(1):99–105

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kooi FL, Toet A (2004) Visual comfort of binocular and 3D displays. Displays 25(2–3):99–108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kozhevnikov M, Hegarty M, Mayer RE (2002) Revising the visualizer–verbalizer dimension: evidence for two types of visualizers. Cogn Instr 20(1):47–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lambooij M, Fortuin M, Heynderickx I, IJsselsteijn W (2009) Visual discomfort and visual fatigue of stereoscopic displays: a review. J Imaging Sci Technol 53(3):30201-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lantz E (2011) Planetarium of the future. Curator Mus J 54(3):293–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaViola JJ Jr, Litwiller T (2011) Evaluating the benefits of 3d stereo in modern video games. In: Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems. ACM, pp 2345–2354

  • Lo WY, Tsai YP, Chen CW, Hung YP (2004) Stereoscopic kiosk for virtual museum. In: Proceedings of international computer symposium. Symposium conducted at the meeting of Ministry of Education, Republic of China

  • Lopez RE, Hamed K (2004) Student interpretations of 2-D and 3-D renderings of the substorm current wedge. J Atmos Solar Terr Phys 66(15):1509–1517

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer RE (2005) Principles for reducing extraneous processing in multimedia learning: coherence, signaling, redundancy, spatial contiguity, and temporal contiguity principles. In: Mayer RE (ed) Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 183–200

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McIntire JP, Havig PR, Geiselman EE (2012) What is 3D good for? A review of human performance on stereoscopic 3D displays. In: SPIE defense, security, and sensing. International Society for Optics and Photonics, pp 83830X–83830X

  • McIntire JP, Havig PR, Geiselman EE (2014) Stereoscopic 3D displays and human performance: a comprehensive review. Displays 35(1):18–26

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michael WB, Guilford JP, Fruchter B, Zimmerman WS (1957) The description of spatial-visualization abilities. Educ Psychol Measur 17:185–199

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mowafy L, Thurman RA (1993) Training pilots to visualize large-scale spatial relationships in a stereoscopic display. In: IS&T/SPIE’s symposium on electronic imaging: science and technology. International Society for Optics and Photonics, pp 72–81

  • Nataupsky M, Crittenden L (1988) Stereo 3-D and non-stereo presentations of a computer-generated pictorial primary flight display with pathway augmentation

  • National Aeronautics and Space Administration (2000) Into the eye of the storm. Photograph. Retrieved from http://www.jpl.nasa.gov/spaceimages/details.php?id=PIA02636

  • National Research Council (2006) Learning to think spatially: GIS as a support system in the K-12 curriculum. The National Academies Press, Washington

    Google Scholar 

  • Nemire K (1998) Enhancing cockpit design with an immersive virtual environment rapid prototyping and simulation system. In: Aerospace/defense sensing and controls. International Society for Optics and Photonics, pp 112–123

  • Newcombe NS (2010) Picture this: increasing math and science learning by improving spatial thinking. Am Educ 34(2):29

    Google Scholar 

  • Okuyama F (1999) Evaluation of stereoscopic display with visual function and interview. In: Electronic Imaging’99. International Society for Optics and Photonics, pp 28–35

  • Paas F, Tuovinen JE, Tabbers H, Van Gerven PW (2003) Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educ Psychol 38(1):63–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patel M, White M, Walczak K, Sayd P (2003) Digitisation to presentation: building virtual museum exhibitions. In: Vision, video and graphics, Bath, England

  • Patterson R, Silzars A (2009) Immersive stereo displays, intuitive reasoning, and cognitive engineering. J Soc Inform Display 17(5):443–448

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pepper RL, Smith DC, Cole RE (1981) Stereo TV improves operator performance under degraded visibility conditions. Opt Eng 20(4):579–585

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfautz JD (2001) Sampling artifacts in perspective and stereo displays. In: Photonics west 2001-electronic imaging. International Society for Optics and Photonics, pp 54–62

  • Piaget J (1956) Child’s conception of space, vol 4. Routledge, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Pietschmann D, Liebold B, Valtin G, Ohler P (2013) Taking space literally: reconceptualizing the effects of stereoscopic representation on user experience. Italian Journal of Game Studies 2(2)

  • Pölönen M, Järvenpää T, Bilcu B (2013) Stereoscopic 3D entertainment and its effect on viewing comfort: comparison of children and adults. Appl Ergon 44(1):151–160

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price A, Lee HS (2010) The effect of two-dimensional and stereoscopic presentation on middle school students’ performance of spatial cognition tasks. J Sci Educ Technol 19(1):90–103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapp DN, Culpepper SA, Kirkby K, Morin P (2007) Fostering students’ comprehension of topographic maps. J Geosci Educ 55(1):5

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinhart WF (1991) Depth cueing for visual search and cursor positioning. In: Electronic Imaging’91, San Jose, CA. International Society for Optics and Photonics, pp 221–232

  • Roussou M (2000) Immersive interactive virtual reality and informal education. In: Stephanidis C (ed) Proceedings of the ERCIM WG UI4ALL one-day joint workshop with i3 Spring Days 2000 on “Interactive Learning Environments for Children”, Athens, Greece

  • Shipley TF, Epstein R, Newcombe N (2014) Initiative 1: characterize spatial skills relevant to STEM and chart their development. Spatial Intelligence and Learning Center. Resource Document. http://spatiallearning.org/index.php/initiatives/initiative-1-characterize-skills. Accessed 31 March 2014

  • Sweller J (1988) Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cogn Sci 12(2):257–285

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi G, Connolly P (2012) Impact of binocular vision on the perception of geometric shapes in spatial ability testing. In: 67th midyear meeting proceedings, Limerick, Ireland

  • Trindade J, Fiolhais C, Almeida L (2002) Science learning in virtual environments: a descriptive study. British J Educ Technol 33(4):471–488

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trotter D (2004) Stereoscopy: modernism and the ‘haptic’. Crit Q 46(4):38–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Beurden MHPH, IJsselsteijn WA, Juola JF (2012) Effectiveness of stereoscopic displays in medicine: a review. 3D Res 3(1):1–13

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Hoogen W, Feys P, Lamers I, Coninx K, Notelaers S, Kerkhofs L, IJsselsteijn W (2012) Visualizing the third dimension in virtual training environments for neurologically impaired persons: beneficial or disruptive? J Neuroeng Rehabil 9(1):73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenberg SG, Kuse AR (1978) Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Percept Mot Skills 47(2):599–604

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vendeland J, Regenbrecht H (2013) Is there any use in stereoscopic slide presentations? http://www.hci.otago.ac.nz/pubs/2013_VendelandRegenbrecht_CHINZ_2013_StereoPres_submission.pdf. Accessed 31 March 2014

  • Vogt F, Wagner AY (2012) Stereo pairs in astrophysics. Astrophys Space Sci 337(1):79–92

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wai J, Lubinski D, Benbow CP (2009) Spatial ability for STEM domains: aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. J Educ Psychol 101(4):817

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheatstone C (1852) XXXVI. Contributions to the physiology of vision.—Part the first. On the some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision. Lond Edinb Dublin Philos Mag J Sci 3(18):241–267

    Google Scholar 

  • White J (2011) Bumble Bee. Photograph. Retrieved from http://www.flickr.com/photos/kiwizone/6547976295/

  • Williams SP, Parrish RV (1990) New computational control techniques and increased understanding for stereo 3-D displays. In: SC-DL tentative. International Society for Optics and Photonics, pp 73–82

  • Wu HK, Shah P (2004) Exploring visuospatial thinking in chemistry learning. Sci Educ 88(3):465–492

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yim MYC, Cicchirillo VJ, Drumwright ME (2012) The impact of stereoscopic three-dimensional (3-D) advertising. J Advert 41(2):113–128

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was conducted in Living Laboratory® at the Museum of Science, Boston. The project was funded by National Science Foundation award DRL-1114645 and supported by the American Association of Variable Star Observers under the direction of Dr. Arne Henden. We thank Dr. Eric Chaisson, Dr. Janice Gobert, Dr. Maria Roussou, Dr. Holly A. Taylor and Ryan Wyatt for their advice on this project and manuscript. We also thank Justin Harris and Rachel Fyler.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to C. Aaron Price.

Electronic supplementary material

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Price, C.A., Lee, HS. & Malatesta, K. Stereoscopy in Static Scientific Imagery in an Informal Education Setting: Does It Matter?. J Sci Educ Technol 23, 721–734 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-014-9500-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-014-9500-1

Keywords

Navigation