Introduction

Grammatical gender is an abstract feature inherent to the noun that is present in approximately 44% of all languages of the world (Corbett, 2013). According to the standard definitions of gender (Corbett, 1991; Hockett, 1958), the feature is only visible as agreement on other targets, such as determiners or adjectives. Furthermore, gender assignment is often non-transparent, with few (or no) links between the gender of a noun and both its meaning and in many cases the morphophonological shape of the noun. Given this complexity, grammatical gender has proven to be a fruitful area for research in multiple fields of linguistics. From a psycholinguistic perspective, such research has examined the representation and processing of gender in the lexicon across numerous child and adult populations and from both L1 and L2 perspectives (e.g. Brouwera et al., 2017; Dussias et al., 2013; Lemmerth & Hopp, 2019; Sagarra & Herschensohn, 2010). Representational studies examining a variety of different languages (for a review, see Klassen, 2016; Sá-Leite et al., 2019) have consistently treated native adult speakers of each language as a homogeneous group. This can be problematic, for the following reasons: (1) There are numerous linguistic contexts in which language variation clearly affects the use of grammatical gender, and (2) not allowing for the possibility that native speakers have different representations even in their L1(s) also runs counter to recent findings in bilingualism research regarding the impact of individual differences (e.g. Dörnyei, 2014).

In spite of this keen interest in grammatical gender across different populations and fields of linguistics, there are still many aspects of this feature that are under debate. By changing the perspective on L1 groups, we aim to elucidate the representation of gender systems in the context of language variation, contributing important novel evidence to the field of lexical access.

Previous Research

Representation of Gender in the Lexicon

Models of lexical access, including both spoken word production and visual word recognition, is an area that enters into the debate on grammatical gender. The level of representation of grammatical gender, the context in which gender information becomes available as well as the mechanisms by which it is selected is not only an area of disagreement across models, but in some cases is merely discussed as a side note.

Language production models such as WEAVER++ (Levelt, 1993; Levelt et al., 1999; Roelofs, 1992, 1997), the interactive two-step model (Dell et al., 1997) and the interactive activation model (McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981; Rumelhart & McClelland, 1982) of word recognition posit that grammatical gender is represented as a feature of the lemma, or syntactic word, which is accessed prior to morphonological information in the case of models such as WEAVER++ that assume discrete processing. In contrast, in the independent network model of language production (Caramazza, 1997), gender information belongs to a syntactic feature network that is independent of the networks storing lexical-semantic and morphophonological information.

Regardless of the level of the feature, most models seem to converge on the notion that gender information is represented as a set of nodes (or determiner forms in the case of the independent network model) to which the relevant lexical entries are linked. An example of the representation for German is shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Representation of the German gender system in the lexicon

L1 Lexical Access Research

Evidence regarding the representation of grammatical gender in the mental lexicon was first offered by Schriefers (1993) for L1 Dutch speakers. In this study, native Dutch speakers performed two L1 picture-word interference tasks in which they were asked to name the picture while ignoring the written distractor word. The stimuli consisted of pictures and words with either the same gender (gender-congruent condition) or a different gender (gender-incongruent condition) and the results showed significantly faster reaction times (RTs) for gender-congruent picture-distractor pairs than for gender-incongruent ones, an effect which Schriefers labelled the gender congruency effect.

Since this initial study, the effect of gender congruency has been investigated using the picture-word interference paradigm in numerous L1 studies, with the findings diverging even within the same language groups (Table 1). We are not aware of any previous studies that have investigated the effect of L1 grammatical gender variation on the representation in the lexicon, despite the relevance of considering L1 individual differences. In the present study, we investigate the representation and processing of grammatical gender in Norwegian by L1 Norwegian speakers. Norwegian is an ideal linguistic context for such a study, given the diversity among the many dialects spoken in Norway as well as the ongoing change to the Norwegian gender system that has recently been attested. Specifically, we ask (i) how L1 dialectal variation affects the representation of grammatical gender in the lexicon; and (ii) whether there are fundamentally different representations of L1 grammatical gender, and if so, how these representations are borne out in language processing. In the remainder of the paper, we offer a summary of gender in Norwegian ("Gender in Norwegian" section), details on the participants and design of the present study ("The present study" section), the results ("Results" section), a discussion ("Discussion" section) and conclusions ("Conclusion" section).

Table 1 Summary of findings from L1 gender studies

Gender in Norwegian

The Norwegian Gender System

While some Germanic languages, notably Dutch, Swedish and Danish, have undergone a historical change from a tripartite system to a binary one (common and neuter), Norwegian dialects have generally retained masculine, feminine and neuter.Footnote 1 The masculine and feminine gender paradigms are very similar, and this syncretism has only increased over the last 400–500 years, while the neuter paradigm has kept or developed unique exponents. The traditional three-gender system of most varieties of spoken Norwegian is illustrated in Table 2. Gender is marked on the indefinite article (en, ei or et, for masculine, feminine or neuter respectively) and the definite suffix, as well as adjectives in the strong declension (indefinite), possessives, and the prenominal determiner in double definite forms (required with modified definite nouns).

Table 2 The traditional three-gender system in many varieties of spoken Norwegian

Due to the extensive syncretism between the masculine and the feminine gender shown in Table 2, the difference between the masculine and the feminine is only visible in two forms: the indefinite article and the possessive. Neuter has its own forms for strong adjectives and prenominal definite articles in addition to the possessive and indefinite article form.

With respect to written Norwegian, there are two standards: bokmål and nynorsk. While nynorsk (based on the dialects) requires the use of all three genders for the indefinite article and the possessive, bokmål (based on Danish) may be used with either a three- or a two-gender system.Footnote 2Bokmål is by far the more widely used written standard, as most books, magazines, newspapers and subtitles are written in this variety (Lundquist & Vangsnes, 2018; Vangsnes et al., 2017), and only 12.2% of all school children in Norway have nynorsk as their main written language (Vangsnes et al., 2017). In most bokmål media and books, the feminine exponents are rarely used—especially not as indefinite articles—meaning that dialect speakers are constantly exposed to a two-gender system.

In recent years, changes in the gender system have been reported in a number of dialects. Based on a spoken corpus of 142 speakers in Oslo, Lødrup (2011) shows that the feminine indefinite article is used infrequently among older speakers and is virtually non-existent in the production of the younger generation. A similar change is attested in two other major cities, Tromsø and Trondheim (Busterud et al., 2019; Rodina & Westergaard, 2015). In all these instances, the feminine indefinite article ei is replaced by masculine en, while the definite suffix -a is retained in its original form; thus, the emerging two-gender system has the pattern en bok-boka ‘a book-the book’ for previously feminine nouns.

It is relevant in the context of this study to note that gender assignment in Norwegian is generally non-transparent. According to Trosterud (2001), who has based his calculations on 31,500 nouns in the Nynorsk Dictionary, masculine nouns make up 52%, feminine nouns 32% and neuter nouns only 16%. By Rodina and Westergaard’s (2015) count in a corpus of child-directed speech (adult Tromsø speakers with a three-gender system), the token frequency of masculine nouns in the input is even higher—62.6%—while feminine and neuter nouns are at 18.9% and 18.5%, respectively. This means that in a two-gender system, the common gender (resulting from the convergence of masculine and feminine) will be significantly more frequent than the neuter, at around 80% (similar to Dutch).

Gender Processing in Norwegian

In a series of studies, Lundquist and colleagues (Lundquist & Vangsnes, 2018; Lundquist et al., 2016) examined whether dialectal differences between Norwegian spoken in the Northern, Western, and Eastern regions affect adult L1 speakers’ predictive use of gender in auditory processing. The results showed that gender prediction did vary across dialects, and that speakers from these regions differed significantly in their predictive use (or lack thereof) of masculine and feminine. L1 speakers from Western Norway (nynorsk area) consistently used gender predictively for masculine, feminine, and neuter nouns. In contrast, in Northern Norway (bokmål area), even speakers who used three genders in production only used neuter in predictive processing. Those speakers in the North who only produced two genders (masculine and neuter) used both of them in prediction, unlike speakers from Eastern Norway who also had a binary gender system though only used neuter predictively. Together, these findings clearly show that L1 speakers of different Norwegian dialects use gender differently in processing, and suggest that such variation may also affect the representation of gender information in the lexicon of L1 Norwegian speakers. The present study explores the gender representation that underlies these effects seen in predictive processing.

The Present Study

Participants

A total of 239 L1 Norwegian adults from across Norway participated in this study (M age = 39 (range 15–75), SD = 15; 88 males, 151 females). Participants were recruited from across Norway, as indicated by the geographical region of the city or town of their self-reported dialect: Eastern Norway (N = 66), Western Norway (N = 37), Mid-Norway (N = 31) and Northern Norway (N = 76). While it was not possible to recruit an even regional distribution of speakers due to the constraints of web-based data collection, importantly, the results we obtain are in accordance with previously found correlations between gender systems, age and dialects.Footnote 3 Ninety-nine participants (41%) reported intermediate proficiency in a language with grammatical gender that learned after the age of 5 [German (N = 45), French (N = 21), Spanish (N = 18) or other gendered languages (N = 15)]. In order to mitigate any possible cross-linguistic influence resulting from additional language knowledge on the data, an accuracy threshold was established (further described in "Materials and Response accuracy" sections).

Method

The participants performed a gender decision task in which they were presented with bare nouns in Norwegian and asked to select the corresponding indefinite article (enM, eiF or etN) as quickly and as accurately as possible. Importantly, the instructions emphasized to the speakers that there were no correct answers and that they should answer according to their own dialect. In order to train the participants on the task and the use of three response keys (N for en, I for ei, and T for et), a short practice session preceded the experimental stimuli. Throughout the task, each noun (randomized by the software) appeared on the screen until the participant responded, or for a maximum of 3500 ms. The entire experiment (including a short language background questionnaire) took approximately 5 min to complete.

Data collection was conducted via Ibex Farm (spellout.net/ibexfarm), a web platform that hosts online experiments in a browser. For each stimulus, Ibex Farm recorded the response key pushed as well as the reaction time (RT). Participants were recruited through social media or email and completed the experiment at home using their private computers, allowing for a much wider recruitment of participants. While not a conventional lab setting, the possible variation in the data resulting from the use of different computers was minimized through the design of the platform itself (i.e., the entire experiment is loaded on the individual computer prior to the start of the experiment such that RT differences between experimental conditions are independent of computer processing and internet connection speeds) and by recruiting a substantial number of participants.

Materials

The task consisted of 108 Norwegian nouns, 12 practice and 96 experimental stimuli (see “Appendix A”). These inanimate, count nouns were divided equally into conditions based on the traditional gender of the noun (i.e., 36 masculine, 36 feminine and 36 neuter). The stimuli were reviewed by two native speakers of Norwegian, one from Northern Norway (bokmål area) and one from Western Norway (nynorsk area) in order to avoid specific nouns that varied in either form or gender across different dialects.Footnote 4 There were no significant differences across gender conditions in mean number of letters (p = 0.354), number of syllables (p = 0.560), or mean word frequency by web (p = 0.998; TenTen, Jakubíček et al., 2013) or subtitle measures (p = 0.311; Opus2, Tiedemann, 2012).

Results

Response Accuracy

Overall, the participants chose the correct gender (defined as the traditional gender of the noun) in 85% of the trials. As regards the three gender conditions, participants performed at ceiling for masculine (masc; 95%) and neuter (neut; 96%), while—unsurprisingly in light of the language changes reported in section the Norwegian gender system—they showed more variation in the feminine condition (fem; 63%). A very small subset of the participants scored below 80% on the masculine and neuter conditions (N = 12) and were not included in the analyses, as this suggested that they were either significantly affected by cross-linguistic influence from proficiency in additional languages or that they did not use the response buttons according to the task instructions.

Responses other than the feminine indefinite determiner (ei) for nouns in the fem condition were not evenly distributed among the participants: some participants never used eiF, others used it consistently, while others still used it to varying degrees. Given that both previous research as well as the current dataset show that feminine is the locus of variation in Norwegian gender, participants were divided into groups based on their responses in the fem condition. Participants who used eiF with 0–2 feminine nouns (maximum of 6%)Footnote 5 were classified as the two-gender group (N = 46)Footnote 6; participants who used eiF with 29–31 feminine nouns (minimum of 94%) were put into the three-gender group (N = 92); and all participants who displayed more variation in their use of eiF (3–28 nouns, 10–90%) formed the unstable gender group (N = 89).

In light of the ongoing changes in the Norwegian gender system (Sect. the Norwegian gender system), it is probable that these groups reflect a representative sample of Norwegian gender use. That is, the largest group of speakers still has a stable three-gender system, significantly fewer speakers have already developed a stable two-gender system, and another large group has an unstable system as it transitions from three to two-genders. Figure 2 illustrates these three speaker groups, showing the distribution of participants according to the number of eiF responses. As can be seen in the figure, there is considerable variation within the unstable group: some of the speakers seem to have an almost intact three-gender system, while others rarely use the feminine at all. Still, we choose to treat this as one group characterized by a competition between feminine and masculine exponents for the traditionally feminine nouns. Note that it is always the masculine en determiner (rather than neuter et) that is used in the fem condition when feminine ei is not selected.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Participant groups according to the number of feminine determiner responses

Reaction Times

To analyze the RT data, we fit a series of mixed effects models to estimate the effects of Group (two-gender, three-gender, unstable) and Condition (masc, fem, neut), as well as the interaction between Group and Condition, using the lme4 package in R (Bates et al., 2015). Our dependent measure is the log-transformed RTs and the models include random intercepts for participant and stimulus, as well as a by-participant slope for Condition. We obtain p-values for the main effects and interactions from a series of likelihood ratio tests (using the anova function in lme4) and from pairwise comparisons between groups and conditions using the emmeans package (Lenth, 2021). The full regression table is given in “Appendix B”.

Results show a main effect of Condition (χ2(2) = 9.6, p < 0.01) and Group (χ2(2) = 25.5, p < 0.001), as well as an interaction between Condition and Group (χ2(4) = 67.4, p < 0.001). With respect to Condition, neut is overall faster than both fem and masc (both ps < 0.01), with no significant difference between fem and masc. In terms of Group, the two-gender group is overall faster than the unstable and three-gender groups (both ps < 0.001), and there is no significant difference between the unstable and the three-gender groups. RT results broken down by group are shown in Fig. 3.

Fig. 3
figure 3

RTs in each condition by group. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals

To determine the locus of the interactions for Group and Condition, we consider the three participant groups separately and do pairwise comparisons for the three gender conditions for each group (values extracted by the emmeans package from the full model specified above, statistics for all the pairwise comparisons within each group given in “Appendix C”). For the two-gender group, there is no significant difference in RTs by condition. Data from this group illustrate that none of the conditions are inherently more difficult, and thus the differences across conditions in the unstable and three-gender groups are presumably not triggered by general problems of lexical access. In the unstable group, the neut condition is faster than the other two conditions (both ps < 0.001), with no significant difference between fem and masc. Finally, in the three-gender group, the masc condition is significantly slower than fem and neut (both ps < 0.001), while we see no difference between fem and neut.

Frequency-Driven Effects

Having established that there is significant variation among the participants with respect to the fem condition—both in terms of the number of eiF responses ("response accuracy" section) and RTs ("reaction times" section)—we now examine how the frequency of feminine exponents in the input may have affected the responses and RTs in the fem condition.Footnote 7 To this end, the frequencies of the lemma (e.g., bok, ‘book’), the definite feminine suffix -a (e.g., boka, ‘the book’) and the feminine indefinite determiner eiF (e.g., ei bok, ‘a book’) for each of the feminine noun stimuli are considered in "frequency-driven effects" section. Frequencies are based on the NoWaC corpus (Guevara, 2010).Footnote 8

Frequency Effects on the Choice of ei F

To start, we examine the relationship between input frequency and participants’ use of the feminine determiner with traditionally feminine nouns. We focus exclusively on the unstable group, as there was little to no variation in the response data for the two-gender and three-gender groups.

The rate at which the unstable group used eiF in the fem condition varied considerably according to the individual stimuli, from 40% (skulder, ‘shoulder’) to 84% (bygd, ‘village’). With respect to the different frequency measures acquired from the NoWaC corpus, we find no effect of the absolute feminine noun frequency by lemma (e.g., bok), definite form (e.g., boka/boken), or indefinite form (e.g., ei/en bok) (all ps > 0.5). Rather, the best predictor for the unstable group’s use of eiF is the proportion of definite feminine forms (e.g., bok-aF vs. bok-enM) with which each stimulus appears in the written corpus (β = − 2.38, se = 0.49, z = − 4.87, p < 0.001). That is, the more often a feminine noun appears with the masculine definite suffix -en, the more likely the participants are to use the masculine indefinite determiner en in the experiment. The relation between enM/eiF responses in the fem condition in the experiment and the proportion of masculine definite endings in the corpus are shown in Fig. 4, with all the fem condition stimuli listed along the X axis by lowest to highest proportion of en vs. ei responses.Footnote 9

Fig. 4
figure 4

The proportion of en vs. ei responses in the experiment and -en vs. -a suffixed forms in the corpus data

Effect of masc/fem Proportions on RTs

In this section, we consider whether the proportion of the masculine definite -en suffix affects RT results in the two-gender, three-gender and unstable groups differently. Comparing the effects of the absolute frequencies (lemmas, definite form, indefinite form), the frequency of the unambiguous feminine forms (ei, suffix -a), and the proportion of masculine vs. feminine forms (e.g., en bok + boken vs. ei bok + boka), we find no effect of the absolute frequency measures on RTs, and only small effects of the frequency of unambiguous feminine forms, either as main effects or interactions with Group.

As regards the RTs, the best predictor is the proportion of masculine vs. feminine forms. We only focus on the indefinite article, i.e., the proportion of the masculine indefinite article en for the feminine nouns. We find that the effect of the indefinite determiner only emerges as an interaction with Group (χ2(2) = 10.57, p < 0.01, statistics from likelihood ratio tests, with random intercepts for Participant and Item, and in addition total frequency (log-transformed) as a fixed effect). The three-gender group responds more slowly to feminine nouns that frequently appear with the masculine determiner (β = 0.38, se = 0.18, t = 2.077), while the two-gender group is unaffected by the proportion of enM vs. eiF. This interaction is illustrated in Fig. 5, see “Appendix D” for the full regression table.

Fig. 5
figure 5

The relation between the log RTs and the proportion of enM vs. eiF used with traditionally fem nouns in the corpus

Based on the number of eiF responses it seems that the three-gender group has a stable tripartite gender system, though they are in fact the group whose RTs are most highly affected by the proportion of masculine vs. feminine determiners in their input. These additional insights from the RT data seem to suggest that the three-gender group’s system is not as stable as their use of the feminine form eiF indicates.

The unstable group’s RTs are also affected by the proportion of enM vs. eiF, though surprisingly to a lesser extent than the three-gender group (again, see “Appendix D” for model estimates). As we saw in "frequency effects on the choice of eiF" section, the unstable group’s number of eiF responses is further affected by the proportion of -en vs. -a definite suffixes in the input, and thus it seems as though the unstable group is aptly named in that they do indeed have an unstable gender system.

In contrast to the other groups, the two-gender group is unaffected by the variation between masculine and feminine forms (neither indefinite determiners nor definite suffixes) in the input.

Frequency Effects on Masculine and Neuter Nouns

We have established that the three groups differ in their response patterns and their RTs, with certain frequency measures for the nouns in the fem condition affecting response patterns to a greater or lesser extent in each group. We now turn our attention to possible frequency effects on the stimuli in the masc and neut conditions, as well as possible interactions with Group. To test this, we compare models with a number of predictors (Condition, Group, and the three frequency measures described in "effect of masc/fem proportions on RTs" section). We find no interaction between Group and Frequency (by any measure), nor any interaction between Condition and Frequency. When comparing the different frequency values (lemma, definite forms, indefinite forms) we only find an effect of the frequency of the indefinite article (χ2 = 6.3, df = 1, p < 0.05), which is not unexpected given that the task asked participants to choose an indefinite determiner form.Footnote 10

Discussion

The data from the gender decision task show that L1 Norwegian speakers are generally consistent in their use of the masculine determiner with masculine nouns and the neuter determiner with neuter nouns (rates of 96% and 97%, respectively). Furthermore, their RTs are unaffected by the frequency of gender-marked forms in their input,Footnote 11 suggesting that masculine and neuter have a stable representation in the gender system of all native speakers of Norwegian. This is in stark contrast to the use of the feminine determiner with traditionally feminine nouns, where the speakers show three distinct patterns: no use of feminine (two-gender group), consistent use of feminine (three-gender group), or inconsistent use of feminine (unstable group). As the focus of the present study is gender variation in L1 speakers, in the remainder of the discussion we focus on the data for feminine nouns, addressing each speaker group in turn.

Three-Gender Group

The response data of the three-gender group show consistent use of the feminine determiner (with a maximum of 6% variation; Fig. 2). From a language use perspective, it is clear that these speakers have a gender system clearly consisting of masculine, feminine and neuter. The RT data, however, suggest that the underlying representation of this tripartite gender system may not be as stable as the language use indicates, as these speakers were the most affected by variation in masculine vs. feminine determiners in the input (Fig. 5). The RTs for this group further show that responses for masculine nouns are significantly slower than for both feminine and neuter nouns (Fig. 3). These are not the first findings pointing to longer RTs for masculine nouns in a language a with tripartite gender system: Opitz and Pechmann (2016) report a series of online experiments with L1 German speakers who consistently display longer RTs for masculine nouns in German. These authors offer a theoretical account involving underspecification of gender features which is beyond the scope of the current study, and we are unaware of any existing psycholinguistic explanation for the apparent processing costs borne by masculine nouns. Though the design of our current study does not allow us to propose such an account, this is relevant avenue for future research as findings in this area will undoubtedly further our understanding of gender in lexical access.

From a representational perspective, it seems as though this group of Norwegian speakers have a gender representation much like that shown in Fig. 1 for German. In the lexicon, masculine nouns are linked to the masculine gender node, feminine nouns to the feminine node, and neuter nouns to the neuter gender node. The links from the masculine and neuter nouns to their respective nodes are stable, while the interaction found between input frequencies and RTs suggest that the links between the feminine nouns and the feminine node are destabilizing (Fig. 6). Furthermore, if this destabilization process continues on the same trajectory, it is likely that three-gender group speakers would join the unstable group, and eventually also the two-gender group.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Representations of the L1 Norwegian gender system in the lexicon across the three speaker groups

Two-Gender Group

Like the three-gender group, the two-gender group is also consistent in their gender use, though in this case they do not use the feminine determiner (with a maximum of 6% variation; Fig. 2). In contrast to the three-gender group, however, RT data support the notion that these speakers have a stable gender system, as they are unaffected by the masculine/feminine variation in their input (Fig. 5). They also display a different pattern of RT results than the previous group, with mean RTs for nouns of each gender being statistically equal.

These results suggest that this group of speakers has a stable bipartite representation of gender in Norwegian. This two-gender representation has links between the masculine and neuter nouns and their respective nodes; however, in this case the traditionally feminine nouns are linked to the masculine gender node. This link between feminine nouns and the masculine node appears to be well-established and stable (Fig. 6).

Unstable Gender Group

The unstable gender group is characterized by a wide variation in their use of the feminine determiner (between 10 and 90%; Fig. 2). Not only was their feminine use inconsistent, but it was also significantly affected by the variation between the masculine definite suffix -en and the feminine definite suffix -a in the input (Fig. 4). In a similar manner, this group’s RTs were affected by the variation between the masculine and feminine indefinite determiners (Fig. 5). They further differed from both of the other two groups in the pattern of their RTs: responses for neuter were significantly faster than both masculine and feminine nouns, which did not differ statistically. This arguably reflects the fact that neuter is the only representation unaffected by the instability of their gender system. While the use of masculine is stable, its representation is affected by the increasing use of masculine exponents with traditionally feminine nouns.

Given that speakers in this group do use feminine gender, it seems that they still do have a representation for feminine. However, considerable instability comes from the fact that feminine nouns are becoming increasingly linked to the masculine gender node, as these speakers’ systems move towards the complete loss of feminine (as in the two-gender group). The variation in the use of feminine exponents with each noun as seen in the corpus data (Fig. 4), together with the variation in feminine use across speakers in this group, likely means that the degree to which nouns are linked to the feminine and/or masculine nodes vary both in terms of the specific lexical item as well as across speakers. In some cases, links between traditionally feminine nouns and the feminine node would be the strongest, in other cases, links to the masculine node would be stronger, and in others still the links would be approximately equal as specific nouns and speakers move through this process of change in gender in Norwegian. The inconsistent nature of these links with traditionally feminine nouns results in processing delays due to increased competition between the masculine and feminine gender nodes that must be resolved prior to the selection of gender information.

Representation of L1 Gender Variation in the Lexicon

In light of the unique RT patterns for each of the speaker groups (Fig. 3), we have proposed three different representations of the Norwegian gender system which are illustrated in Fig. 6.

We therefore submit that it is indeed possible for different L1 speakers to have different underlying representations of grammatical gender in the lexicon in L1 variation contexts. Furthermore, these representations affect language processing differently: for speakers with a tripartite gender system, responses for masculine nouns show the highest processing costs; those with an unstable gender system display processing costs with both masculine and feminine gender; and speakers who have a binary system show no significant processing costs by gender at all. The results from the unstable gender group in particular leave questions open for further research, including the extent to which the instability in the gender system is due to specific lexical items (i.e., if speakers consistently use masculine or feminine determiners depending on the noun) and whether their behaviour would differ in gender assignment vs. gender agreement contexts.Footnote 12

These findings have important implications beyond Norwegian, and will hopefully propel forward further work on the representation of gender information in language variation. There are many contexts in which gender variation has been documented but not examined with respect to the implications for lexical access (e.g. Danish and Swedish, Josefsson, 2014). In a few cases, studies have offered important evidence regarding gender in child bidialectal acquisition (e.g. Venetian and Italian: Kupisch & Klaschik, 2017; Dutch and dialects of Dutch in the Netherlands: Cornips & Hulk, 2008), leaving the representational perspective as a fruitful avenue for future work.

Another important contribution of the present study regards the traditional assumption in linguistic research that all speakers with a similar linguistic profile form a homogeneous group. Our results clearly show that speakers with the same L1 should not necessarily be treated as a single group, though this is perhaps less surprising given that it is well-known that there is significant gender variation in Norwegian. It is, however, also relevant to note that individual differences between speakers are increasingly shown to be important, particularly in the context of bilinguals (e.g. Bayram et al., 2017; Dörnyei, 2014; Flores & Rinke, 2020), though this is arguably also relevant for L1 speakers (e.g. Dąbrowska, 2012). Outside of sociolinguistic factors such as socioeconomic status and education level as well as popular psycholinguistic measures like working memory capacity, individual differences are not often considered at the level of language representation in lexical access. In this same vein, future psycholinguistic studies should consider speakers’ gender representations on an individual basis, along the lines of the gender use strategy analyses in both child and adult offline code-switching data (e.g. Cantone & Müller, 2005; Klassen, 2016).

Conclusion

This study has offered novel evidence for representational differences in the lexicon of L1 speakers of Norwegian, showing not only that speakers with the same L1 can have different underlying representations for grammatical gender, but also that these representations have different consequences in language processing. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the impact of gender variation on lexical access in the L1, and the results suggest that this should be an important area for further research, investigating other contexts of language variation, as well as different age groups.