Skip to main content
Log in

The Effects of Phonological Short-Term Memory and Speech Perception on Spoken Sentence Comprehension in Children: Simulating Deficits in an Experimental Design

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The roles of phonological short-term memory (pSTM) and speech perception in spoken sentence comprehension were examined in an experimental design. Deficits in pSTM and speech perception were simulated through task demands while typically-developing children (N \(=\) 71) completed a sentence-picture matching task. Children performed the control, simulated pSTM deficit, simulated speech perception deficit, or simulated double deficit condition. On long sentences, the double deficit group had lower scores than the control and speech perception deficit groups, and the pSTM deficit group had lower scores than the control group and marginally lower scores than the speech perception deficit group. The pSTM and speech perception groups performed similarly to groups with real deficits in these areas, who completed the control condition. Overall, scores were lowest on noncanonical long sentences. Results show pSTM has a greater effect than speech perception on sentence comprehension, at least in the tasks employed here.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. There are multiple theories with different explanations of how working memory and linguistic knowledge are organized (e.g. MacDonald and Christiansen 2002; Waters and Caplan 1996). The focus of the current study is on phonological short-term memory rather than the broader construct of working memory.

References

  • Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition: A comparison of tests. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(5), 970–983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 47–89). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bird, J., Bishop, D. V. M., & Freeman, N. H. (1995). Phonological awareness and literacy development in children with expressive phonological impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 446–462.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Uncommon understanding. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioral marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 391–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2001). Non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communicative Disorders, 36(4), 421–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botting, N., Riches, N., Gaynor, M., & Morgan, G. (2010). Gesture production and comprehension in children with specific language impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 51–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botting, N., Simkin, Z., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006). Associated reading skills in children with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). Reading and Writing, 19, 77–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Adolf, S. M., Hogan, T. P., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2005). Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1378–1396.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • de Bree, E. H., Rispens, J. E., & Gerrits, E. (2007). Non-word repetition in Dutch children with (a risk of) dyslexia and SLI. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21(11–12), 935–944.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dick, F., Bates, E., Wulfeck, J., Utman, J., Dronkers, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2001). Language deficits, localization, and grammar: Evidence for a distributive model of language breakdown in aphasic patients and neurologically intact individuals. Psychological Review, 108(4), 759–788.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171(3968), 303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, H. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fortunato-Traveres, T., de Andrade, C. R. F., Befi-Lopes, D. M., Epstein, B., Tornyoa, L. B., & Schwartz, R. B. (2012). Syntactic structural alignment in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 1097–1111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerrits, E. (2003). Speech perception of children at risk for dyslexia and children with specific language impairment. In M. J. Sole, D. Recasens, & J. Romero (Eds.). Proceedings of the 15th international conference on phonetic sciences (pp. 2357–2360).

  • Gerrits, E., & de Bree, E. H. (2009). Early language development of children at familial risk of dyslexia: Speech perception and production. Journal of Communication Disorders, 42, 180–194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goulandris, N. K., Snowling, M. J., & Walker, I. (2000). Is dyslexia a form of specific language impairment? A comparison of dyslexia and language impaired children as adolescents. Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 103–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayiou-Thomas, M., Bishop, D. V. M., & Plunkett, K. (2004). Simulating SLI: General cognitive processing stressors can produce a specific linguistic profile. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(6), 1347–1362.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joanisse, M. F., Manis, F. R., Keating, P., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2000). Language deficits in dyslexic children: Speech perception, phonology, and morphology. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 30–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joanisse, M. F., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1998). Specific language impairment in children: An impairment in grammar or processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(7), 240–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99(1), 122–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman brief intelligence test (2nd ed.). Bloomington, MN: Pearson Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klatt, D. H. (1980). Software for a cascade/parallel formant synthesizer. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 67(3), 971–995.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S., & Griffith, B. C. (1957). The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(5), 358–368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loucas, T., Riches, N. G., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Chandler, S., et al. (2010). Speech perception and phonological short-term memory capacity in language impairment: Preliminary evidence from adolescents with specific language impairment (SLI) and autism spectrum disorders (ASD). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 45(3), 275–286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, M. C., & Christiansen, M. H. (2002). Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109, 35–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marton, K., & Schwartz, R. G. (2003). Working memory capacity and language processes in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1138–1153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McDonald, J. L. (2006). Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgment performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(3), 381–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, J. W. (1995). Sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: The role of phonological working memory. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 187–199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, J. W., & Evans, J. L. (2009). Complex sentence comprehension and working memory in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2), 269–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morrill, T. H., Dilley, L. C., McAuley, D., & Pitt, M. A. (2014). Distal rhythm influences whether or not listeners hear a word in continuous speech: Support for a perceptual grouping hypothesis. Cognition, 131(1), 69–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, S., & Robertson, E. K. (2015). A closer look at phonology as a predictor of spoken sentence processing and reading. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 33(4), 399–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pizzioli, F., & Schelstraete, M. (2011). Children with specific language impairment: The effect of argument-structure complexity on auditory sentence comprehension. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 25(1), 1–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaza, M., & Cohen, H. (2003). The interaction between phonological processing, syntactic awareness, and naming speed in the reading and spelling performance of first-grade children. Brain and Cognition, 53, 287–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rispens, J. E., & Been, P. (2007). Subject-verb agreement and phonological processing in developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment (SLI): A closer look. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 42(3), 293–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rispens, J. E., Roeleven, S., & Koster, C. (2004). Sensitivity to subject-verb agreement in spoken language in children with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 333–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, E. K. (2008). The relationship between dyslexia and specific language impairment: multiple phonological processing deficits. Dissertation Abstracts International, Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 69(6-B), 3870.

  • Robertson, E. K., & Joanisse, M. F. (2010). Spoken sentence comprehension in children with dyslexia and language impairment: The roles of syntax and working memory. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 141–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, E. K., Joanisse, M. F., Desroches, A. S., & Ng, S. (2009). Categorical speech perception deficits distinguish language and reading impairments in children. Developmental Science, 12(5), 753–767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E. M., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shafiro, V., & Raphael, L. J. (2007). Phonetic interpretation of white noise in stop and fricative contexts. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36(6), 457–467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sussman, J. E. (1993). Perception of formant transition cues to place of articulation in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1286–1299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thordardottir, E., & Brandeker, M. (2013). The effect of bilingual exposure versus language specific impairment on nonword repetition and sentence imitation scores. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46, 1–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Lely, H. K. J. (1994). Canonical linking rules: Forward versus reverse linking in normally developing and specifically language impaired children. Cognition, 51, 29–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Lely, H. K. J., & Stollwerck, L. (1997). Binding theory and grammatical specific language impairment in children. Cognition, 62, 245–290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vandewalle, E., Boets, B., Ghesquière, P., & Zink, I. (2012). Auditory processing and speech perception in children with specific language impairment: Relations with oral language and literacy skills. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 635–644.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. (1996). The capacity theory or sentence comprehension: Critique of Just and Carpenter (1992). Psychological Review, 103, 761–772.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (2011). Woodcock reading mastery tests-revised normative update. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., Alario, F. X., & Lorenzi, C. (2005). Deficits in speech perception predict language learning impairment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102, 14110–14115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., & Lorenzi, C. (2011). Noise on, voicing off: Speech perception deficits in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 362–372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the Cape Breton-Victoria Regional school board and participating children, parents, teachers, principals, Robertson and Joanisse for providing permission to adapt the sentence-picture matching task from the 2010 study, Robertson, Joanisse, Desroches, and Ng for providing permission to use the speech identification under noise task from their 2009 study, and Suzanne Myers, Kelsey Morrison, Kevin MacDonald, Kathleen Oliver, Kenzie Kozera, and Jennifer Gallant for help with stimuli preparation, data collection, and analyses.

Funding   This study was funded by Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Discovery Grant (Grant Number 402411-2011) awarded to Erin K. Robertson.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erin K. Robertson.

Ethics declarations

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Parental Questionnaire

Background questions. The following questions will help us interpret the data. Please circle Yes or No for each question.

Is English your child’s first language?

Yes

No

Does your child have a history of hearing impairments?

Yes

No

Does your child have a neurological disorder, such as autism?

Yes

No

Does your child have Attention Deficit (Hyperactivity) Disorder?

Yes

No

Appendix 2

Stimuli for Sentence-Picture Matching task (Robertson and Joanisse 2010)

Short Sentences

Canonical

  • The man is pointing at the boy

  • The doctor pinches the girl

  • This is the boy that points at the man

  • This is the girl that pinches the doctor

Noncanonical

  • The boy is tapped by the girl

  • The man is pointed at by the boy

  • This is the boy that is pointed at by the man

  • This is the man that is pointed at by the boy

Long Sentences

Canonical

  • The doctor with the short blue hair pinches the girl with the nice blond hair

  • The boy in the dark blue pants waves at the man in the dark gray shirt

  • The mother with the long brown hair washes the girl with the nice blond hair

  • The boy in the dark blue pants taps the girl with the nice black hair

  • This is the girl with the nice blond hair that washes the mother with the long brown hair

  • This is the boy in the dark blue pants that taps the girl with the nice blond hair

  • This is the doctor with the short red hair that pinches the girl with the nice blond hair

  • This is the man in the dark gray shirt that waves at the boy in the dark blue pants

Noncanonical

  • The boy in the dark blue pants is tapped by the girl with the nice blond hair

  • The doctor with the short red hair is pinched by the girl with the nice blond hair

  • The girl with the nice blond hair is washed by the mother with the short brown hair

  • The boy in the dark blue pants is waved at by the man in the dark gray shirt

  • This is the man in the light brown shirt that is waved at by the boy in the dark blue pants

  • This is the girl with the nice blond hair that the boy in the dark blue pants taps

  • This is the boy in the dark blue pants that the man in the dark gray shirt waves at

  • This is the girl with the nice blond hair that the mother with the long brown hair washes

Appendix 3

Example of images accompanying short sentences (Robertson and Joanisse 2010)

figure a

The sentence accompanying these images is: The man is pointed at by the boy (Robertson and Joanisse 2010).

Appendix 4

Example of images accompanying long sentences (Robertson and Joanisse 2010)

figure b

The sentence accompanying these images is: This is the boy in the dark blue pants that taps the girl with the nice blond hair (Robertson and Joanisse 2010).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Higgins, M.C., Penney, S.B. & Robertson, E.K. The Effects of Phonological Short-Term Memory and Speech Perception on Spoken Sentence Comprehension in Children: Simulating Deficits in an Experimental Design. J Psycholinguist Res 46, 1213–1235 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-017-9490-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-017-9490-2

Keywords

Navigation