Skip to main content
Log in

Processing Control Information in a Nominal Control Construction: An Eye-Tracking Study

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In an eye-tracking experiment, we examined the processing of the nominal control construction. Participants’ eye-movements were monitored while they read sentences that included either giver control nominals (e.g. promise in Luke’s promise to Sophia to photograph himself) or recipient control nominals (e.g. plea in Luke’s plea to Sophia to photograph herself). In order to examine both the initial access of control information, and its later use in on-line processing, we combined a manipulation of nominal control with a gender match/mismatch paradigm. Results showed that there was evidence of processing difficulty for giver control sentences (relative to recipient control sentences) at the point where the control dependency was initially created, suggesting that control information was accessed during the early parsing stages. This effect is attributed to a recency preference in the formation of control dependencies; the parser prefers to assign a recent antecedent to PRO. In addition, readers slowed down after reading a reflexive pronoun that mismatched with the gender of the antecedent indicated by the control nominal (e.g. Luke’s promise to Sophia to photograph herself). The mismatch cost suggests that control information of the nominal control construction was used to constrain dependency formation involving a controller, PRO and a reflexive, confirming the use of control information in on-line interpretation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We adopt Culicover and Jackendoff’s account of nominal control throughout the paper, as a syntax analysis of nominal control construction is yet to be clearly spelled out (cf. Boeckx and Hornstein 2003; Hornstein and Polinsky 2010).

  2. The control and recency effects were visible at the critical positions in First pass, Regression-path, and Total time in Experiment 1, and in Regression-path, Total reading, and Second pass time in Experiment 2 in Betancort et al. (2006). Thus although the effects were slightly delayed in Experiment 2, the effects are overall clearly visible in early measures of both experiments.

  3. Note that there is no inconsistency between the NP1 control noun preference found by Kwon and Sturt (2014) and the NP2 recency preference predicted in the present study. This is because, in the present study, both NP1 and NP2 precede the infinitival verb, so a recency strategy can lead to a preference for NP2 as an antecedent of PRO. In Kwon and Sturt’s study, in contrast, only one NP precedes the infinitival verb, so a recency preference cannot apply (see Example 5 above). Instead, the preference in that study is likely to be due to the cost of maintaining a temporarily unassigned antecedent for PRO, as discussed above.

  4. See also Sturt and Kwon (2015) for a similar gender mismatch design, which, however, investigated only NP1 control.

  5. Although the first appearance of the mismatch effect was obtained as early as the spill-over region, some other studies examining reflexive-antecedent dependencies have found earlier effects in First-pass measures on the reflexive itself (e.g. Sturt 2003). We suggest that this variability in timing may be partly due to factors that influence the degree of memory interference during retrieval of the antecedent (see Sturt and Kwon 2015).

References

  • Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Betancort, M., Carreiras, M., & Acuña-Fariña, C. (2006). Processing controlled PROs in Spanish. Cognition, 100, 217–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bever, T. G., & Sanz, M. (1997). Empty categories access their antecedents during comprehension of Spanish unaccusatives. Linguistic Inquiry, 28, 69–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boeckx, C., & Hornstein, N. (2003). Reply to “Control is not movement”. Linguistic Inquiry, 34, 269–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boland, J., Tanenhaus, M., & Garnsey, S. (1990). Evidence for the immediate use of verb control information in sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 413–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carreiras, M., Garnham, A., Oakhill, J., & Cain, K. (1996). The use of stereotypical gender information in constructing a mental model: Evidence from English and Spanish. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 639–663.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Culicover, P. W., & Jackendoff, R. (2001). Control is not movement. Linguistic Inquiry, 32, 493–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Culicover, P. W., & Jackendoff, R. (2006). Turn over control to the semantics. Syntax, 9, 131–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demberg, V., & Keller, F. (2008). Data from eye-tracking corpora as evidence for theories of syntactic processing complexity. Cognition, 109(2), 193–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Demestre, J., Meltzer, S., García-Albea, J. E., & Vigil, A. (1999). Identifying the null subject: Evidence from event-related brain potential. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 293–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Featherston, S., Gross, M., Münte, T. F., & Clahsen, H. (2000). Brain potentials in the processing of complex sentences: An ERP study of control and raising constructions. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 141–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Filik, R., & Sanford, A. J. (2008). When is cataphoric reference recognised? Cognition, 107, 1112–1121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L. (1978). On comprehending sentences: Syntactic parsing strategies. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT.

  • Frazier, L. (1987). Sentence processing: A tutorial review. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading (pp. 559–586). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L., & Clifton, C. (1989). Successive cyclicity in the grammar and parser. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4, 93–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L., & Clifton, C. (1996). Construal. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L., Clifton, C., & Randall, J. (1983). Filling gaps: Decision principles and structure in sentence comprehension. Cognition, 13, 187–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garnham, A. (2001). Mental models and the interpretation of anaphora. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68, 1–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. (2000). The dependency locality theory: A distance-based theory of linguistic complexity. In A. Marantz, Y. Miyashita, & W. O’Neil (Eds.), Image, language, brain: Papers from the first mind articulation project symposium (pp. 95–126). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hornstein, N. (1999). Movement and control. Linguistic Inquiry, 30, 69–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hornstein, N., & Polinsky, M. (2010). Control as movement: Across languages and constructions. In N. Hornstein & M. Polinsky (Eds.), Movement theory of control (pp. 1–41). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R., & Culicover, P. W. (2003). The semantic basis of control in English. Language, 79, 517–556. Linguistic Society of America.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434–446.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kwon, N., Monahan, P., & Polinsky, M. (2010). Object control in Korean: A backward control impostor. In N. Hornstein & M. Polinsky (Eds.), Movement theory of control (pp. 299–328). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kwon, N., & Sturt, P. (2014). The use of control information in dependency formation: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Memory and Language, 73, 59–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, R. L., & Vasishth, S. (2005). An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval. Cognitive Science, 29, 375–419.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, R. L., Vasishth, S., & Van Dyke, J. A. (2006). Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 447–454.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mauner, G., Tanenhaus, M., & Carlson, G. (1995). Implicit arguments in sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 357–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McElree, B., & Bever, T. (1989). The psychological reality of linguistically defined gaps. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 21–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicol, J. (1988). Co-references in parsing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. MIT.

  • Oakhill, J. V., Garnham, A., & Reynolds, D. J. (2005). Immediate activation of stereotypical gender information in reading. Memory and Cognition, 33, 972–983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1975). The perceptual span and peripheral cues in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 65–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1978). Eye movements in reading and information processing. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 618–660.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, D. J., Garnham, A., & Oakhill, J. (2006). Evidence of immediate activation of gender information from a social role name. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 886–903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, P. (1967). The grammar of English predicate complement constructions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sag, I., & Fodor, J. (1995). Extraction without traces. In: Proceedings of the 13th west coast conference on formal linguistics, pp. 365–384.

  • Sturt, P. (2003). The time-course of the application of binding constraints in reference resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(3), 542–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sturt, P., Scheepers, C., & Pickering, M. J. (2002). Ambiguity resolution after initial misanalysis: The role of recency. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 371–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sturt, P., & Kwon, N. (2015). The processing of raising and nominal control: An eye-tracking study. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00331.

  • Walenski, M. (2002). Relating parsers and grammars: On the structure and real-time comprehension of English infinitival complements. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, San Diego.

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Dr. Hui Fang for assistance in stimuli generation and Meredith Englund, Caitlin Richter and Lewis White for assistance in data collection. This research was supported by the faculty research fund of Konkuk University in 2014.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nayoung Kwon.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kwon, N., Sturt, P. Processing Control Information in a Nominal Control Construction: An Eye-Tracking Study. J Psycholinguist Res 45, 779–793 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-015-9374-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-015-9374-2

Keywords

Navigation