Skip to main content
Log in

Eye Gaze and Head Posture Jointly Influence Judgments of Dominance, Physical Strength, and Anger

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Social status hierarchies are a universal principle of organization in human societies. Status judgments are often influenced by perceptions of the face and posture. Two important nonverbal cues of social status are head postures and eye gaze. Prior research has shown contradictory results and little is known about the interaction of these two cues. Study 1 investigated how eye gaze (direct vs. averted) and head postures (bowed vs. neutral vs. raised) impact judgments of dominance and physical strength. Judgments of dominance were influenced more than judgments of physical strength. Furthermore, raised heads implied dominance and strength, but in contrast to common assumptions, a bowed head conveyed dominance if the eyes gazed at the observer. Study 2 showed that bowed heads with direct gaze conveyed anger, potentially explaining the increased judgments of dominance. Taken together, the results show that head posture and gaze interactively modulated status-related traits and emotions, namely, dominance, strength, and anger, and help clarify prior incompatible findings on head postures and eye gaze.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Further details are available from the authors or can be computed from the publicly available data.

  2. We used the ± 25 degrees due to practical reasons related to FaceGen. These degrees of tilt is the maximum tilt at which the targets’ eyes and, consequently, their gazes can still be clearly seen. For this reason, we used these levels to manipulate our targets’ head positions in order to maximize the tilt manipulation while still enabling the gaze manipulation.

  3. We ran an additional study on Amazon Mturk where we asked participants (N = 84) to judge the perceived gaze of the target faces. We wanted to check if the faces with direct gaze were really perceived as looking directly towards participants and if the faces with averted gaze were seen by participants as looking away from them. Participants had to judge on a scale from -3 (looks away from me) to + 3 (looks directly at me). The methodology was otherwise identical to the main studies. Perceived gaze judgments were influenced by gaze direction, F(1, 707.27) = 8177.8, p < .001. Faces with direct gaze were perceived as looking more towards the participants (M = 1.60 [1.44, 1.75]) than faces with averted gaze (M = -2.30 [− 2.51, − 2.09]), B = 1.94 [1.90, 1.98], β = 0.16. However, we found an interaction between head posture and gaze direction, F(1, 708.795) = 15.35, p < .001, B = − 0.10 [− 0.15, − 0.05], β = − 0.01—albeit this effect was much weaker than the main effect of gaze. When the gaze was direct, faces with bowed heads were perceived as having more direct gaze (M = 2.17 [2.03, 2.31]) than faces with raised heads (M = 1.53, [1.39, 1.67], p < .001), and neutral heads (M = 1.07 [0.93, 1.21], p < .001). Faces with bowed heads were also judged as having more direct gaze than faces with neutral heads (p < .001). When the gaze was averted, both faces with bowed heads (M = − 2.22, [− 2.37, − 2.08]), and neutral heads (M = − 2.20 [− 2.34, − 2.06]) were judged as having less averted gaze than faces with raised heads (M = − 2.46 [− 2.60, − 2.32], p < .001). Faces with bowed heads and neutral heads did not show significant differences (p = .70). Due to these results, we re-ran the analyses of Study 1 and Study 2, substituting the judged directness of the gaze for the gaze contrast. We found essentially the same results except a strengthened linear head contrast effect on dominance. See discussion and Supplementary materials file – Study 1 and Study 2 with perceived gaze as an independent variable. Note, however, that the pictures we intended to show direct gaze were not all judged as looking directly into the camera, which is a limitation of our study and might have attenuated effects.

  4. In this definition, we associate motivation and potential to influence in order to define dominance. Influence is one of the main qualities of social dominance (e.g., Cheng et al., 2013).

  5. Due to a programming mistake, 2 sets of the total 12 missed one face in all samples. Thus, these participants judged only 59 faces twice (118 judgments in total).

  6. More recently, Schneider and Carbon (2017) asked participants to judge faces on dominance. In contrast to previous studies, they did not find an effect of head posture. Raised heads were not perceived as more dominant than bowed heads. Given the presence of muscles around human necks and the role of muscular strength in perceiving dominance (e.g., Toscano et al., 2016), the authors wrote that one likely reason for these results was that they only showed faces without necks. Therefore, it might be the case that our stimuli, where the necks can be seen by the participants, could have influenced our data. However, as it can be seen in Fig. 1, bowed heads exposed less their necks than neutral and raised heads, but were perceived as more dominant than the former. Nonetheless, we suggest that further studies investigating the effects found in our research should only use faces without necks.

  7. We ran an additional study and found that both female and male faces were correctly identified. We asked participants (N = 90) on Amazon MTurk to select the gender of the faces used in both studies. Male faces were identified as males in 99% of the cases, p < .001. Female faces were selected as female in most cases (91%), p < .001.

References

  • Argyle, M., & Cook, M. (1976). Gaze and mutual gaze. Oxford: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, J., Woike, B. A., & Hyman, L. M. (1992). Which are the stimuli in facial displays of anger and happiness? Configurational bases of emotion recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 1050–1066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aviezer, H., Trope, Y., & Todorov, A. (2012). Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions. Science, 338, 1225–1229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). The eye direction detector (EDD) and the shared attention mechanism: Two cases for evolutionary psychology. In C. Moore & J. Philip (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 41–59). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bassili, J. N. (1979). Emotion recognition: The role of facial movement and the relative importance of upper and lower areas of the face. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2049–2058.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bindemann, M., Burton, A. M., & Langton, S. R. (2008). How do eye gaze and facial expression interact? Visual Cognition, 16, 708–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaker, N. M., & van Vugt, M. (2014). The status-size hypothesis: How cues of physical size and social status influence each other. In J. T. Cheng, J. L. Tracy, & C. Anderson (Eds.), The psychology of social status (pp. 119–137). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, D., & Sulikowski, D. (2010). A new viewpoint on the evolution of sexually dimorphic human faces. Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 573–585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R., Wallace, S., & Benson, P. J. (1996). Real men don’t look down: Direction of gaze affects sex decisions on faces. Visual Cognition, 3, 393–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Foulsham, T., Kingstone, A., & Henrich, J. (2013). Two ways to the top: Evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 103–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiao, J. Y. (2010). Neural basis of social status hierarchy across species. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20, 803–809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiao, J. Y., Adams, R. B., Peter, U. T., Lowenthal, W. T., Richeson, J. A., & Ambady, N. (2008). Knowing who’s boss: fMRI and ERP investigations of social dominance perception. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 11, 201–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Correll, J., Hudson, S. M., Guillermo, S., & Earls, H. A. (2016). Of kith and kin: Perceptual enrichment, expectancy, and reciprocity in face perception. Personality and Social Psychology Review. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868316657250.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coss, R. G. (1978). Perceptual determinants of gaze aversion by the lesser mouse lemur (Microcebus muri-nus), the role of two facing eyes. Behaviour, 64, 248–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, D. D. (2005). Dominance, status, and social hierarchies. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The evolutionary psychology handbook (pp. 676–697). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalmaso, M., Galfano, G., Coricelli, C., & Castelli, L. (2014). Temporal dynamics underlying the modulation of social status on social attention. PLoS ONE, 9, e93139.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dalmaso, M., Pavan, G., Castelli, L., & Galfano, G. (2012). Social status gates social attention in humans. Biology Letters, 8, 450–452.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. New York: D. Appleton.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • de Gelder, B., Meeren, H. K. M., Righart, R., Stock, J., van de Riet, W. A. C., & Tamietto, M. (2006). Beyond the face: Exploring rapid influences of context on face processing. Progress in Brain Research, 155, 37–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. (2007). Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dotsch, R., & Todorov, A. (2012). Reverse correlating social face perception. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 562–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ewbank, M. P., Jennings, C., & Calder, A. J. (2009). Why are you angry with me facial expressions of threat influence perception of gaze direction. Journal of Vision, 9(12), 1–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Facegen Main Software Development Kit 3.5. (2013). Vancouver, BC: Singular inversions.

  • Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 9602–9605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99, 689–723.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, J. B., & Ambady, N. (2011). A dynamic interactive theory of person construal. Psychological Review, 118, 247–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giessner, S. R., Ryan, M. K., Schubert, T. W., & van Quaquebeke, N. (2011). The power of pictures: Vertical picture angles in power pictures. Media Psychology, 14, 442–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & LeBeau, L. S. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and the vertical dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 898–924.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hehman, E., Flake, J. K., & Freeman, J. B. (2015). Static and dynamic facial cues differentially affect the consistency of social evaluations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 1123–1134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hehman, E., Leitner, J. B., & Gaertner, S. L. (2013). Enhancing static facial features increases intimidation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 747–754.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., Blairy, S., & Kleck, R. E. (2000). The influence of facial emotion displays, gender, and ethnicity on judgments of dominance and affiliation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 265–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2015). The role of social context for the interpretation of emotional facial expressions. Understanding facial expressions in communication (pp. 119–141). India: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, E., Wolf, E. B., Looser, C., & Cuddy, A. (2016). Visual attention to powerful postures: People avert their gaze from nonverbal dominance displays. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 68, 60–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, R., White, D., Van Montfort, X., & Mike Burton, A. (2011). Variability in photos of the same face. Cognition, 121, 313–323.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Main, J. C., Little, A. C., Welling, L. L., Feinberg, D. R., et al. (2010). Facial cues of dominance modulate the short-term gaze-cuing effect in human observers. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 617–624.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M. (2000). Everyday data analysis in social psychology. Comparisons of linear models. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 370–392). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., McClelland, G. H., & Ryan, C. S. (2009). Data analysis. A model comparison approach. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., Westfall, J., & Kenny, D. A. (2012). Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: A new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 54–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knutson, B. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion influence interpersonal trait inferences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20, 165–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lobmaier, J., Tiddeman, B. P., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). Emotional expression modulates perceived gaze direction. Emotion, 8, 573–577.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., & Larson, C. L. (2010). What makes an angry face look so… angry? Examining visual attention to the shape of threat in children and adults. Visual Cognition, 18, 1165–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lukaszewski, A. W., Simmons, Z. L., Anderson, C., & Roney, J. R. (2010).The role of physical formidability in human social status allocation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, 385–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, C. N., & Martin, D. (2007). A boy primed Sue: Feature-based processing and person construal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 793–805.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Social hierarchy: The self-reinforcing nature of power and status. The Academy of Management Annals, 2, 351–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Main, J. C., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., & Little, A. (2009). Integrating gaze direction and sexual dimorphism of face shape when perceiving the dominance of others. Perception, 38, 1275–1283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, A. A., Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Why do fear and anger look the way they do? Form and social function in facial expressions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 73–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martinez, L., Falvello, V. B., Aviezer, H., & Todorov, A. (2016). Contributions of facial expressions and body language to the rapid perception of dynamic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 939–952.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McArthur, L. Z., & Baron, R. M. (1983). Toward an ecological theory of social perception. Psychological Review, 90, 215–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mendelson, M. J., Haith, M. M., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1982). Face scanning and responsiveness to social cues in infant rhesus monkeys. Developmental Psychology, 18, 222–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mignault, A., & Chaudhuri, A. (2003). The many faces of a neutral face: Head tilt and perception of dominance and emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 111–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oosterhof, N. N., & Todorov, A. (2008). The functional basis of face evaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 11087–11092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I., Rowland, D., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., et al. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature, 394, 884–887.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perrett, D. I., & Mistlin, A. J. (1990). Perception of facial characteristics by monkeys. In W. C. Stebbins & M. A. Berkley (Eds.), Comparative perception: Complex signals. Wiley series in neuroscience (Vol. 2, pp. 187–215). Oxford: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redican, W. K. (1982). An evolutionary perspective on human facial displays. In P. Ekman (Ed.), Emotion in the human face (pp. 212–280). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, G., Lie, H. C., Thevaraja, N., Taylor, L., Iredell, N., Curran, C., et al. (2011). Facial attractiveness ratings from video-clips and static images tell the same story. PLoS ONE, 6, e26653.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Richeson, J. A., Todd, A. R., Trawalter, S., & Baird, A. A. (2008). Eye-gaze direction modulates race-related amygdala activity. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 11, 233–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rule, N. O., Adams, R. B., Jr., Ambady, N., & Freeman, J. B. (2012). Perceptions of dominance following glimpses of faces and bodies. Perception, 41, 687–706.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rule, N. O., Ambady, N., & Adams, R. B., Jr. (2009). Personality in perspective: Judgmental consistency across orientations of the face. Perception, 38, 1688–1699.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rymarczyk, K., Żurawski, Ł., Jankowiak-Siuda, K., & Szatkowska, I. (2016). Do dynamic compared to static facial expressions of happiness and anger reveal enhanced facial mimicry? PLoS ONE, 11, e0158534.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid Mast, M., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Who is the boss and who is not? Accuracy of judging status. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 145–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, T. M., & Carbon, C. C. (2017). Taking the perfect selfie: Investigating the impact of perspective on the perception of higher cognitive variables. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, T. M., Hecht, H., & Carbon, C. C. (2012). Judging body weight from faces: The height–weight illusion. Perception, 41, 121–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sell, A., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2014). The human anger face evolved to enhance cues of strength. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 425–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sell, A., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Sznycer, D., von Rueden, C., & Gurven, M. (2009a). Human adaptations for the visual assessment of strength and fighting ability from the body and face. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276, 575–584.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2009b). Formidability and the logic of human anger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 15073–15078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., & Hasegawa, T. (2005). Direct gaze captures visuospatial attention. Visual Cognition, 12, 127–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sessa, P., & Dalmaso, M. (2016). Race perception and gaze direction differently impair visual working memory for faces: An event-related potential study. Social Neuroscience, 11, 97–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2013). Life after p-hacking. In Meeting of the society for personality and social psychology, New Orleans, LA (pp. 17–19).

  • Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 245–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, C. A., Oldmeadow, J. A., Santos, I. M., Towler, J., Michael Burt, D., & Young, A. W. (2013). Social inferences from faces: Ambient images generate a three-dimensional model. Cognition, 127, 105–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, C. A., Young, A. W., & Rhodes, G. (2017). Facial first impressions from another angle: How social judgements are influenced by changeable and invariant facial properties. British Journal of Psychology, 108, 397–415.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tiedens, L. Z. (2001). Anger and advancement versus sadness and subjugation: The effect of negative emotion expressions on social status conferral. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 86–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tiedens, L. Z., Ellsworth, P. C., & Mesquita, B. (2000). Stereotypes about sentiments and status: Emotional expectations for high- and low-status group members. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 560–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S., & Bayliss, A. (2011). The impact of social gaze perception on attention. In A. Calder, G. Rhodes, M. Johnson, & J. Haxby (Eds.), Handbook of face perception (pp. 551–570). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todorov, A., & Porter, J. (2014). Misleading first impressions: Different for different facial images of the same person. Psychological Science, 25, 1404–1417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toscano, H., Schubert, T. W., Dotsch, R., Falvello, V., & Todorov, A. (2016). Physical strength as a cue to dominance: A data-driven approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 1603–1616.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toscano, H., Schubert, T. W., & Sell, A. N. (2014). Judgments of dominance from the face track physical strength. Evolutionary Psychology, 12, 1–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Vugt, M., & Tybur, J. M. (2015). The evolutionary foundations of hierarchy: Status, dominance, prestige, and leadership. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (2nd ed., pp. 788–809). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Rueden, C. (2014). The roots and fruits of social status in small-scale human societies. In J. Cheng, J. Tracy, & C. Anderson (Eds.), The psychology of social status (pp. 179–200). New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Rueden, C., Gurven, M., & Kaplan, H. (2011). Why do men seek status? Fitness payoffs to dominance and prestige. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 278, 2223–2232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Windhager, S., Schaefer, K., & Fink, B. (2011). Geometric morphometrics of male facial shape in relation to physical strength and perceived atractiveness, dominance, and masculinity. American Journal of Human Biology, 23, 805–814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zebrowitz, L. A. (2011). Ecological and social approaches to face perception. In A. Calder, G. Rhodes, M. Johnson, & J. Haxby (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of face perception (pp. 31–50). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zebrowitz, L. A., Kikuchi, M., & Fellous, J.-M. (2010). Facial resemblance to emotions: Group differences, impression effects, and race stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 175–189.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was conducted with support from Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (FCT) Grant SFRH/BD/75435/2010.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hugo Toscano.

Additional information

All data, analysis and materials have been made publicly available via Open Science Framework and can be accessed at https://osf.io/s8zrg/files/.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 85 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Toscano, H., Schubert, T.W. & Giessner, S.R. Eye Gaze and Head Posture Jointly Influence Judgments of Dominance, Physical Strength, and Anger. J Nonverbal Behav 42, 285–309 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-018-0276-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-018-0276-5

Keywords

Navigation