Skip to main content
Log in

Picture Power: Gender Versus Body Language in Perceived Status

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Power hierarchies in interaction are maintained due to a variety of cues, including gender and body language, and can keep competent individuals from being regarded as high status. The present study primed participants with an image consisting of two components—gender (man or woman) and body pose (dominant or submissive)—and then asked participants to classify written target words as either dominant or submissive. In response to these target words, we measured accuracy (% incorrect) and classification speed (RT), in addition to event-related potentials (ERPs), from 23 participants. Although we did not find ERP differences in the predicted N400 component, error rate and RT measures indicated that regardless of the gender of the prime, dominant poses facilitated identification of dominant words. Interestingly, whereas female submissive posing facilitated classification of submissive target words, male submissive posing did not. These results support the idea that women can use counter-stereotypical nonverbal displays, dominant poses, to change how they are initially perceived in terms of power. Interestingly, men may be more limited in the success of their counter-stereotypical, submissive, posing. Potential underlying mechanisms are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This effect size was computed from the alternate statistic f 2 = 0.35 in the program G-star power.

  2. With one being low V, four being neutral, and seven being high V, low V poses were rated respectively: 2.38, 1.50, 2.50, 4.38, and 3.38. The italicized score is what caused that pose and its high V counterpart to be discarded. High V poses were rated respectively: 5.13, 5.38, 6.50, 5.88, 6.25.

  3. The EEG was sampled at 250 Hz using a band-pass filler of 0.1–30 Hz, and impedances were kept below 40 kΩ (the Geonet system uses high-impedance amplifiers). The ERPs were vertex referenced for recording and average referenced for analysis and presentation. Following re-referencing, the brain waves were baseline corrected to a 100-ms prestimulus window. Eye artifacts during data collection were monitored with four EOG electrodes, with voltage shifts above 70 µV marked as bad (for more on the EOG algorithm, see Gratton et al. 1983; Miller et al. 1988). Non-EOG channels were marked as bad if there were shifts within the electrode of greater than 200 µV for any single trial. If over 20 % of the channels were bad for a trial, the whole trial was rejected. Considering all of the participants, 8.31 % (SD = 6.79 %) of the trials were rejected; after removing one participant for excessive artifacts (26.34 %), 7.49 % (SD = 5.67 %) of the trials were rejected.

  4. The 128 electrodes were broken up into five clusters of channels that corresponded roughly to basic anatomical structures of the brain (Kelly et al. 2004).

  5. All reported error rate means and standard deviations are the untransformed values. Analyses were conducted on transformed values, according to the arcsine of the square root function, as is appropriate for proportions (Sokal and Rohlf 2012).

  6. It is not unprecedented to discover behavioral effects but not the expected N400 effect. See Brown and Hagoort (1993) for an example and Holcomb et al. (2005) for a more nuanced interpretation.

References

  • Aguinis, H., & Henley, C. A. (2001). Effects of nonverbal behavior on perceptions of a female employee’s power bases. The Journal of Social Psychology, 141(4), 537–549.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aguinis, H., Simonsen, M. M., & Pierce, C. A. (1998). Effects of nonverbal behavior on perceptions of power bases. The Journal of Social Psychology, 138, 455–469.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C., John, O. P., Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (2001). Who attains social status? Effects of personality and physical attractiveness in social groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aries, E. (1996). Men and women in interaction: Reconsidering the differences. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banaji, M. R., & Hardin, C. D. (1996). Automatic stereotyping. Psychological Science, 7(3), 136–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S., McCarthy, G., & Wood, C. C. (1985). Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60(4), 343–355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biernat, M. (2009). Stereotypes and shifting standards. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 137–152). New York: Taylor & Francis Group LLC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, C., & Hagoort, P. (1993). The processing nature of the N400: Evidence from masked priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(1), 34–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (2004). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind (2nd ed.). Boston: Pearson Education, Incorporated.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carli, L. L., Loeber, C. C., & LaFleur, S. J. (1995). Nonverbal behavior, gender, and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(6), 1030–1041.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carney, D. R., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Yap, A. J. (2010). Power posing: Brief nonverbal displays affect neuroendocrine levels and risk tolerance. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1363–1368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carney, D. R., Hall, J. A., & LeBeau, L. S. (2005). Beliefs about the nonverbal expression of social power. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 29(2), 105–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catalyst. (2013). Women CEOs of the fortune 1000. Retrieved from http://www.catalyst.org/knowledge/women-ceos-fortune-1000

  • Cejka, M. A., & Eagly, A. H. (1999). Gender-stereotypic images of occupations correspond to the sex segregation of employment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(4), 413–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Center for American Women and Politics. (2013). Women in the US Congress 2013. Retrieved from http://www.cawp.rutgers.edu/fast_facts/levels_of_office/documents/cong.pdf.

  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. Den Hagg: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Gelder, B. (2006). Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language. Neuroscience, 7, 242–249.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Gelder, B., van den Stock, J., Meeren, H. K. M., Sinke, C. B. A., Kret, M. E., & Tamietto, M. (2010). Standing up for the body. Recent progress in uncovering the networks involved in the perception of bodies and bodily expressions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 513–527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Lemus, S., Spears, R., & Moya, M. (2012). The power of a smile to move you: Complementary submissiveness in women’s posture as a function of gender salience and facial expression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(11), 1480–1494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo, B. M. (1992). Nonverbal behavior and self-presentation. Psychological Bulletin, 111(2), 203–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Ellyson, S. L., Keating, C. F., Heltman, K., & Brown, C. E. (1988). The relationship of social power to visual displays of dominance between men and women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(2), 233–242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Karau, St. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109(3), 573–598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 123–174). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisek, M. H., & Ofshe, R. (1970). The process of status evolution. Sociometry, 33(3), 327–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2006). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foschi, M. (2000). Double standards for competence: Theory and research. Annual Review of Sociology, 26, 21–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gifford, R., & Hine, D. W. (1994). The role of verbal behavior in the encoding and decoding of interpersonal dispositions. Journal of Research in Personality, 28(2), 115–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The ambivalent sexism inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifacts. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468–484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hai, D. M., Khairullah, Z. Y., & Coulmas, N. (1982). Sex and the single armrest: Use of personal space during air travel. Psychological Report, 51, 743–749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1990). Nonverbal sex differences: Accuracy of communication and expressive style. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1998). How big are nonverbal sex differences? The case of smiling and sensitivity to nonverbal cues. In D. J. Canary & K. Dindia (Eds.), Sex differences and similarities in communication (pp. 155–178). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & LeBeau, L. S. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and the vertical dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 131(6), 898–924.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hemmer, J., & Kleiber, D. (1981). Tomboys and sissies: Androgynous children? Sex Roles, 7, 1205–1212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henley, N. M. (1995). Body politics revisited: What do we know today? In P. J. Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody (Eds.), Gender, power, and communication in human relationships (pp. 27–61). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., Adams, R. B, Jr, & Kleck, R. E. (2004). Facial appearance, gender, and emotion expression. Emotion, 4(4), 378–388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hilgenkamp, K. D., & Livingston, M. M. (2002). Tomboys, masculine characteristics, and self-ratings of confidence in career success. Psychological Reports, 90, 743–749.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hinzman, L., & Kelly, S. D. (2013). Effects of emotional body language on rapid out-group judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(1), 152–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, A. K., Sidanius, J., Patto, F., Levin, S., Thomsen, L., Kteily, N., & Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (2012). Social dominance orientation: Revisiting the structure and function of a variable predicting social and political attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(5), 583–606.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holcomb, P. J., Reder, L., Misra, M., & Grainger, J. (2005). The effects of prime visibility on ERP measures of masked priming. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(1), 155–172.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keating, C. F. (2011). Channeling charisma through face and body status cues. In D. Chadee & A. Kostic (Eds.), Social psychological dynamics (pp. 93–111). Jamaica: University of the West Indies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, S. D., Kravitz, C., & Hopkins, M. (2004). Neural correlates of bimodal speech and gesture comprehension. Brain and Language, 89, 253–260.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Key, A. P. F., Dove, G. O., & Maguire, M. J. (2005). Linking brainwaves to the brain: An ERP primer. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(2), 183–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., & Ristikari, T. (2011). Are leader stereotypes masculine? A meta-analysis of three research paradigms. Psychological Bulletin, 137(4), 616–642.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463–470.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62(14), 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Letters to Nature, 307, 161–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landy, D., & Sigall, H. (1974). Beauty is talent: Task evaluation as a function of the performer’s physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29(3), 299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leuthold, H., Filik, R., Murphy, K., & Mackenzie, I. G. (2012). The on-line processing of socio-emotional information in prototypical scenarios: Inferences from brain potentials. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(4), 457–466.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levant, R. F., & Pollack, W. S. (1995). Introduction. In R. F. Levant & W. S. Pollack (Eds.), A New psychology of men (pp. 1–10). New York: BasicBooks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, M., Lynch, A., Brewer, G., & Bruno, D. (2014). Detection of sexual orientation (“gaydar”) by homosexual and heterosexual women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43(2), 345–352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maricchiolo, F., Livi, S., Bonaiuto, M., & Gnisci, A. (2011). Hand gestures and perceived influence in small group interaction. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14(2), 755–764.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazur, A., Rosa, E., Faupel, M., Heller, J., Leen, R., & Thurman, B. (1980). Physiological aspects of communication via mutual gaze. American Journal of Sociology, 86(1), 50–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meeren, H. K. M., van Heijnsbergen, C. C. R. J., & de Gelder, B. (2005). Rapid perceptual integration of facial expression and emotional body language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 102(45), 16518–16523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Gratton, G., & Yee, C. M. (1988). Generalized implementation of an eye movement correction procedure. Psychophysiology, 25, 241–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, H. A., & Porter, N. K. (1988). Leadership and nonverbal behavior of Hispanic females across school equity environments. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 12, 147–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K. R., Myors, B., & Wolach, A. H. (2009). Statistical power analysis: A simple and general model for traditional and modern hypothesis tests. New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Neil, J. M., Good, G. E., & Holmes, S. (1995). Fifteen years of theory and research on men’s gender role conflict: New paradigms for empirical research. In R. F. Levant & W. S. Pollack (Eds.), A new psychology of men (pp. 164–206). New York: BasicBooks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parker, P. S., & Ogilvie, D. T. (1996). Gender, culture, and leadership: Toward a culturally distinct model of African-American women executives’ leadership strategies. Leadership Quarterly, 7, 189–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pleck, J. H. (1995). The gender role strain paradigm: An update. In R. F. Levant & W. S. Pollack (Eds.), A new psychology of men (pp. 11–32). New York: BasicBooks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poling, T. H. (1978). Sex differences, dominance, and physical attractiveness in the use of nonverbal emblems. Psychological Reports, 43, 1087–1092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, G. N., & Butterfield, D. A. (1989). The “good manager” Did androgyny fare better in the 1980s? Group and Organization Management, 14(2), 216–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. L. (1987). Nonverbal behavior, dominance, and the basis of status in task groups. American Sociological Review, 52, 683–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenwasser, S. M., & Dean, N. G. (1989). Gender role and political office. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 13(1), 77–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., Moss-Racusin, C. A., Phelan, J. E., & Nauts, S. (2012). Status incongruity and backlash effects: Defending the gender hierarchy motivates prejudice against female leaders. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 165–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid Mast, M., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Who is the boss and who is not? Accuracy of judging status. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(3), 145–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sczesny, S. (2003). A closer look beneath the surface: Various facets of the think-manager-think- male stereotype. Sex Roles, 49, 353–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sczesny, S., Spreemann, S., & Stahlberg, D. (2006). Masculine = competent? Physical appearance and sex as sources of gender-stereotypic attributions. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 65(1), 15–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sokal, R. R., & Rohlf, F. J. (2012). Biometry: The principles and practice of statistics in biological research (4th ed.). New York: W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stekelenburg, J. J., & de Gelder, B. (2004). The neural correlates of perceiving human bodies: An ERP study on the body-inversion effect. NeuroReport, 15(9), 777–780.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tesink, C. M. J. Y., Petersson, K. M., van Berkum, J. J. A., van den Brink, D., Buitelaar, J. K., & Hagoort, P. (2009). Unification of speaker and meaning in language comprehension: An fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(11), 2085–2099.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tiedens, L. Z., & Fragale, A. R. (2003). Power moves: Complementarity in dominant and submissive nonverbal behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(3), 558–568.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Berkum, J. J. A. (2008). Understanding sentences in context: What brain waves can tell us. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(6), 376–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Berkum, J. J. A., van den Brink, D., Tesink, C. M. J. Y., Kos, M., & Hagoort, P. (2008). The neural integration of speaker and message. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(4), 580–591.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van den Stock, J., Righart, R., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Body expressions influence recognition of emotions in the face and voice. Emotion, 7(3), 487–494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Heijnsbergen, C. C. R. J., Meeren, H. K. M., Grézes, J., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Rapid detection of fear in body expressions, an ERP study. Brain Research, 1186, 233–241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Visscher, P. M. (2008). Sizing up human height variation. Nature Genetics, 40(5), 489–490.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, K. R., Crites, S. L, Jr, Taylor, J. H., & Corral, G. (2009). Wait, what? Assessing stereotype incongruities using the N400 ERP component. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 191–198.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, P. (2010). Nonverbal gender differences: examining gestures of university-educated Mandarin Chinese speakers. Text and Talk, 30(3), 333–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the Psychology Department at Colgate University for allowing them access to their facilities to run the experiment, Dr. Carrie Keating for her intellectual contributions, Timothy Collett for his technical assistance, and research assistant Rachel Goldberg for her professionalism and organization.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to April H. Bailey.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bailey, A.H., Kelly, S.D. Picture Power: Gender Versus Body Language in Perceived Status. J Nonverbal Behav 39, 317–337 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-015-0212-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-015-0212-x

Keywords

Navigation