Skip to main content
Log in

Betting Decision Under Break-Streak Pattern: Evidence from Casino Gaming

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Gambling Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cognitive bias is prevalent among gamblers, especially those with gambling problems. Grounded in the heuristics theories, this study contributes to the literature by examining a cognitive bias triggered by the break streak pattern in the casino setting. We postulate that gamblers tend to bet on the latest outcome when there is a break-streak pattern. Moreover, three determinants of the betting decision under break-streak pattern, including the streak length of the alternative outcome, the frequency of the latest outcome, and gender, were identified and examined in this study. A non-participatory observational study was conducted among the Cussec gamblers in a casino in Macao. An analysis of 1229 bets confirms our postulation, particularly when the streak of the alternative outcome is long, the latest outcome is frequent, and the gamblers are females. The findings provide meaningful implications for casino management and public policymakers regarding the minimization of gambling harm.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Afifi, T. O., Cox, B. J., Martens, P. J., Sareen, J., & Enns, M. W. (2010). Demographic and social variables associated with problem gambling among men and women in Canada. Psychiatry Research, 178(2), 395–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Altmann, E. M., & Burns, B. D. (2005). Streak biases in decision making: Data and a memory model. Cognitive Systems Research, 6(1), 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asparouhova, E., Hertzel, M., & Lemmon, M. (2009). Inference from streaks in random outcomes: Experimental evidence on beliefs in regime shifting and the law of small numbers. Management Science, 55(11), 1766–1782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ayton, P., & Fischer, I. (2004). The hot hand fallacy and the gambler’s fallacy: Two faces of subjective randomness? Memory and Cognition, 32(8), 1369–1378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Babad, E., & Katz, Y. (1991). Wishful thinking—Against all odds. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21(23), 1921–1938.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Hillel, M., & Wagenaar, W. A. (1991). The perception of randomness. Advances in Applied Mathematics, 12(4), 428–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaszczynski, A., Collins, P., Fong, D., Ladouceur, R., Nower, L., Shaffer, H. J., et al. (2011). Responsible gambling: General principles and minimal requirements. Journal of Gambling Studies, 27(4), 565–573.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blaszczynski, A., Ladouceur, R., & Shaffer, H. J. (2004). A science-based framework for responsible gambling: The Reno Model. Journal of Gambling Studies, 20(3), 307–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blinder, D. S., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). Beliefs about what types of mechanisms produce random sequences. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 21(4), 414–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burns, B. D., & Corpus, B. (2004). Randomness and inductions from streaks: “Gambler’s fallacy” versus “hot hand”. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 179–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlbring, P., Degerman, N., Jonsson, J., & Andersson, G. (2012). Internet-based treatment of pathological gambling with a three-year follow-up. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 41(4), 321–334.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, K. A., & Shu, S. B. (2007). The rule of three: How the third event signals the emergence of a streak. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 104(1), 113–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, Y. B. (1993). Paradigms and conventions: Uncertainty, decision making, and entrepreneurship. Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clotfelter, C. T., & Cook, P. J. (1993). Notes: The “gambler’s fallacy” in lottery play. Management Science, 39(12), 1521–1525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croson, R., & Sundali, J. (2005). The gambler’s fallacy and the hot hand: Empirical data from casinos. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 30(3), 195–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dane, E., & Pratt, M. G. (2007). Exploring intuition and its role in managerial decision making. Academy of Management Review, 32(1), 33–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delfabbro, P. (2004). The stubborn logic of regular gamblers: Obstacles and dilemmas in cognitive gambling research. Journal of Gambling Studies, 20(1), 1–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Denzin, N. K. (1968). On the ethics of disguised observation. Social Problems, 15(4), 502–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Marklein, F., & Sunde, U. (2009). Biased probability judgment: Evidence of incidence and relationship to economic outcomes from a representative sample. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 72(3), 903–915.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dormann, C. F., Elith, J., Bacher, S., Buchmann, C., Carl, G., Carré, G., & Lautenbach, S. (2013). Collinearity: A review of methods to deal with it and a simulation study evaluating their performance. Ecography, 36(1), 27–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ESRC. (2015). ESRC framework for research ethics: Updated January 2015. http://www.esrc.ac.uk/_images/framework-for-research-ethics_tcm8-33470.pdf.

  • Ferguson, C. J., Coulson, M., & Barnett, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of pathological gaming prevalence and comorbidity with mental health, academic and social problems. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(12), 1573–1578.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS (3rd ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finlay, K., Marmurek, H. H. C., Kanetkar, V., & Londerville, J. (2010). Casino décor effects on gambling emotions and intentions. Environment and Behavior, 42(4), 524–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fong, L. H. N., Law, R., & Lam, D. (2014). An examination of factors driving Chinese gamblers’ fallacy bias. Journal of Gambling Studies, 30(3), 757–770.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fortune, E. E., & Goodie, A. S. (2012). Cognitive distortions as a component and treatment focus of pathological gambling: A review. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 26(2), 298–310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gooding, P., & Tarrier, N. (2009). A systematic review and meta-analysis of cognitive-behavioural interventions to reduce problem gambling: Hedging our bets? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(7), 592–607.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, M. D. (1994). The role of cognitive bias and skill in fruit machine gambling. British Journal of Psychology, 85(3), 351–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Homan, R. (1980). The ethics of covert methods. British Journal of Sociology, 31(1), 46–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, H., & Kacperczyk, M. (2009). The price of sin: The effects of social norms on markets. Journal of Financial Economics, 93(1), 15–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huber, J., Kirchler, M., & Stöckl, T. (2010). The hot hand belief and the gambler’s fallacy in investment decisions under risk. Theory and Decision, 68(4), 445–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson, A., Grant, J., Kim, S., Odlaug, B., & Götestam, K. G. (2009). Risk factors for problematic gambling: A critical literature review. Journal of Gambling Studies, 25(1), 67–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joukhador, J., Maccallum, F., & Blaszczynski, A. (2003). Differences in cognitive distortions between problem and social gamblers. Psychological Reports, 92(3c), 1203–1214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3(3), 430–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1996). On the reality of cognitive illusions. Psychological Review, 103(3), 582–591.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keren, G., & Lewis, C. (1994). The two fallacies of gamblers: Type I and type II. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 60(1), 75–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Khatri, N., & Ng, H. A. (2000). The role of intuition in strategic decision making. Human Relations, 53(1), 57–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, J. Y. M., Chan, K. W., & Lam, L. W. (2013). Defining who you are not: The roles of moral dirtiness and occupational and organizational disidentification in affecting casino employee turnover intention. Journal of Business Research, 66(9), 1659–1666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lam, D. (2009). The world of Chinese gambling. Norwood: Peacock Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(2), 311–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLaren, V. V., Fugelsang, J. A., Harrigan, K. A., & Dixon, M. J. (2011). The personality of pathological gamblers: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 1057–1067.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matzler, K., Bailom, F., & Mooradian, T. A. (2007). Intuitive decision making. MIT Sloan Management Review, 49(1), 13–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mertler, C. A., & Vannatta, R. A. (2010). Advanced and multivariate statistical methods (4th ed.). Glendale, CA: Pyrczak Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myrseth, H., Brunborg, G., & Eidem, M. (2010). Differences in cognitive distortions between pathological and non-pathological gamblers with preferences for chance or skill games. Journal of Gambling Studies, 26(4), 561–569.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ndubisi, N. O., Nataraajan, R., & Chew, J. (2014). Ethical ideologies, perceived gambling value, and gambling commitment: An Asian perspective. Journal of Business Research, 67(2), 128–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowak, D., & Aloe, A. (2014). The prevalence of pathological gambling among college students: A meta-analytic synthesis, 2005–2013. Journal of Gambling Studies, 30(4), 819–843.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parke, J. (2002). Slot machine gamblers—Why are they so hard to study? Journal of Gambling Issues, (6), 1–11. http://jgi.camh.net/doi/full/10.4309/jgi.2002.6.7.

  • Petrocelli, J. V., & Sherman, S. J. (2010). Event detail and confidence in gambling: The role of counterfactual thought reactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1), 61–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prentice, C., & Woodside, A. G. (2013). Problem gamblers’ harsh gaze on casino services. Psychology and Marketing, 30(12), 1108–1123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapoport, A., & Budescu, D. V. (1997). Randomization in individual choice behavior. Psychological Review, 104(3), 603–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raylu, N., & Oei, T. P. S. (2002). Pathological gambling: A comprehensive review. Clinical Psychology Review, 22(7), 1009–1061.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 195–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seo, M. G., & Barrett, L. F. (2007). Being emotional during decision making—Good or bad? An empirical investigation. Academy of Management Journal, 50(4), 923–940.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, M., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (2005). Intuition: Myth or a decision-making tool? Management Learning, 36(3), 353–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suetens, S., & Tyran, J.-R. (2011). The gambler’s fallacy and gender. CentER Discussion Paper No. 2011-011. http://ssrn.com/paper=1759997.

  • Sundali, J., & Croson, R. (2006). Biases in casino betting: The hot hand and the gambler’s fallacy. Judgment and Decision Making, 1(1), 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tang, C. S. K., & Wu, A. M. S. (2012). Gambling-related cognitive biases and pathological gambling among youths, young adults, and mature adults in Chinese societies. Journal of Gambling Studies, 28(1), 139–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terrell, D. (1994). A test of the gambler’s fallacy: Evidence from pari-mutuel games. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 8(3), 309–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toneatto, T. (1999). Cognitive psychopathology of problem gambling. Substance Use and Misuse, 34(11), 1593–1604.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toneatto, T., & Ladoceur, R. (2003). Treatment of pathological gambling: A critical review of the literature. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 17(4), 284–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), 207–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1130.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tyszka, T., Zielonka, P., Dacey, R., & Sawicki, P. (2008). Perception of randomness and predicting uncertain events. Thinking & Reasoning, 14(1), 83–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagenaar, W. A. (1988). Paradoxes of gambling behavior. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, H. M. (2004). Is there a place for covert research methods in criminology? Graduate Journal of Social Science, 1(1), 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welte, J., Barnes, G., Tidwell, M. C., & Hoffman, J. (2008). The prevalence of problem gambling among US adolescents and young adults: Results from a national survey. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24(2), 119–133.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lawrence Hoc Nang Fong.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fong, L.H.N., So, A.S.I. & Law, R. Betting Decision Under Break-Streak Pattern: Evidence from Casino Gaming. J Gambl Stud 32, 171–185 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-015-9550-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-015-9550-1

Keywords

Navigation