Skip to main content
Log in

Why Do You Ask? The Effects of Perceived Motives on the Effort that Managers Allocate Toward Delivering Feedback

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Business and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although people are generally motivated to perform well at work, there is often ambiguity regarding whether they are meeting their organization’s standards. As such, people often seek feedback from others. To date, feedback-seeking research has emphasized the feedback seeker, identifying traits and circumstances associated with feedback seeking, whereas far less is known about this process from the feedback source’s point of view. However, we expect that feedback sources will vary in their willingness to allocate effort toward delivering feedback. Specifically, integrating the cost-value framework of feedback with self-regulatory theories of goal prioritization, we predict that effort allocated toward a feedback episode is determined by the feedback source’s perceptions of the feedback seeker’s motives for seeking feedback. Across two complementary studies, we found perceived instrumental motives (i.e., a desire to improve one’s performance) to be positively related to the amount of effort put toward delivering feedback, and perceived image enhancement motives (i.e., a desire to impress the feedback source) to be negatively related to effort allocation. Importantly, Study 1 was a field study in which managers were asked to report on a recent episode in which a subordinate had sought their feedback, and Study 2 used an experimental design in which feedback-seeking motives were manipulated. Thus, the current research makes an important contribution to the literature by considering the often overlooked role that the feedback source plays in the feedback process. Moreover, triangulation of both field and experimental data enhances both the external and internal validity of our conclusions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. When measuring perceived feedback-seeking motives, we also consider ego enhancement motives, which are characterized by a desire to be reassured about the quality of one’s work or past behavior. However, relatively little empirical research has considered ego enhancement motives (Ashford et al., 2016). Furthermore, research and theorizing on person perception indicates that perceivers’ two dominant concerns center on cooperation vs. exploitation (which aligns with our focus on image enhancement motives) and level of competence (which aligns with our focus on instrumental motives; Cuddy et al., 2008). As a result, we anticipate that ego enhancement motives may not generally be as salient of a motive, particularly when interacting with unfamiliar individuals (as in Study 2). Thus, although we developed a measure of enhancement motives in our current study for completeness, we do not make a priori hypotheses regarding its impact on feedback source’s effort or include it in our focal studies.

  2. In addition to this focal screening question, participants were asked several other questions about their employment experiences (e.g., “Are you currently self-employed?”). This was done to ensure that our screening criterion was not obvious, thereby limiting the prospect of potential participants providing false information for the purpose of gaining access to the study. Additionally, the study was only visible to individuals residing in the USA with a 95% MTurk approval rating based on at least 100 HITs.

  3. Individual differences collected were implicit person theory (Dweck, 2000), self-monitoring (Wilmot et al., 2017), and political skill (Ferris et al., 2007). These variables were collected for exploratory purposes and are therefore not discussed further. However, a correlation matrix including these individual differences, as well as perceived ego enhancement motives, is presented in the SOM (Table SOM.4).

  4. We also included three items to measure the ego enhancement factor for scale development purposes. As a final validation step, we conduct a confirmatory factor analysis using the data from the current study. The three-factor solution fit the data well (CFI = .912, RMSEA = .083). Furthermore, the three-factor model was a better fit than several plausible alternative factor structures (e.g., a one-factor solution; \(\Delta {\chi }^{2}=228.67\), \(\Delta df=3\), p < .001, CFI = .591, RMSEA = .175).

  5. We also conducted hypothesis tests including ego enhancement motives as an exploratory variable. These results are presented in the SOM.

  6. Similar to Study 1, participants were asked two additional questions about their employment experiences to help ensure that the screening criterion was not obvious. Specifically, participants were also asked “Do you currently, or have you ever, worked abroad?” and “Are you currently, or have you ever been, self-employed?” Likewise, participation was restricted to US residents with a 95% MTurk approval rating based on at least 500 HITs.

  7. We also conducted the analyses for Study 2 using only the items with an inter-rater reliability above .70. Adjusting the cut-off to .70 results in one fewer feedback sensitivity item, however, the results are the same regardless of whether this item is included or not. Therefore, we retained this item and the a priori cut-off of .65.

  8. Given the p < .10 significance level, we probed this interaction for the sake of completeness. As was the case in Study 1, the form of this interaction did not match our predictions. Likewise, the form of the interaction did not replicate the interaction observed in Study 1. Thus, we do not consider this interaction further.

References

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amy Minnikin.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 911 KB)

Appendices

Appendix 1. Perceived feedback-seeking motives scale

Instructions:

Please think about why this subordinate may have asked you for feedback and indicate your agreement with the following statements:

They sought feedback because they…

  1. 1.

    Wanted to learn more about the performance expectations that others set for them. (Instrumental)

  2. 2.

    Wanted to improve their job-related skills. (Instrumental)

  3. 3.

    Wanted to “learn the ropes” after new performance goals and expectations were set for them. (Instrumental)

  4. 4.

    Wanted to understand whether they were meeting expectations. (Instrumental)

  5. 5.

    Were searching for hints that could help them improve their performance. (Instrumental)

  6. 6.

    Were taking an opportunity to remind you of their accomplishments. (Image Enhancement)

  7. 7.

    Knew it would enhance the way you saw them. (Image Enhancement)

  8. 8.

    Were aiming to communicate to you that they are competent. (Image Enhancement)

  9. 9.

    Were trying to influence how you see them. (Image Enhancement)

  10. 10.

    Wanted to feel better about their performance. (Ego Enhancement)

  11. 11.

    Were looking for you to reassure them. (Ego Enhancement)

  12. 12.

    Were hoping you would make them feel more confident about performing a specific task. (Ego Enhancement)

Response scale: 1 (Strongly Disagree), 2 (Disagree), 3 (Neither Agree nor Disagree), 4 (Agree), 5 (Strongly Agree).

Appendix 2. Study 2 vignettes

All vignettes were preceded by these instructions:

Please imagine you are a manager at a small advertising firm. You have 15 employees that directly report to you, one of whom is named Joe. Joe’s performance is generally middle of the pack, and you have been supervising him for three years.

One day, you get the following email from Joe:

A: High Image Enhancement Motives, Low Instrumental Motives

figure a

You’ve given Joe feedback before that he hasn’t used, so you are skeptical any feedback you give him will translate into significant improvements in the quality of his logo. You also know that Joe is bragging, and hoping you will give him some praise. It seems like he is asking you for feedback in an attempt to enhance the way you think about him and his work, perhaps to put himself in a good position for a pay raise.

B: High Image Enhancement Motives, High Instrumental Motives

figure b

You know that every time you give Joe feedback, he works hard to implement it, so you feel like any feedback you give him will translate into significant improvements in the quality of his logo. You also know that Joe is bragging, and hoping you will give him some praise. It seems like he is asking you for feedback in an attempt to enhance the way you think about him and his work, perhaps to put himself in a good position for a pay raise.

C: Low Image Enhancement Motives, Low Instrumental Motives

figure c

You’ve given Joe feedback before that he hasn’t used, so you are skeptical any feedback you give him will translate into significant improvements in the quality of his logo. You also don’t think that Joe is bragging or looking for praise. It doesn’t seem like he is asking for feedback in order to enhance the way you think about him or his work.

D: Low Image Enhancement Motives, High Instrumental Motives

figure d

You know that every time you give Joe feedback, he works hard to implement it, so you feel like any feedback you give him will translate into significant improvements in the quality of his logo. You also don’t think that Joe is bragging or looking for praise. It doesn’t seem like he is asking for feedback in order to enhance the way you think about him or his work.

Appendix 3

Fig. 2
figure 2

Study 2 feedback stimulus

2

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Minnikin, A., Beck, J.W. & Shen, W. Why Do You Ask? The Effects of Perceived Motives on the Effort that Managers Allocate Toward Delivering Feedback. J Bus Psychol 37, 813–830 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-021-09776-x

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-021-09776-x

Keywords

Navigation