Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Gain–loss framing and patients’ decisions: a linguistic examination of information framing in physician–patient conversations

  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Medicine Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

When discussing risks and benefits with cancer patients, physicians could focus on losses such as mortality rates and cancer recurrence or, alternatively, gains such as survival rates and curing cancer. Previous research has shown that the way health information is framed influences individuals’ preferences and choices. We operationalized gain–loss framing as physicians’ choice of words related to gains (cancer survival), or losses (cancer mortality). In an exploratory analysis, we investigated (a) whether physicians used gain or loss words as a function of their recommendation, (b) whether physicians’ choice of words was associated with patients’ treatment choices. We analyzed transcribed consultations with male patients who had intermediate-risk prostate cancer. Using an iterative process of gathering and evaluating words, we created gain- and loss-dictionaries. The loss-dictionary included words related to cancer death and cancer progression. The gain-dictionary included words related to survival and cure. Using Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count software, we calculated the number of words related to gains and losses in each transcript. We found that physicians who recommended immediate cancer treatment for prostate cancer (vs. active surveillance) used slightly fewer words related to losses and significantly fewer words related specifically to death from cancer. Further analysis showed that loss words were associated with the patient’s choice of immediate cancer treatment. A novel method of automated text analysis showed that physicians’ use of loss words was correlated with physicians’ recommendations for cancer treatment versus active surveillance. Additionally, loss words in consultations were associated with patients’ choice of cancer treatment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akl, E. A., Oxman, A. D., Herrin, J., Vist, G. E., Terrenato, I., Sperati, F., et al. (2011). Framing of health information messages. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD006777.pub2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albertsen, P. C., Hanley, J. A., Gleason, D. F., & Barry, M. J. (1998). Competing risk analysis of men aged 55 to 74 years at diagnosis managed conservatively for clinically localized prostate cancer. JAMA, 280, 975–980.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R. L. (2018). Contextualizer (Version 1.06) [Software]. https://context.ryanb.cc.

  • Chapman, A. R., Litton, E., Chamberlain, J., & Ho, K. M. (2015). The effect of prognostic data presentation format on perceived risk among surrogate decision makers of critically ill patients: A randomized comparative trial. Journal of Critical Care, 30, 231–235.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, R., Kordts-Freudinger, R., Gawronski, B., & Strack, F. (2009). Fast and fragile: A new look at the automaticity of negation processing. Experimental Psychology, 56, 434–446.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donovan, J. L., Hamdy, F. C., Lane, J. A., Mason, M., Metcalfe, C., Walsh, E., et al. (2016). Patient-reported outcomes after monitoring, surgery, or radiotherapy for prostate cancer. New England Journal of Medicine, 375, 1425–1437.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fridman, I., Ubel, P. A., Blumenthal-Barby, J., England, C. V., Currier, J. S., Eyal, N., et al. (2020). “Cure” versus “clinical remission”: the impact of a medication description on the willingness of people living with HIV to take a medication. AIDS and Behavior, 24, 2054–2061.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gamache, D. L., McNamara, G., Mannor, M. J., & Johnson, R. E. (2015). Motivated to acquire? The impact of CEO regulatory focus on firm acquisitions. Academy of Management Journal, 58, 1261–1282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamdy, F. C., Donovan, J. L., Lane, J. A., Mason, M., Metcalfe, C., Holding, P., et al. (2016). 10-year outcomes after monitoring, surgery, or radiotherapy for localized prostate cancer. New England Journal of Medicine, 375, 1415–1424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, N. G., & Kerr, A. M. (2017). Rethinking risk: Prospect theory application in health message framing research. Health Communication, 32, 131–141.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F., & Krippendorff, K. (2007). Answering the call for a standard reliability measure for coding data. Communication Methods and Measures, 1, 77–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidenreich, A., Aus, G., Bolla, M., Joniau, S., Matveev, V. B., Schmid, H. P., et al. (2009). EAU guidelines on prostate cancer. Actas Urologicas Espanolas, 33, 113–126.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hilton, D. J. (2011). Linguistic polarity, outcome framing, and the structure of decision making: A pragmatic approach. In G. Keren (Ed.), Perspectives on framing (pp. 135–156). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogarth, R. M., & Einhorn, H. J. (1992). Order effects in belief updating: The belief-adjustment model. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 1–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes-Rovner, M., Kelly-Blake, K., Dwamena, F., Dontje, K., Henry, R. C., Olomu, A., et al. (2011). Shared decision making guidance reminders in practice (SDM-GRIP). Patient Education and Counseling, 85, 219–224.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, A., & Wang, R. J.-H. (2017). Automated text analysis for consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 44, 1274–1306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanze, D., Huang, L., Conley, M. A., & Higgins, E. T. (2017). Male and female entrepreneurs get asked different questions by VCs—And it affects how much funding they get. Harvard Business Review, June, 27.

  • Kühberger, A., & Tanner, C. (2010). Risky choice framing: Task versions and a comparison of prospect theory and fuzzy-trace theory. Journal of behavioral decision making, 23, 314–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lavine, H., Thomsen, C. J., Zanna, M. P., & Borgida, E. (1998). On the primacy of affect in the determination of attitudes and behavior: The moderating role of affective-cognitive ambivalence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 398–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76, 149–188.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, N., Idson, L. C., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). Predicting the intensity of losses vs. non-gains and non-losses vs. gains in judging fairness and value: A test of the loss aversion explanation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 527–534.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKenzie, C. R. (2004). Framing effects in inference tasks—And why they are normatively defensible. Memory & Cognition, 32, 874–885.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeil, B. J., Pauker, S. G., Sox, H. C., Jr., & Tversky, A. (1982). On the elicitation of preferences for alternative therapies. New England Journal of Medicine, 306, 1259–1262.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meyerowitz, B. E., & Chaiken, S. (1987). The effect of message framing on breast self-examination attitudes, intentions, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 500.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nan, X., Daily, K., & Qin, Y. (2018). Relative persuasiveness of gain-vs. loss-framed messages: A review of theoretical perspectives and developing an integrative framework. Review of Communication, 18, 370–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, D. J., & Jensen, J. D. (2006). The advantages of compliance or the disadvantages of noncompliance? A meta-analytic review of the relative persuasive effectiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages. Annals of the International Communication Association, 30, 1–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, D. J., & Jensen, J. D. (2007). The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed loss-framed messages for encouraging disease prevention behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Journal of health communication, 12, 623–644.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, D. J., & Jensen, J. D. (2009). The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encouraging disease detection behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Communication, 59, 296–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W. (1997). Writing about emotional experiences as a therapeutic process. Psychological Science, 8, 162–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Booth, R. J., & Francis, M. E. (2007). Linguistic inquiry and word count: LIWC [Computer software]. Austin, TX: liwc.net.

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Boyd, R. L., Jordan, K., & Blackburn, K. (2015). The development and psychometric properties of LIWC2015. Austin, TX: University of Texas at Austin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perneger, T. V., & Agoritsas, T. (2011). Doctors and patients’ susceptibility to framing bias: A randomized trial. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 26, 1411–1417.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, A. J., Rosenfeld, B., Kornblith, A. B., Gibson, C., Scher, H. I., Curley-Smart, T., et al. (2003). The memorial anxiety scale for prostate cancer. Cancer, 97, 2910–2918. https://doi.org/10.1002/cncr.11386

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, A. J., & Salovey, P. (1997). Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: The role of message framing. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 3.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, A. J., Salovey, P., Antone, C., Keough, K., & Martin, C. D. (1993). The influence of message framing on intentions to perform health behaviors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherr, K. A., Delaney, R. K., Ubel, P. A., Kahn, V., & Fagerlin A. (Unpublished). Preparing patients with early stage prostate cancer to participate in clinical appointment using a shared decision-making training video.

  • Scherr, K. A., Fagerlin, A., Hofer, T., Scherer, L. D., Holmes-Rovner, M., Williamson, L. D., et al. (2017a). Physician recommendations trump patient preferences in prostate cancer treatment decisions. Medical Decision Making, 37, 56–69.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scherr, K. A., Fagerlin, A., Williamson, L. D., Davis, J. K., Fridman, I., Atyeo, N., et al. (2017b). The physician recommendation coding system (PhyReCS) a reliable and valid method to quantify the strength of physician recommendations during clinical encounters. Medical Decision Making, 37, 46–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siminoff, L. A., & Fetting, J. H. (1989). Effects of outcome framing on treatment decisions in the real world: Impact of framing on adjuvant breast cancer decisions. Medical Decision Making, 9, 262–271.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tausczik, Y. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2010). The psychological meaning of words: LIWC and computerized text analysis methods. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29, 24–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, I., Thrasher, J. B., Aus, G., Burnett, A. L., Canby-Hagino, E. D., Cookson, M. S., et al. (2007). Guideline for the management of clinically localized prostate cancer: 2007 update. The Journal of Urology, 177, 2106–2131.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1991). Loss aversion in riskless choice: A reference-dependent model. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 106, 1039–1061.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ubel, P. A. (2015). Medical facts versus value judgments—Toward preference-sensitive guidelines. New England Journal of Medicine, 372, 2475–2477.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • van den Bergh, R. C., Korfage, I. J., & Bangma, C. H. (2012). Psychological aspects of active surveillance. Current Opinion in Urology, 22, 237–242.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van den Bergh, R. C., Roemeling, S., Roobol, M. J., Aus, G., Hugosson, J., Rannikko, A. S., et al. (2009). Outcomes of men with screen-detected prostate cancer eligible for active surveillance who were managed expectantly. European Urology, 55, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eururo.2008.09.007

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van’t Riet, J., Cox, A. D., Cox, D., Zimet, G. D., De Bruijn, G.-J., Van den Putte, B., et al. (2016). Does perceived risk influence the effects of message framing? Revisiting the link between prospect theory and message framing. Health Psychology Review, 10, 447–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veldwijk, J., Essers, B. A., Lambooij, M. S., Dirksen, C. D., Smit, H. A., & de Wit, G. A. (2016). Survival or mortality: Does risk attribute framing influence decision-making behavior in a discrete choice experiment? Value in Health, 19, 202–209.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilt, T. J., Brawer, M. K., Jones, K. M., Barry, M. J., Aronson, W. J., Fox, S., et al. (2012). Radical prostatectomy versus observation for localized prostate cancer. New England Journal of Medicine, 367, 203–213.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilt, T. J., Jones, K. M., Barry, M. J., Andriole, G. L., Culkin, D., Wheeler, T., et al. (2017). Follow-up of prostatectomy versus observation for early prostate cancer. New England Journal of Medicine, 377, 132–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zikmund-Fisher, B. J., Mayman, G., Fagerlin, A., Anderson, B., & Schulkin, J. (2014). Patient numeracy: What do patients need to recognize, think, or do with health numbers. In B. L. Anderson (Ed.), Numerical reasoning in judgments and decision making about health (pp. 80–104). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ilona Fridman.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Ilona Fridman, Angela Fagerlin, Karen A. Scherr, Laura D. Scherer, Hanna Huffstetler, and Peter A. Ubel declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Human and animal rights and informed consent

All procedures followed were in accordance with ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000. Informed consent was obtained from all patients for being included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 81 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fridman, I., Fagerlin, A., Scherr, K.A. et al. Gain–loss framing and patients’ decisions: a linguistic examination of information framing in physician–patient conversations. J Behav Med 44, 38–52 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-020-00171-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-020-00171-0

Keywords

Navigation