Skip to main content
Log in

References to Mental States in Mother–Child Conversation in the Second Year of Life

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Child and Family Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research into social influences on young children’s growing understanding of others’ inner states has highlighted the value of early conversational exchanges. This longitudinal study was aimed at investigating whether and how mothers changed their inner state words when children were 16 months olds (T1) and 4 months later (T2), and the children’ s ability to use references to mental states at 20 months. Overall language measures (e.g. number of words, mean length utterances) and internal state labels were analyzed during play interactions at T1 and T2 for the mothers, and at T2 for the children. Results highlighted that the mothers partially modified their mentalistic vocabulary over 4 months. Moreover, the use of maternal references directed to the other at T1 is linked to the children’s ability to use references with a commentary function at T2. Conversation with the caregivers during daily activity may constitute, for children, a key practice to enhance their metarepresentation skills.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adrian, J. E., Clement, R. A., & Villanueva, L. (2007). Mothers’ use of cognitive state verbs in picture-book reading and the development of children’s understanding of mind: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 78, 1052–1067.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. A., Warren, F., Andrews, B. J., Grant, J., & Todd, S. (2011). Developmental continuity in theory of mind: Speech and accuracy of belief-desire reasoning in children and adults. Child Development, 82(5), 1691–1703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W., & Baird, J. A. (2005). Why language matters for theory of mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellagamba, F., Laghi, F., Lonigro, A., Pace, C. S., & Longobardi, E. (2014). Concurrent relations between inhibitory control, vocabulary and internal state language in 18- and 24-month-old children Italian-speaking infants. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(4), 420–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornestein, M. H., Cote, L. R., & Venuti, P. (2001). Parenting beliefs and behaviors in northern and southern groups of Italian mothers of young infants. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 663–675.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., Cote, L. R., Maital, S., Painter, K., Park, S. Y., Pascual, L., et al. (2004). Crosslinguistic analysis of vocabulary in young children: Spanish, Dutch, French, Hebrew, Italian, Korean, and American English. Child Development, 75, 1115–1139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906–921.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Bellagamba, F., & Colonnesi, C. (2004). The role of declarative pointing in developing a theory of mind. Infancy, 5, 291–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camioni, L., & Longobardi, E. (2001). Noun versus verb emphasis in Italian mother-to-child speech. Journal of Child Language, 28, 773–785.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiarella, S., Kristen, S., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Sodian, B. (2012). Concurrent relations between perspective-taking skills, desire understanding and internal state vocabulary. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14, 480–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Odorico, L., Salerni, N., Cassibba, R., & Jacob, V. (1999). Stability and change of maternal speech to Italian infants from 7 to 21 months of age: A longitudinal study of its influence on early stages of language acquisition. First Language, 19, 313–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., & Herrera, C. (1997). Conflict resolution with, friends, siblings, and mothers: A longitudinal perspective. Aggressive Behavior, 23, 343–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ensor, R., Devine, R. T., Marks, A., Hughes, C. (2013). Mothers’ cognitive references to two-year-olds predict theory of mind at ages 6 and 10. Child Development. http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/cdev.12186/abstract

  • Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2008). Content or connectedness? Mother–child talk and early social understanding. Child Development, 79, 201–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2006). MacArthur-bates, communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2012). Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically-developing and late-talking toddlers. Child Deevelopment, 83(1), 203–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwyn, S. W., Acredolo, L. P., & Brown, C. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 81–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1992). How should frequency in input be measured? First Language, 12, 233–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howe, N., Rinaldi, C. M., & Recchia, H. E. (2010). Patterns in mother–child internal state discourse across four contexts. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 56(1), 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Longobardi, E. & Caselli, M. C. (1999). Gesturing in mother–child interactions. Cognitive Development, 14, 57–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuersten-Hogan, R., & McHale, J. P. (2000). Stability of emotion talk in families from the toddler to the preschool years. The Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human Development, 161(1), 115–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, E. C., & Rescorla, L. (2008). The use of psychological state words by late talkers at ages 3, 4, and 5 years. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 21–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lemche, E., Joraschky, P., & Klann-Delius, G. (2013). Predictors of early person reference development: Maternal language input, attachment and neurodevelopmental markers. Infant Behavior and Development, 36, 575–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Litowitz, B. E. (2014). Coming to terms with intersubjectivity keeping language in mind. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 62, 294–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Longobardi, E., & Renna, M. (2010). Riferimenti a stati interni nell’input rivolto a bambini con ritardo mentale e con sviluppo tipico. [Mental state talk addressed to children with mental delay and with typical development.]. Psichiatria dell’Infanzia e dell’Adolescenza, 77, 87–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longobardi, E., Rossi-Arnaud, C., Spataro, P., Putnick, D., & Bornstein, M. H. (2015). Children’s acquisition of nouns and verbs in Italian: Contrasting the role of frequency and positional salience in maternal language. Journal of Child Language, 42, 95–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Clark-Carter, D., Das Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2003). Pathways to understanding mind: Construct validity and predictive validity of maternal mind-mindedness. Child Development, 74, 1194–1211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Das Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 1715–1726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olineck, K. M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2005). Infants’ ability to distinguish between intentional and accidental actions and its relations to internal state language. Infancy, 8(1), 91–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olineck, K. M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2007). Imitation of intentional actions and internal state language in infancy predict preschool theory of mind skills. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 14–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavarini, G., de Hollanda Souza, D., & Hawak, C. (2013). Parental practice and theory of mind development. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22, 844–853.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poulin-Dubois, D., Chiarella, S., & Polonia, A. (2009). Toddlers’ vocabulary about the mind: A crosslinguistic study using the Internal State Language Questionnaire. Rivista di Psicolinguistica Applicata, 9, 35–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mothers’ mental state language and theory o f mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 734–751.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saylor, M., & Baldwin, D. (2004). Action analysis and change blindness: Possible links. In D. T. Levin (Ed.), Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adults and children (pp. 37–56). Westport, CT: Greenwood/Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C., & Carpenter, C. (2009). Maternal mental state talk and infants’ early gestural communication. Journal of Child Language, 36(5), 1053–1074.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slaughter, V., Peterson, C., & Mackintosh, E. (2007). Mind what mother says: Narrative input and theory of mind in typical children and those on the autism spectrum. Child Development, 78, 839–858.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D. K. (2004). Mental state discourse, theory of mind, and the internalization of self-other understanding. Developmental Review, 24, 159–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D. K., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., Roche, J., & Doyle, E. (2005). Theory of mind and mental state discourse during book reading and story-telling tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 81–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taumoepeau, M., & Fuffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development, 77, 465–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taumoepeau, M., & Fuffman, T. (2008). Stepping stones to others’ mind: Maternal talk relates to child mental state language and emotion understanding at 15, 24 and 33 months. Child Development, 79, 284–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: Development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Mass. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, A., Burchard, R., Deutsch, W., Jahn, K., & Nakath, J. (1996). The sibling effect in the development of personal reference: A longitudinal study. Sprache and Kognition, 15, 3–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Bartsch, K. (1994). Before belief: Children’s early psychological theory. In C. Lewis & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Children’s early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 331–354). Hove: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Emiddia Longobardi.

Appendices

Appendix 1

In order to accurately clarify our codes of the caregiver–child conversations, we reported some examples below. The terms linked to mental state talk and references are underlined.

Sixteen-Month-Old Children

  1. (1)

    Child: She puts a piece of Lego on another

    Mother: “Good Livia!” (Volitional term referred to child)

  2. (2)

    Child: He throws down the pieces of Lego

    Mother: “You’re angry today!” (Emotional term referred to child)

  3. (3)

    Child: He feeds the teddy bear

    Mother: “Pippo is really hungry” (Physiological term referred to Other)

  4. (4)

    Child: “Dadada” (She takes a little pony from the basket)

    Mother: “Well, you want to play with pony” (Volitional term referred to child)

Twenty-Month-Old Children

  1. (1)

    Child: She feeds more time the dolly

    Mother: “Lilly is hungry, too” (Physiological term referred to Other)

    Child: “Sauce” (She feeds the doll)

    Mother: “She likes a lot” (Emotional term referred to Other)

    ChildLike pappa” (Emotional term referred to Other)

  2. (2)

    Child: “Uuh booboo (He looks a picture on the book)

    Mother: “Yes, baby is crying, sad” (Emotional term referred to Other)

  3. (3)

    Child: “Pretend” (pretends to eat an apple) (Cognition term referred to self)

    Mother: “Do you like it?” (Emotional term referred to Child)

  4. (4)

    Child: He does fall the ground a glass of water

    Mother: “What a pest you are!” (Moral term referred to child)

Appendix 2

See Table 5.

Table 5 Different internal state words used by mothers (T1 and T2) and children (T2) during play interaction

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Longobardi, E., Lonigro, A. & Laghi, F. References to Mental States in Mother–Child Conversation in the Second Year of Life. J Child Fam Stud 25, 756–766 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-015-0255-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-015-0255-y

Keywords

Navigation