Skip to main content
Log in

Sunk Cost Effect in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The sunk cost effect, an interesting and well-known decision bias, is pervasive in real life and has been studied in various disciplines. In this study, we modified a task exemplifying the sunk cost effect and used it to evaluate this behavior in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). The control group exhibited a typical sunk cost effect in our task. We found that the sunk cost effect was lower in the ASD group than in the control group. The results agree with previous evidence of reduced sensitivity to context stimuli in individuals with ASD and extend this finding to the context of the sunk cost effect. Our findings are useful in addressing the practical implications on their socioeconomic behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arkes, H. R., & Blumer, C. (1985). The psychology of sunk cost. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35(1), 124–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Augenblick, N. (2016). The sunk-cost fallacy in penny auctions. The Review of Economic Studies, 83(1), 58–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Is asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism necessarily a disability? Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 489–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2010). The influence of affect on higher level cognition: A review of research on interpretation, judgement, decision making and reasoning. Cognition & Emotion, 24(4), 561–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogdanov, M., Ruff, C. C., & Schwabe, L. (2017). Transcranial stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex increases the impact of past expenses on decision-making. Cerebral Cortex, 27(2), 1094–1102.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boulter, C., Freeston, M., South, M., & Rodgers, J. (2014). Intolerance of uncertainty as a framework for understanding anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(6), 1391–1402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bray, S., Shimojo, S., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human medial orbitofrontal cortex is recruited during experience of imagined and real rewards. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(5), 2506–2512.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, M., Ashwin, C., & Lewton, M. (2017). Brief report: Intuitive and reflective reasoning in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(8), 2595–2601.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, M., Chapman, E., & Ashwin, C. (2014). Adolescents with autism spectrum disorder show a circumspect reasoning bias rather than ‘jumping-to-conclusions’. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(3), 513–520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, M., Lewton, M., & Ashwin, C. (2016). Reasoning on the autism spectrum: A dual process theory account. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(6), 2115–2125.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C. F. (2011). Behavioral game theory: Experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Conlon, D. E., & Garland, H. (1993). The role of project completion information in resource allocation decisions. Academy of Management Journal, 36(2), 402–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Martino, B., Harrison, N. A., Knafo, S., Bird, G., & Dolan, R. J. (2008). Explaining enhanced logical consistency during decision making in autism. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(42), 10746–10750.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2007). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: How low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. Neuroscientist, 13(6), 580–593.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desir, M. P., & Karatekin, C. (2018). Parental reactions to parent-and sibling-directed aggression within a domestic violence context. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 2018, 1359104518755219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doya, K. (2008). Modulators of decision making. Nature Neuroscience, 11(4), 410–416.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, G. D., Baron-Cohen, S., & Skylark, W. J. (2017). People with autism spectrum conditions make more consistent decisions. Psychological Science, 28(8), 1067–1076.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, D., Pommerenke, K., Lukose, R., Milam, G., & Huberman, B. A. (2007). Searching for the sunk cost fallacy. Experimental Economics, 10(1), 79–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fujino, J., Fujimoto, S., Kodaka, F., Camerer, C. F., Kawada, R., Tsurumi, K., et al. (2016b). Neural mechanisms and personality correlates of the sunk cost effect. Scientific Reports, 6, 33171.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fujino, J., Hirose, K., Tei, S., Kawada, R., Tsurumi, K., Matsukawa, N., et al. (2016a). Ambiguity aversion in schizophrenia: An fMRI study of decision-making under risk and ambiguity. Schizophrenia Research, 178(1), 94–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fujino, J., Tei, S., Hashimoto, R. I., Itahashi, T., Ohta, H., Kanai, C., et al. (2017a). Attitudes toward risk and ambiguity in patients with autism spectrum disorder. Molecular Autism, 8(1), 45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fujino, J., Tei, S., Jankowski, K. F., Kawada, R., Murai, T., & Takahashi, H. (2017b). Role of spontaneous brain activity in explicit and implicit aspects of cognitive flexibility under socially conflicting situations: A resting-state fMRI study using fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations. Neuroscience, 367, 60–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haller, A., & Schwabe, L. (2014). Sunk costs in the human brain. Neuroimage, 97, 127–133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hallsson, B. G., Siebner, H. R., & Hulme, O. J. (2018). Fairness, fast and slow: A review of dual process models of fairness. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 89, 49–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartley, C. A., & Phelps, E. A. (2012). Anxiety and decision-making. Biological Psychiatry, 72(2), 113–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Itahashi, T., Yamada, T., Watanabe, H., Nakamura, M., Ohta, H., Kanai, C., et al. (2015). Alterations of local spontaneous brain activity and connectivity in adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Molecular Autism, 6(1), 1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism and asperger syndrome faster than normal on the embedded figures test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 527–534.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Just, D. R., & Wansink, B. (2011). The flat-rate pricing paradox: Conflicting effects of “all-you-can-eat” buffet pricing. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 93(1), 193–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kang, M. J., Rangel, A., Camus, M., & Camerer, C. F. (2011). Hypothetical and real choice differentially activate common valuation areas. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(2), 461–468.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Krzyzanowska, K., Collins, P. J., & Hahn, U. (2017). Between a conditional’s antecedent and its consequent: Discourse coherence vs. probabilistic relevance. Cognition, 164, 199–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259–289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Locey, M. L., Jones, B. A., & Rachlin, H. (2011). Real and hypothetical rewards. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(6), 552–564.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luke, L., Clare, I. C., Ring, H., Redley, M., & Watson, P. (2012). Decision-making difficulties experienced by adults with autism spectrum conditions. Autism, 16(6), 612–621.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magalhães, P., White, K. G., Stewart, T., Beeby, E., & van der Vliet, W. (2012). Suboptimal choice in nonhuman animals: Rats commit the sunk cost error. Learning & Behavior, 40(2), 195–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mair, P., Schoenbrodt, F., & Wilcox, R. (2016). WRS2: Wilcox robust estimation and testing.

  • Matsuoka, K., & Kim, Y. (2006). Japanese adult reading test (JART). Tokyo: Shinkou-Igaku Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsuoka, K., Uno, M., Kasai, K., Koyama, K., & Kim, Y. (2006). Estimation of premorbid IQ in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease using Japanese ideographic script (Kanji) compound words: Japanese version of National Adult Reading Test. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 60(3), 332–339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minassian, A., Paulus, M., Lincoln, A., & Perry, W. (2007). Adults with autism show increased sensitivity to outcomes at low error rates during decision-making. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(7), 1279–1288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mobbs, D., & Kim, J. J. (2015). Neuroethological studies of fear, anxiety, and risky decision-making in rodents and humans. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 5, 8–15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mosner, M. G., Kinard, J. L., McWeeny, S., Shah, J. S., Markiewitz, N. D., Damiano-Goodwin, C. R., et al. (2017). Vicarious effort-based decision-making in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(10), 2992–3006.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., & Belleville, S. (1993). A study of perceptual analysis in a high-level autistic subject with exceptional graphic abilities. Brain and Cognition, 23(2), 279–309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mussey, J. L., Travers, B. G., Klinger, L. G., & Klinger, M. R. (2015). Decision-making skills in ASD: Performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. Autism Research, 8(1), 105–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Philip, R. C., Dauvermann, M. R., Whalley, H. C., Baynham, K., Lawrie, S. M., & Stanfield, A. C. (2012). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the fMRI investigation of autism spectrum disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(2), 901–942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(5), 777–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., Saksida, L., Alcántara, J., & Weisblatt, E. (2003). Towards an understanding of the mechanisms of weak central coherence effects: Experiments in visual configural learning and auditory perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 375–386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(5), 733–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pulcu, E., Trotter, P., Thomas, E., McFarquhar, M., Juhász, G., Sahakian, B., et al. (2014). Temporal discounting in major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine, 44(9), 1825–1834.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robic, S., Sonie, S., Fonlupt, P., Henaff, M.-A., Touil, N., Coricelli, G., et al. (2015). Decision-making in a changing world: A study in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(6), 1603–1613.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D. (2011). The roles of dopamine and serotonin in decision making: Evidence from pharmacological experiments in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(1), 114–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, S., Robbert, T., & Straus, L. (2015). On the sunk-cost effect in economic decision-making: A meta-analytic review. Business Research, 8(1), 99–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruderman, L., Ehrlich, D. B., Roy, A., Pietrzak, R. H., Harpaz-Rotem, I., & Levy, I. (2016). Posttraumatic stress symptoms and aversion to ambiguous losses in combat veterans. Depression and Anxiety, 33(7), 606–613.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, P., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2016). Emotional decision-making in autism spectrum disorder: The roles of interoception and alexithymia. Molecular Autism, 7(1), 43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sharp, C., Monterosso, J., & Montague, P. R. (2012). Neuroeconomics: A bridge for translational research. Biological Psychiatry, 72(2), 87–92.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sleesman, D. J., Conlon, D. E., McNamara, G., & Miles, J. E. (2012). Cleaning up the big muddy: A meta-analytic review of the determinants of escalation of commitment. Academy of Management Journal, 55(3), 541–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & Stanovich, P. J. (2010). A framework for critical thinking, rational thinking, and intelligence. In D. Preiss & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Innovations in educational psychology: Perspectives on learning, teaching and human development (pp. 195–237). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strough, J., Mehta, C. M., McFall, J. P., & Schuller, K. L. (2008). Are older adults less subject to the sunk-cost fallacy than younger adults? Psychological Science, 19(7), 650–652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, H. (2013). Molecular neuroimaging of emotional decision-making. Neuroscience Research, 75(4), 269–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, H.-T., & Yates, J. F. (1995). Sunk cost effects: The influences of instruction and future return estimates. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 63(3), 311–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tei, S., Fujino, J., Hashimoto, R. I., Itahashi, T., Ohta, H., Kanai, C., et al. (2018). Inflexible daily behaviour is associated with the ability to control an automatic reaction in autism spectrum disorder. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 8082.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tei, S., Fujino, J., Kawada, R., Jankowski, K. F., Kauppi, J. P., van den Bos, W.,et al (2017). Collaborative roles of temporoparietal junction and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in different types of behavioural flexibility. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 6415.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. (1980). Toward a positive theory of consumer choice. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 1(1), 39–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. (1994). The winner’s curse: Paradoxes and anomalies of economic life. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thiébaut, F. I., White, S. J., Walsh, A., Klargaard, S. K., Wu, H.-C., Rees, G., et al. (2016). Does faux pas detection in adult autism reflect differences in social cognition or decision-making abilities? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(1), 103–112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5(4), 297–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeng, J., Zhang, Q., Chen, C., Yu, R., & Gong, Q. (2013). An fMRI study on sunk cost effect. Brain Research, 1519, 63–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to extend their gratitude to the research team of the Medical Institute of Developmental Disabilities Research at Showa University for their assistance in data acquisition. This work was supported by grants-in-aid for scientific research A (24243061), C (17K10326), Young Scientists B (17K16398), and on Innovative Areas (23120009, 16H06572), from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan (MEXT); SENSHIN Medical Research Foundation; and Takeda Science Foundation. A part of this study is the result of the Strategic Research Program for Brain Sciences by agency for medical research and development (JP18dm0107151), “Research and development of technology for enhancing functional recovery of elderly and disabled people based on non-invasive brain imaging and robotic assistive devices”, the Commissioned Research of National Institute of Information and Communications Technology, JAPAN, and the Joint Usage/Research Program of Medical Institute of Developmental Disabilities Research, Showa University. These agencies had no further role in the study design, the collection, analysis and interpretation of data, the writing of the report, or in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JF, ST, TI, YA, HO, RH, MN, NK, and HT designed research; JF, ST, TI, and CK participated in the data acquisition; JF, YA HO, CK, MK, MN, and NK were in charge of the clinical assessment. JF, and ST analyzed data; TI, YA HO, CK, MK, RH, MN, NK, and HT helped with interpretation of data. JF, ST, TI, YA, HO, CK, MK, RH, MN, NK, and HT wrote the paper. All authors have made intellectual contribution to the work and approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hidehiko Takahashi.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 53 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fujino, J., Tei, S., Itahashi, T. et al. Sunk Cost Effect in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 49, 1–10 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3679-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3679-6

Keywords

Navigation