Skip to main content
Log in

Dual Pathways from Reactive Aggression to Depressive Symptoms in Children: Further Examination of the Failure Model

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The failure model posits that peer rejection and poor academic performance are dual pathways in the association between early aggressive behavior and subsequent depressive symptoms. We examined this model using an accelerated longitudinal design while also incorporating proactive and reactive aggression and gender moderation. Children in 1st, 3rd, and 5th grades (n = 912; ages 6–12; 48% female) were rated three times annually by their primary teachers on measures of proactive and reactive aggression, peer rejection, academic performance, and depressive symptoms. Using Bayesian cross-classified estimation to account for nested and planned-missing data, path models were estimated to examine whether early reactive aggression predicted subsequent peer rejection and academic performance, and whether these, in turn, predicted subsequent depressive symptoms. From 1st to 3rd grade, reactive aggression predicted peer rejection (not academic performance), proactive aggression predicted academic performance (not peer rejection), and academic performance and peer rejection both predicted depressive symptoms. From 3rd to 5th grade, however, neither peer rejection nor academic performance predicted subsequent depressive symptoms. Results were not moderated by gender. Overall, these findings provide mixed and limited support for the failure model among school-age children. Early reactive aggression may be a key risk factor for social problems, whereas proactive aggression may be linked to improved academic functioning. The “dual pathways” of peer rejection and academic performance may operate during early but not later elementary school. Limitations and implications are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1: Results of final model

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. These teachers routinely evaluate and report on their students in study domains (i.e., academic performance, social-emotional functioning, behavior problems) as part of their professional duties and school procedures. Further, the data were de-identified to the researchers and only presented back to school staff in aggregate descriptive statistics. For these reasons the institutional review board and the school administration determined that teacher participation was unlikely to affect individual students. Thus, parent consent and youth assent were not required; teacher consent was sufficient.

  2. True mediation is established when variable X at Time T is associated with variable Y at Time T + 2, and both are associated with variable M at time T + 1 (Kline 2016; Little 2013). Given the accelerated design, the direct paths from first to fifth grade were not estimable because no participants were observed at both time points. Thus, only the indirect paths were estimated, and direct effects could not be examined.

  3. Because software limitations allowed no more than two cross-classified occasions, these models were specified in several ways: piecewise models containing two adjacent occasions at a time (1st to 3rd; 3rd to 5th); and whole models containing all three occasions with the cross-classified adjustments applied to one interval or the other. Because single models are more parsimonious than piecewise models, and because the 3rd and 5th grade variables were most of interest as outcomes, we treated this particular model—i.e., 1st to [3rd to 5th] (brackets denote cross-classified adjustments)—as the primary model for interpretation. The other versions were estimated as secondary evidence to assess the robustness of results. In some model variants (but not the primary/final model), model convergence required fixing means to zero due to variance restriction imposed by cross-classification adjustments.

  4. As mentioned in the previous footnote, three alternative variants tested to overcome software limitations: 1st to 3rd piecewise model (n = 647); 3rd to 5th piecewise model (n = 646); and whole model with cross-classification adjustment applied to1st to 3rd (n = 912). Prior to these analyses, all path estimates from the initial model (Fig. 1, Table 3) were first classified into three mutually exclusive groups based on their significance: (a) significant effect (95% CIs ≠ 0); (b) significant direction (one-tailed ps < .05 but 95% CIs = 0); and (c) nonsignificant (one-tailed ps ≥ .05). From the original model to all three alternative models, there were no meaningful changes in these significance classifications for any parameter estimates. A few negligible changes were observed in covariate paths for gender across models (e.g., from nonsignificant to significant), but these were explained by the omission of other regression path terms (as necessitated by piecewise model building), upon which the covariate effect is conditional.

References

  • Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brennan, L. M., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., & Wilson, M. (2012). Longitudinal predictors of school-age academic achievement: Unique contributions of toddler-age aggression, oppositionality, inattention, and hyperactivity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 1289–1300.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Buhi, E. R., Goodson, P., & Neilands, T. B. (2008). Out of sight, not out of mind: Strategies for handling missing data. American Journal of Health Behavior, 32(1), 83–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, J. D., Loeber, R., Lahey, B. B., & Rathouz, P. J. (2005). Developmental transitions among affective and behavioral disorders in adolescent boys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 1200–1210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, S. B., Spieker, S., Burchinal, M., & Poe, M. D. (2006). Trajectories of aggression from toddlerhood to age 9 predict academic and social functioning through age 12. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 791–800.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Card, N., & Little, T. (2006). Proactive and reactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis of differential relations with psychosocial adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 466–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, X., Huang, X., Chang, L., Wang, L., & Li, D. (2010). Aggression, social competence, and academic achievement in Chinese children: A 5-year longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 583–592.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coie, J., Terry, R., Lenox, K., & Lochman, J. (1995). Childhood peer rejection and aggression as predictors of stable patterns of adolescent disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 697–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Day, D. M., Bream, L. A., & Pal, A. (1992). Proactive and reactive aggression: An analysis of subtypes based on teacher perceptions. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21, 210–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dishion, T. J., & Patterson, G. R. (1997). The timing and severity of antisocial behavior: Three hypotheses within an ecological framework.

  • Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children's peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1146–1158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. A., Lochman, J. E., Harnish, J. D., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Reactive and proactive aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 37–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, S. C., Fite, P. J., Hendrickson, M. L., Rubens, S. L., & Mages, A. K. (2015). The role of reactive aggression in the link between hyperactive-impulsive behaviors and peer rejection in adolescents. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 46, 903–912.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, S. C., Blossom, J. B., Canter, K. S., Poppert-Cordts, K., Kanine, R., Garcia, A., & Roberts, M. C. (2016a). Self-reported emotion reactivity among early-adolescent girls: Evidence for convergent and discriminant validity in an urban community sample. Behavior Therapy, 47, 299–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, S. C., Pederson, C. A., Fite, P. J., Blossom, J. B., & Cooley, J. L. (2016b). Teacher-reported irritable and defiant dimensions of oppositional defiant disorder: Social, behavioral, and academic correlates. School Mental Health, 8, 292–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fite, P. J., Stoppelbein, L., & Greening, L. (2009). Proactive and reactive aggression in a child psychiatric inpatient population. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 38, 199–205.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fite, P. J., Colder, C. R., & Pelham, W. E. (2010). A factor-analytic approach to distinguish pure and co-occurring dimensions of proactive and reactive aggression. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35, 578–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fite, P. J., Hendrickson, M., Rubens, S., Gabrielli, J., & Evans, S. (2013). The role of peer rejection in the link between reactive aggression and academic performance. Child and Youth Care Forum, 42, 193–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fite, P. J., Rubens, S. L., Preddy, T. M., Raine, A., & Pardini, D. A. (2014). Reactive/proactive aggression and the development of internalizing problems in males: The moderating effect of parent and peer relationships. Aggressive Behavior, 40, 69–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fite, P. J., Craig, J. L., Colder, C. R., Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (2016). Proactive and reactive aggression. In R. J. R. Levesque (Ed.), Encyclopedia of adolescence. Cham: Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-32132-5_211-2.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gooren, E. M., van Lier, P. A., Stegge, H., Terwogt, M. M., & Koot, H. M. (2011). The development of conduct problems and depressive symptoms in early elementary school children: The role of peer rejection. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, 245–253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Granic, I., & Patterson, G. R. (2006). Toward a comprehensive model of antisocial development: A dynamic systems approach. Psychological Review, 113, 101–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., McGee, R., & Angell, K. E. (1998). Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: Emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 128–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, J. W., & Claussen, A. H. (2017). Evidence base update for psychosocial treatments for disruptive behaviors in children. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 46(4), 477–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (4th ed.). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Little, T. D., Henrich, C. C., Jones, S. M., & Hawley, P. H. (2003). Disentangling the “whys” from the “whats” of aggressive behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 122–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loth, A. K., Drabick, D. A. G., Leibenluft, E., & Hulvershorn, L. A. (2014). Do childhood externalizing disorders predict adult depression? A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 1115–1116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsman, M., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2017). Three insights from a Bayesian interpretation of the one-sided P value. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 77(3), 529–539.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathieson, L. C., & Crick, N. R. (2010). Reactive and proactive subtypes of relational and physical aggression in middle childhood: Links to concurrent and longitudinal adjustment. School Psychology Review, 39, 601–611.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarty, C. A., Mason, W. A., Kosterman, R., Hawkins, J. D., Lengua, L. J., & McCauley, E. (2008). Adolescent school failure predicts later depression among girls. Journal of Adolescent Health, 43, 180–187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, M. T., Hubbard, J. A., McAuliffe, M. D., Rubin, R. M., & Dearing, K. F. (2006). Childhood aggression, depressive symptoms, and peer rejection: The mediational model revisited. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 240–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, M. T., Hubbard, J. A., Rubin, R. M., & McAuliffe, M. D. (2008). The relation between childhood aggression and depressive symptoms: The unique and joint mediating roles of peer rejection and peer victimization. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54, 316–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, B., & Asparouhov, T. (2012). Bayesian structural equation modeling: A more flexible representation of substantive theory. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 313–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nocentini, A., Calamai, G., & Menesini, E. (2012). Codevelopment of delinquent and depressive symptoms across adolescence: Time-invariant and time-varying effects of school and social failure. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41, 746–759.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1994). The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 424–443.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Obradović, J., Burt, K. B., & Masten, A. S. (2010). Testing a dual cascade model linking competence and symptoms over 20 years from childhood to adulthood. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39, 90–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Panak, W. F., & Garber, J. (1992). Role of aggression, rejection, and attributions in the prediction of depression in children. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 145–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, G. R., & Capaldi, D. M. (1990). A mediational model for boys’ depressed mood. In J. E. Rolf, A. Masten, D. Ciccheti, K. Neuchterlein, & S. Weintraub (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 141–163). Boston: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, G. R., & Stoolmiller, M. (1991). Replications of a dual failure model for boys' depressed mood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 491–498.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, G. R., & Yoerger, K. (1993). Developmental models for delinquent behavior. In S. Hodgins, S. (Ed.), Mental disorder and crime (pp. 140–172). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

  • Patterson, G. R., DeBaryshe, B. D., & Ramsey, E. (1989). A developmental perspective on antisocial behavior. American Psychologist, 44, 329–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pedersen, S., Vitaro, F., Barker, E. D., & Borge, A. I. H. (2007). The timing of middle-childhood peer rejection and friendship: Linking early behavior to early-adolescent adjustment. Child Development, 78, 1037–1051.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Polman, H., de Castro, B. O., Koops, W., van Boxtel, H. W., & Merk, W. W. (2007). A meta-analysis of the distinction between reactive and proactive aggression in children and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 522–535.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, R. F. (2007). Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42, 185–227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salmivalli, C., & Helteenvuori, T. (2007). Reactive, but not proactive aggression predicts victimization among boys. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 198–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D., Gorman, A. H., Duong, M. T., & Nakamoto, J. (2008). Peer relations and academic achievement as interacting predictors of depressive symptoms during middle childhood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 289–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. L., Rose, A. J., & Schwartz-Mette, R. A. (2010). Relational and overt aggression in childhood and adolescence: Clarifying mean-level gender differences and associations with peer acceptance. Social Development, 19, 243–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoolmiller, M., Kim, H. K., & Capaldi, D. M. (2005). The course of depressive symptoms in men from early adolescence to young adulthood: Identifying latent trajectories and early predictors. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 331–345.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • van Lier, P. A. C., & Koot, H. M. (2010). Developmental cascades of peer relations and symptoms of externalizing and internalizing problems from kindergarten to fourth-grade elementary school. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 569–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Lier, P. A. C., Vitaro, F., Barker, E. D., Brendgen, M., Tremblay, R. E., & Boivin, M. (2012). Peer victimization, poor academic achievement, and the link between childhood externalizing and internalizing problems. Child Development, 83, 1775–1788.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2002). Reactively and proactively aggressive children: Antecedent and subsequent characteristics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 495–506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, B. A., Jarrett, M. A., & Ollendick, T. H. (2013). Self-regulation deficits explain the link between reactive aggression and internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in children. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 35, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zyphur, M. J., & Oswald, F. L. (2015). Bayesian estimation and inference: A user’s guide. Journal of Management, 41, 390–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

For their helpful feedback and consultation on this study, we thank Michael Roberts, Christopher Cushing, Anne Williford, Eric Vernberg, and Jennifer Blossom. We are also grateful to the school administrators and teachers for their research partnership and participation. This work was completed as part of the first author’s doctoral dissertation, with support from the American Psychological Foundation (Elizabeth Munsterberg Koppitz Child Psychology Graduate Fellowship, SCE) and the University of Kansas (Lillian Jacobey Baur Early Childhood Fellowship and Doctoral Student Research Fund, SCE; Faculty Research Fund, PJF).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Spencer C. Evans.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

Ethical approval was obtained from the institutional review board at the University of Kansas (Human Subjects Committee - Lawrence #20175)

Informed Consent

Informed consent was collected.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Evans, S.C., Fite, P.J. Dual Pathways from Reactive Aggression to Depressive Symptoms in Children: Further Examination of the Failure Model. J Abnorm Child Psychol 47, 85–97 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0426-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0426-6

Keywords

Navigation