Skip to main content
Log in

The Nature of Social Positive Illusory Bias: Reflection of Social Impairment, Self-Protective Motivation, or Poor Executive Functioning?

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study examined if a social positive illusory bias (PIB) is: a) simply a reflection of low adult-rated social acceptance, b) evident when children’s perceived social acceptance is measured implicitly, and c) directly relates to impaired executive functioning (EF). Participants were 8 to 12 year-old children (N = 120; 55 boys and 65 girls) with and without clinical symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Ratings of the child’s social acceptance were obtained from an adult and the child using the Self-Perception Profile for Children (Harter 2012); social bias was calculated as the discrepancy between standardized adult- and child-ratings. Children also completed a reaction time measure to assess implicit perceptions of social acceptance and a battery of EF measures. Depression symptoms were assessed based on parent report. Group comparisons were focused on the presence or absence of social PIB rather than on ADHD diagnostic status. Relative to non-PIB children, those with a social PIB were significantly higher in self-reported social acceptance, but significantly lower on adult reports and implicit perceived social acceptance; these children also were significantly higher in depression symptoms. EF impairments were indirectly related to social PIB as a function of adult-rated social impairment. Results suggest that social PIB is not merely a reflection of low adult-ratings of social acceptance. However, the high explicit self-reports of social acceptance in children with a social PIB are not fully consistent with their implicit self-perceptions of social acceptance. Results are discussed in light of the self-protective and cognitive deficits hypotheses regarding the nature of social PIB.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. One additional child from the original sample was excluded due to non-compliance with study procedures which invalidated relevant measures. Four additional participants (3 with ADHD and 1 control) were excluded because their accuracy on the IAT was more than 2.5 standard deviations below the mean accuracy for children without ADHD (63 %).

  2. Parent and teacher reports of symptoms were based on children’s unmedicated behavior; however for a subset of medicated children (n = 10), teachers did not have an opportunity to observe the child unmedicated and provided symptom ratings based on medicated behavior.

  3. Of the 65 control children, 63 had no more than 3 symptoms in either ADHD symptom domain and no more than 4 symptoms total. The remaining 2 subjects classified as controls had 4 symptoms of inattention or hyperactivity-impulsivity (but not both); neither of these children displayed a social PIB. Because of the dimensional approach to data analysis, these additional control subjects were retained, even though their symptom levels were slightly elevated.

  4. For 16 % of participants, teachers declined to complete study measures; when teacher data was not available, parent data was used instead. For children who had multiple teachers, the teacher who was most familiar with the child was selected to provide ratings.

  5. Some researchers have measured a social PIB as the discrepancy between unstandardized adult- and child-ratings and have defined extreme PIB as a score greater than one (Linnea et al. 2012; McQuade et al. 2011). Using this alternative definition, the –PIB group was too small to be examined (n = 6). However, the +PIB (n = 25) and No-Bias (n = 83) group differences were consistent for adult-rated (p < 0.001) and implicit social acceptance (p = 0.033), depression (p = 0.003), and EF (p = 0.001). For self-perceived social acceptance, the +PIB group (M = 3.42, SD = 0.61) was higher than the No-Bias group (M = 3.24, SD = 0.58) but this difference was not significant (p = 0.174). This non-significant effect may be due to a reduction in power from the smaller sample size of the +PIB group. Additionally, the non-standardized discrepancy score was more strongly associated with adult-rated social acceptance (r = −0.72) than child rated acceptance (r = 0.40). This may bias results such that effects are stronger for adult-rated measures but weaker for child-rated measures (De Los Reyes and Kazdin 2004).

References

  • Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit attitudes: evidence of race evaluations form ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychological Science, 17, 53–58. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01664.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beer, J. S. (2012). This time with motivation: the implications of social neuroscience for research on motivated self- and other-perception. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 38–45. doi:10.1007/s11031-011-9259-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). The role immaturity in human development. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 153–169. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.122.2.153.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, C., Prior, M., & Kinsella, G. (2002). The relationship between executive function abilities, adaptive behaviour, and academic achievement in children with externalising behaviour problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 785–796. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00084.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., & Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: a normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 347–368. doi:10.1037/a0014211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Los Reyes, A., & Kazdin, A. E. (2004). Measuring informant discrepancies in clinical child research. Psychological Assessment, 16, 330–334. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.16.3.330.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, M. B., & Milich, R. (1997). Effects of positive feedback on the social interactions of boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a test of the self-protective hypothesis. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26, 256–265. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2603_4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emeh, C. C., & Mikami, A. Y. (2014). The influence of parent behaviors on positive illusory bias in children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 18, 456–465. doi:10.1177/1087054712441831.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emeh, C. C., Mikami, A. Y., & Teachman, B. A. (2015). Explicit and implicit positive illusory bias in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Attention Disorders. doi:10.1177/1087054715612261. Advance online publication

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fried, R., Hirshfeld-Becker, D., Petty, C., Batchelder, H., & Biederman, J. (2015). How informative is the CANTAB to assess executive functioning in children with ADHD? A controlled study. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19, 468–475. doi:10.1177/1087054712457038.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the implicit association test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1022–1038. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: an improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gresham, F. M., Lane, K. L., MacMillan, D. L., Bocian, K. M., & Ward, S. L. (2000). Effects of positive and negative illusory biases: comparisons across social and academic self-concept domains. Journal of School Psychology, 38, 151–175. doi:10.1016/S0022-4405(99)00042-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harter (2012). Self-perception profile for children: Manual and questionnaires. Denver: University of Denver, Department of Psychology.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heath, N. L., & Glen, T. (2005). Positive illusory bias and the self-protective hypothesis in children with learning disabilities. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 272–281. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3402_6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoza, B., Pelham Jr., W. E., Dobbs, J., Owens, J. S., & Pillow, D. R. (2002). Do boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder have positive illusory self-concepts? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 268–278. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoza, B., Murray-Close, D., Arnold, L. E., Hinshaw, S. P., & Hechtman, L. (2010). Time- dependent changes in positively biased self-perceptions of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a developmental psychopathology perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 375–390. doi:10.1017/S095457941000012X.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hoza, B., Vaughn, A., Waschbusch, D. A., Murray-Close, D., & McCabe, G. (2012). Can children with ADHD be motivated to reduce bias in self-reports of competence? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80, 245–254. doi:10.1037/a0027299.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hoza, B., McQuade, J. D., Murray-Close, D., Shoulberg, E., Molina, B. G., Arnold, L. E., et al. (2013). Does childhood positive self-perceptual bias mediate adolescent risky behavior in youth from the MTA study? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 81, 846–858. doi:10.1037/a0033536.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Huang-Pollock, C. L., Mikami, A. Y., Pfiffner, L., & McBurnett, K. (2009). Can executive functions explain the relationship between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and social adjustment? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 679–691. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9302-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hymel, S., Bowker, A., & Woody, E. (1993). Aggressive versus withdrawn unpopular children: variations in peer and self-perceptions in multiple domains. Child Development, 64, 879–896. doi:10.2307/1131224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jurado, M. B., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17, 213–233. doi:10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman brief intelligence test (second ed.). Bloomington: Pearson, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurman, J., Rothschild-Yakar, L., Angel, R., & Katz, M. (2015). How good am I? Implicit and explicit self-esteem as a function of perceived parenting styles among children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders. doi:10.1177/1087054715569599. Advance online publication

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laird, R. D., & Weems, C. F. (2011). The equivalence of regression models using difference scores and models using separate scores for each informant: implications for the study of informant discrepancies. Psychological Assessment, 23, 388–397. doi:10.1037/a0021926.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linnea, K., Hoza, B., Tomb, M., & Kaiser, N. (2012). Does a positive bias relate to social behavior in children with ADHD? Behavior Therapy, 43, 862–875. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2012.05.004.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Luciana, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2002). Assessment of neuropsychological function in children through the Cambridge neuropsychological testing automated battery (CANTAB): normative performance in 4 to 12 year-olds. Developmental Neuropsychology, 22, 595–624. doi:10.1207/S15326942DN2203_3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGlynn, S. M., & Schacter, D. L. (1989). Unawareness of deficits in neuropsychological syndromes. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 143–205. doi:10.1080/01688638908400882.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McQuade, J. D., Tomb, M., Hoza, B., Waschbusch, D. A., Hurt, E. A., & Vaughn, A. J. (2011). Cognitive deficits and positively biased self-perceptions in children with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 307–319. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9453-7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McQuade, J. D., Murray-Close, D., Shoulberg, E. K., & Hoza, B. (2013). Working memory and social functioning in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 422–435. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mikami, A. E., Calhoun, C. D., & Abikoff, H. B. (2010). Positive illusory bias and response to treatment among children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 39, 373–385. doi:10.1080/15374411003691735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding and using the implicit association test: II. Method variables and construct validity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 166–180. doi:10.1177/0146167204271418.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohan, J. L., & Johnston, C. (2002). Are the performance overestimates given by boys with ADHD self-protective? Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 31, 230–241. doi:10.1207/153744202753604502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owens, J. S., Goldfine, M. E., Evangelista, N. M., Hoza, B., & Kaiser, N. M. (2007). A critical review of self-perceptions and the positive illusory bias in children with ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 10, 335–351. doi:10.1007/s10567-007-0027-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Owensworth, T. L., McFarland, K. M., & Young, R. (2002). The investigation of factors underlying deficits in self-awareness and self-regulation. Brain Injury, 16, 291–309. doi:10.1080/02699050110103986.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E. M., Greenslade, K. E., & Milich, R. (1992). Teacher ratings of DSM-III—R symptoms for the disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 210–218. doi:10.1097/00004583-199203000-00006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelham, W. E., Fabiano, G. A., & Massetti, G. M. (2005). Evidence-based assessment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 449–476. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, D. (1984). The illusion of incompetence among academically competent children. Child Development, 55, 2000–2016. doi:10.2307/1129775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scanlon, G., McEnteggart, C., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2014). Using the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP) to assess implicit gender bias and self-esteem in typically-developing children and children with ADHD and with dyslexia. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 19, 48–59. doi:10.1037/h0100577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shad, M. U., Tamminga, C. A., Cullum, M., Haas, G. L., & Keshavan, M. S. (2006). Insight and frontal cortical function in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophrenia Research, 86, 54–70. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.06.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., & Schwab-Stone, M. E. (2000). NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 28–38. doi:10.1097/00004583-200001000-00014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, H. R., Miyake, A., & Hankin, B. L. (2015). Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 328. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00328.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, E. N., Owens, E. B., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2012). Is the positive illusory bias illusory? Examining discrepant self-perceptions of competence in girls with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 987–998. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9615-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomaes, S., Bushman, B. J., de Castro, B. O., Cohen, G. L., & Denissen, J. A. (2009). Reducing narcissistic aggression by buttressing self-esteem: an experimental field study. Psychological Science, 20, 1536–1542. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02478.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K. A., & Charak, D. (2008). Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children: I. Latent structure. Developmental Psychology, 44, 575–587. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.575.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the families and teachers who generously participated in this study. We would like to acknowledge Rebecca Zakarian and Angelina Gómez for their important role in collecting this data.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Julia D. McQuade.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

McQuade, J.D., Mendoza, S.A., Larsen, K.L. et al. The Nature of Social Positive Illusory Bias: Reflection of Social Impairment, Self-Protective Motivation, or Poor Executive Functioning?. J Abnorm Child Psychol 45, 289–300 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0172-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0172-6

Keywords

Navigation