Skip to main content
Log in

How Prosodic Sensitivity Contributes to Reading Comprehension: a Meta-Analysis

  • META-ANALYSIS
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This meta-analysis examined the contribution of prosodic sensitivity to reading comprehension, and characterized the pathways by which this contribution occurs and the factors affecting it. Using meta-regression with robust variance estimation to synthesize and quantify 53 papers comprising 62 studies on word reading (77 effect sizes, N = 6,043 participants) and 26 studies on reading comprehension (34 effect sizes, N = 2,736 participants), we found an overall significant correlation between prosodic sensitivity and both word reading (r = .34) and reading comprehension (r = .31). The subgroup moderation analysis indicated that readers’ first language rhythmic class significantly affected the strength of correlation between prosodic sensitivity and both word reading and reading comprehension, while the task-related prosodic level influenced only the strength between prosodic sensitivity and word reading. The two-stage meta-analytic structural equation modeling revealed that prosodic sensitivity indirectly contributed to reading comprehension via metalinguistic skills, vocabulary, and word reading. These findings suggest that 1) despite the existence of a moderate association between prosodic sensitivity and reading comprehension, reader- and task-related factors should be considered when evaluating the strength of this association, and 2) prosodic sensitivity’s contribution to reading comprehension is largely contingent upon its link with other linguistic and cognitive skills.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

Data are available upon request.

References

References marked with an asterisk (*) indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.

  • Bergmann, A., Hall, K. C., & Ross, S. M. (2007). Language files. The Ohio State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, M., Salverda, A. P., Dilley, L. C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2011). Distal prosody influences lexical interpretation in online sentence processing. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 33(33), 1631–1639. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/0d48j8d8

  • *Calet, N., Gutiérrez-Palma, N., Simpson, I., González-Trujillo, M. C., & Defior, S. (2015). Suprasegmental phonology development and reading acquisition: A longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading19(1), 51–71. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2014.976342

  • *Chan, J. S., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2018). Explaining phonology and reading in adult learners: Introducing prosodic awareness and executive functions to reading ability. Journal of Research in Reading, 41(1), 42–57.https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12083

  • *Chan, J., Wade-Woolley, L., Heggie, L., & Kirby, J. (2020). Understanding prosody and morphology in school-age children’s reading. Reading and Writing, 33(5), 1295–1324.https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-10005-4

  • Chen, H. (2013). The roles of phonological knowledge in L2 lower achievers’ reading development. Journal of Asia TEFL, 10(2), 1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, M.W.-L. (2015). metaSEM: An R package for meta-analysis using structural equation modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1521. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01521

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, M. W. L., & Chan, W. (2005). Meta-analytic structural equation modeling: A two-stage approach. Psychological Methods, 10(1), 40–64. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.10.1.40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Chung, W. L., & Bidelman, G. M. (2021). Mandarin-speaking preschoolers’ pitch discrimination, prosodic and phonological awareness, and their relation to receptive vocabulary and reading abilities. Reading and Writing, 34(2), 337–353. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716414000514

  • *Chung, W. L., Jarmulowicz, L., & Bidelman, G. M. (2017). Auditory processing, linguistic prosody awareness, and word reading in Mandarin-speaking children learning English. Reading and Writing, 30(7), 1407–1429. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9730-8

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Set correlation and contingency tables. Applied Psychological Measurement, 12(4), 425–434. https://doi.org/10.1177/014662168801200410

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Taylor & Francis Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203774441

  • Couper-Kuhlen, E. (1986). An introduction to English prosody. Edward Arnold.

  • Dahan, D. (2015). Prosody and language comprehension. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6(5), 441–452. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1355

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dauer, R. M. (1983). Stress-timing and syllable-timing reanalyzed. Journal of Phonetics, 11(1), 51–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(19)30776-4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, S. H., Holliman, A. J., Dobson, G. J., & Harrison, E. C. J. (2018). Assessing direct contributions of morphological awareness and prosodic sensitivity to children’s word reading and reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(6), 527–534. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1483376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Defior, S., Gutiérrez-Palma, N., & Cano-Marín, M. J. (2012). Prosodic awareness skills and literacy acquisition in Spanish. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 41(4), 285–294. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-011-9192-0.

  • *Deng, Q., Choi, W., & Tong, X. (2019). Bidirectional cross-linguistic association of phonological skills and reading comprehension: Evidence from Hong Kong Chinese-English bilingual readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 52(4), 299–311. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219419842914.

  • Deng, Q., & Tong, S. X. (2021). Suprasegmental but not segmental phonological awareness matters in understanding bilingual reading comprehension difficulties in Chinese and English: A 3-year longitudinal study. Annals of Dyslexia, 71, 150–169. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-021-00213-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dilley, L. C., & McAuley, J. D. (2008). Distal prosodic context affects word segmentation and lexical processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(3), 294–311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.06.006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dispaldro, M., Leonard, L. B., & Deevy, P. (2013). Real-word and nonword repetition in Italian-speaking children with specific language impairment: A study of diagnostic accuracy. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 56(1), 323–336. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0304)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dowhower, S. L. (1991). Speaking of prosody: Fluency’s unattended bedfellow. Theory into Practice, 30(3), 165–175. https://doi.org/10.1080/00405849109543497

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315(7109), 629–634. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J.D. (2002). Prosodic disambiguation in silent reading. North East Linguistics Society, 32(1), 8. https://scholarworks.umass.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1478&context=nels

  • Frazier, L., Carlson, K., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2006). Prosodic phrasing is central to language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(6), 244–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.04.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Goetry, V., Wade-Woolley, L., Kolinsky, R., & Mousty, P. (2006). The role of stress processing abilities in the development of bilingual reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 29(3), 349–362. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00313.x

  • *Goodman, I., Libenson, A., & Wade‐Woolley, L. (2010). Sensitivity to linguistic stress, phonological awareness and early reading ability in preschoolers. Journal of Research in Reading, 33(2), 113–127. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2009.01423.x

  • *Gutiérrez-Fresneda, R., Jiménez-Pérez, E., & De Vicente-Yagüe-Jara, M. (2021). Importance of prosodic awareness when learning how to read in primary school students. Culture and Education33(3), 505–528. https://doi.org/10.1080/11356405.2021.1949108

  • *Gutiérrez-Palma, N., & Palma-Reyes, A. (2007). Stress sensitivity and reading performance in Spanish: A study with children. Journal of Research in Reading, 30(2), 157–168. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2007.00339.x

  • *Gutiérrez-Palma, N., Raya-García, M., & Palma-Reyes, A. (2009). Detecting stress patterns is related to children's performance on reading tasks. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(1), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716408090012

  • Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedges, L. V., Tipton, E., & Johnson, M. C. (2010). Robust variance estimation in meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Research Synthesis Methods, 1(1), 39–65. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heffner, C. C., Dilley, L. C., McAuley, J. D., & Pitt, M. A. (2013). When cues combine: How distal and proximal acoustic cues are integrated in word segmentation. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(9), 1275–1302. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.672229

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Heggie, L., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2018). Prosodic awareness and punctuation ability in adult readers. Reading Psychology, 39(2), 188–215. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702711.2017.1413021

  • *Holliman, A., Critten, S., Lawrence, T., Harrison, E., Wood, C., & Hughes, D. (2014a). Modeling the relationship between prosodic sensitivity and early literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 49(4), 469–482. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.82

  • *Holliman, A.J., Gutiérrez Palma, N., Critten, S., Wood, C., Cunnane, H., & Pillinger, C. (2017a). Examining the independent contribution of prosodic sensitivity to word reading and spelling in early readers. Reading and Writing, 30, 509–521. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9687-z

  • *Holliman, A. J., Mundy, I. R., Wade-Woolley, L., Wood, C., & Bird, C. (2017b). Prosodic awareness and children’s multisyllabic word reading. Educational Psychology, 37(10), 1222–1241. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2017.1330948

  • *Holliman, A. J., Williams, G. J., Mundy, I. R., Wood, C., Hart, L., & Waldron, S. (2014b). Beginning to disentangle the prosody-literacy relationship: A multi-component measure of prosodic sensitivity. Reading and Writing, 27(2), 255–266. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9443-6

  • *Holliman, A. J., Wood, C., & Sheehy, K. (2008). Sensitivity to speech rhythm explains individual differences in reading ability independently of phonological awareness. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 357–367. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151007X241623

  • *Holliman, A. J., Wood, C., & Sheehy, K. (2010). Does speech rhythm sensitivity predict children's reading ability 1 year later? Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(2), 356–366. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018049

  • *Hsuan, C. H., Tsai, H. J., & Stainthorp, R. (2018). The role of phonological and orthographic awareness in learning to read among Grade 1 and 2 students in Taiwan. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(1), 117–143. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000194

  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Kent, W. M. (2013). Intonation and reading skills in fourth-grade students [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Wayne State University. https://digitalcommons.wayne.edu/oa_dissertations/773

  • *Kim, Y. S. G., & Petscher, Y. (2016). Prosodic sensitivity and reading: An investigation of pathways of relations using a latent variable approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(5), 630–645. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000078

  • *Kitzen, K. R. (2001). Prosodic sensitivity, morphological ability, and reading ability in young adults with and without childhood histories of reading difficulty. (Publication No. 3005743) [Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University]. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global.

  • *Leong, C. K., Cheng, P. W., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The role of sensitivity to rhymes, phonemes, and tones in reading English and Chinese pseudowords. Reading and Writing, 18(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-004-3357-2

  • Li, X., Yang, Y., & Hagoort, P. (2008). Pitch accent and lexical tone processing in Chinese discourse comprehension: An ERP study. Brain Research, 1222, 192–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.05.031

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Liao, C. H. (2006). The development of phonological awareness, rapid naming, and orthographic processing in children learning to read Chinese [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Alberta.

  • Lim, A. J. M., & Cheung, M. W. L. (2021). Evaluating FIML and multiple imputation in joint ordinal-continuous measurements models with missing data. Behavior Research Methods, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01582-w

  • *Lin, J., & Zhang, H. (2021). Cross-linguistic influence of phonological awareness and phonological recoding skills in Chinese reading acquisition among early adolescent students. The Journal of General Psychology, 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2021.1922345

  • *Lochrin, M., Arciuli, J., & Sharma, M. (2015). Assessing the relationship between prosody and reading outcomes in children using the PEPS-C. Scientific Studies of Reading, 19(1), 72–85. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2014.976341

  • MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., Sugawara, H., & M. (1996). Power Analysis and Determination of Sample Sizefor Covariance Structure Modeling. Psychological Methods, 1(2), 130–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *McBride-Chang, C., Tong, X., Shu, H., Wong, A. M. Y., Leung, K. W., & Tardif, T. (2008). Syllable, phoneme, and tone: Psycholinguistic units in early Chinese and English word recognition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12(2), 171–194. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430801917290

  • McQueen, J. M., & Dilley, L. (2020). Prosody and spoken-word recognition. In C. Gussenhoven & A. Chen (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of language prosody (pp. 509–521). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Melsen, W. G., Bootsma, M. C. J., Rovers, M. M., & Bonten, M. J. M. (2014). The effects of clinical and statistical heterogeneity on the predictive values of results from meta-analyses. Clinical Microbiology and Infection, 20(2), 123–129. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-0691.12494

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2006). Prosody of syntactically complex sentences in the oral reading of young children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(4), 839–843. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.4.839

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2008). A Longitudinal Study of the Development of Reading Prosody as aDimension of Oral Reading Fluency in Early Elementary School Children. Reading Research Quarterly, 43(4), 336–354. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.43.4.2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., & PRISMA Group*, T. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Annals of Internal Medicine151(4), 264–269. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

  • *Mundy, I. R., & Carroll, J. M. (2012). Speech prosody and developmental dyslexia: Reduced phonological awareness in the context of intact phonological representations. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24(5), 560–581. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2012.662341

  • Nagy, W. E., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2014). Morphological knowledge and literacy acquisition. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 3–12. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nespor, M., Shukla, M., & Mehler, J. (2011). Stress‐timed vs. syllable‐timed languages. In M. van Oostendorp, C.J. Ewen, E. Hume, & K. Rice (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to phonology (pp. 1147–1159). John Wiley & Sons Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444335262.wbctp0048

  • *Obergfell, A., Schabmann, A., & Schmidt, B. (2022). The relationship between basic auditory processing, prosodic sensitivity and reading in German adults. Journal of Reading in Research, 45(1), 119–136. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12387

  • Peng, P., Lee, K., Luo, J., Li, S., Joshi, R. M., & Tao, S. (2021). Simple view of reading in Chinese: A one-stage meta-analytic structural equation modeling. Review of Educational Research, 91(1), 3–33. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654320964198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. Precursors of Functional Literacy, 11, 67–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 22–37. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.827687

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, R. A., & Brown, S. P. (2005). On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(1), 175–181. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (2000). Lexical and nonlexical print-to-sound translation of disyllabic words and nonwords. Journal of Memory and Language, 42(3), 342–364. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2687

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Schmidt, B., Breuer-Küppers, P., Valhaus-Aretz, D., Obergfell, A., Schabmann, A. (2023). Prosodic sensitivity and phoneme awareness as predictors of reading fluency in German. Reading and Writing, 36, 223–239. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-022-10313-2

  • Schumacker, R.E., & Lomax, R.G. (2004). A beginner's guide to structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410610904

  • *Shu, H., Peng, H., & McBride‐Chang, C. (2008). Phonological awareness in young Chinese children. Developmental Science, 11(1), 171–181. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00654.x

  • Tanner-Smith, E. E., Tipton, E., & Polanin, J. R. (2016). Handling complex meta-analytic data structures using robust variance estimates: A tutorial in R. Journal of Developmental and Life-Course Criminology, 2(1), 85–112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40865-016-0026-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Tarczynski-Bowles, M. L. (2013). Assessment of a new speech rhythm sensitivity measure and its relation with children's reading skill development [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Coventry University.

  • Thompson, B., Diamond, K. E., McWilliam, R., Snyder, P., & Snyder, S. W. (2005). Evaluating the quality of evidence from correlational research for evidence-based practice. Exceptional Children, 71(2), 181–194. https://doi.org/10.1177/001440290507100204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Thompson, D. M. (2011). Contributions of prosodic sensitivity and morphological awareness to word level reading: A perceptual task development study. [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Middle Tennessee State University. http://jewlscholar.mtsu.edu/handle/mtsu/4125

  • *Thongseiratch, T., Kraiwong, T., & Roengpitya, R. (2021). The effects of lexical tone awareness on early word recognition, word reading, and spelling from dictation of Thai children: A longitudinal study. Frontiers in Psychology. 12(719552), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.719552

  • *Tong, X., Deng, Q., & Tong, S. X. (2023). Speech prosody and reading comprehension in Chinese–English bilingual children: The mediating role of syntactic awareness. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1044/2023_JSLHR-22-00026

  • *Tong, X., Tong, X., & King Yiu, F. (2018). Beyond auditory sensory processing deficits: Lexical tone perception deficits in Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51(3), 293–301. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219417712018

  • *Tong, X., Tong, X., & McBride-Chang, C. (2015a). A tale of two writing systems: Double dissociation and metalinguistic transfer between Chinese and English word reading among Hong Kong children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(2), 130–145. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413492854

  • *Tong, X., Tong, X., & McBride-Chang, C. (2015b). Tune in to the tone: Lexical tone identification is associated with vocabulary and word recognition abilities in young Chinese children. Language and Speech, 58(4), 441–458. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830914562988

  • van Aert, R. C. M., Wicherts, J. M., & van Assen, M. A. L. M. (2016). Conducting meta-analyses based on p values: Reservations and recommendations for applying p-uniform and p-curve. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(5), 713–729. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616650874

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viechtbauer, W., & Cheung, M.W.-L. (2010). Outlier and influence diagnostics for meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 1(2), 112–125. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wade-Woolley, L., Wood, C., Chan, J., & Weidman, S. (2021). Prosodic competence as the missing component of reading processes across languages: Theory, evidence and future research. Scientific Studies of Reading, 26(2), 165–181. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2021.1995390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Wade-Woolley, L., & Heggie, L. (2015). Implicit knowledge of word stress and derivational morphology guides skilled readers’ decoding of multisyllabic words. Scientific Studies of Reading, 19(1), 21–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2014.947647

  • Walker, D. A. (2003). JMASM9: Converting Kendall’s tau for correlational or meta-analytic analyses. Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods, 2(2), 525–530. https://doi.org/10.22237/jmasm/1067646360

  • *Wang, H. L. S., Huss, M., Hämäläinen, J. A., & Goswami, U. (2012). Basic auditory processing and developmental dyslexia in Chinese. Reading and Writing, 25(2), 509–536. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9284-5

  • Wang, L.-C., Liu, D., Kwan-Chen, L.L.-Y., Chung, K.K.-H., & Chen, J.-K. (2022). Lower prosodic sensitivity in Chinese children with dyslexia and its impact on Chinese reading. Dyslexia, 28(3), 342–358. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1720

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Wang, M., Yang, C., & Cheng, C. (2009). The contributions of phonology, orthography, and morphology in Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(2), 291–314. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716409090122

  • *Weidman, S. (2021) Exploring the role of prosody in passage reading of experienced and early readers. [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Nottingham Trent University.

  • *Whalley, K., & Hansen, J. (2006). The role of prosodic sensitivity in children's reading development. Journal of Research in Reading, 29(3), 288–303. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00309.x

  • *Williams, G. J., & Wood, C. (2012). Sensitivity to the acoustic correlates of lexical stress and their relationship to reading in skilled readers. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8(4), 267–280. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10053-008-0122-0

  • Wolters, A. P., Kim, Y. S. G., & Szura, J. W. (2020). Is reading prosody related to reading comprehension? A meta-analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 26(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2020.1850733

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Wood, C. (2006). Metrical stress sensitivity in young children and its relationship to phonological awareness and reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 29(3), 270–287. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00308.x

  • Wood, C., & Terrell, C. (1998). Poor readers’ ability to detect speech rhythm and perceive rapid speech. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(3), 397–413. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00760.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Yin, L., Li, W., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Zhang, J., Shu, H., & Jiang, W. (2011). The role of tone awareness and pinyin knowledge in Chinese reading. Writing Systems Research, 3(1), 59–68. https://doi.org/10.1093/wsr/wsr010

  • *Yuan, H., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2022). The role of phonological awareness, pinyin letter knowledge, and visual perception skills in kindergarteners' Chinese character reading. Behavioral Sciences (Basel, Switzerland)12(8), 254. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080254

  • *Zhang, H., Lin, J., Cheng, X., Wang, C., Wei, X. (2022). Concurrent and longitudinal contributions of phonological awareness to early adolescent Chinese reading acquisition. The Journal of General Psychology, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2022.2047003

  • *Zhang, J., & McBride-Chang, C. (2014). Auditory sensitivity, speech perception, L1 Chinese, and L2 English reading abilities in Hong Kong Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 1001. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035086

  • *Zhang, J., Meng, Y., Wu, C., & Zhou, D. Q. (2017). Writing system modulates the association between sensitivity to acoustic cues in music and reading ability: Evidence from Chinese–English bilingual children. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1965. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01965

  • *Zhou, L. (2017). Learning to read and spell in Chinese: The role of cognitive skills. [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of College London. https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/id/eprint/1545271

Download references

Funding

The work was partially supported by the Research Fellow Scheme (RFS2021-7H05) and General Research Fund (17609518, 17620520) from the Research Grants Council of the Hong Kong Special Administrative Region, China to Shelley Xiuli Tong. Qinli Deng is supported by the Postdoctoral Fellowship from Hong Kong Research Grants Council (PDFS2122-7H05).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shelley Xiuli Tong.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 78.1 KB)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tong, S.X., Lentejas, K., Deng, Q. et al. How Prosodic Sensitivity Contributes to Reading Comprehension: a Meta-Analysis. Educ Psychol Rev 35, 78 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-023-09792-8

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-023-09792-8

Keywords

Navigation