Skip to main content
Log in

The Role of Effort in Understanding Educational Achievement: Objective Effort as an Explanatory Construct Versus Effort as a Student Perception

  • Review Article
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Almost anything worth doing takes effort, so it is no surprise that effort has played such a central role in how researchers, theoreticians, instructors, and even students think about student learning and achievement. In this special issue, the authors of the target articles explore the importance of effort to students’ self-regulated learning within multiple domains. To further support research progress on effort, we distinguish between objective effort as a direct causal agent of learning gains and effort as a student perception. We argue that understanding effort as a student perception shows promise for discovering ways to improve self-regulated learning and student achievement. In developing these arguments, we consider the contribution of the target articles to five themes relevant to metacognitively driven self-regulated learning, with the aim of fostering progressive research programs aimed at revealing the potential roles of effort in student achievement.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Lay use of the term effort (“conscious exertion of power: hard work; example, a job requiring time and effort,” Merriam-Webster, n.d.) would imply the same—that is, more effort is indicated by working harder and not simply by working longer. Even so, students may say that they are using more effort when they use more time, and we return to the role of time in students’ perceptions of effort in the next section.

  2. This claim is apparent from Bjork and Bjork (2011) who define desirable difficulties as “better conditions of learning that, while apparently creating difficulty, actually lead to more durable and flexible learning” (p. 58), making it clear that it is the conditions that are desirable—not the difficulties per se.

References

Note: The target articles are not currently in references, and to complete the commentary, we will need page numbers for all quotations.

  • Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2011). Making things hard on yourself, but in a good way: creating desirable difficulties to enhance learning. In M. A. Gernsbacher, R. W. Pew, L. M. Hough, & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Psychology and the real world: essays illustrating fundamental contributions to society (pp. 55–64). New York, NY: Worth Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe & A. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: knowing about knowing (pp. 185–205). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: an integrated framework. In Oxford Psychology series, No. 26. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Bruin, A., Dunlosky, J., & Cavalcanti, R. (2017). Monitoring and regulation of learning in medical education: the need for predictive cues. Medical Education, 51(6), 575–584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeShon, R. P., Brown, K. G., & Greenis, J. L. (1996). Does self-regulation require cognitive resources? Evaluation of resource allocation models of goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81(5), 595–608. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.81.5.595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Ariel, R. (2011). Self-regulated learning and the allocation of study time. In B. Ross (Ed.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 54, pp. 103–140).

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Mueller, M. (2016). Recommendations for exploring the disfluency hypothesis toward establishing whether perceptually degrading materials impacts performance. Metacognition and Learning, 11(1), 123–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Tauber, S. K. (2014). Understanding people’s metacognitive judgments: an isomechanism framework and its implications for applied and theoretical research. In T. Perfect & D. S. Lindsay (Eds.), Handbook of applied memory (pp. 444–464). Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87(3), 215–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janes, J., Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2018). How do students use self-testing across multiple study sessions when preparing for a high-stakes exam? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 7(2), 230–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, N.J: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Renkl, A., & Paas, F. (2010). Facilitating flexible problem solving: a cognitive load perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 22(2), 175–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirk-Johnson, A., Galla, B. M., & Fraundorf, S. H. (2019). Perceiving effort as poor learning: the misinterpreted-effort hypothesis of how experienced effort and perceived learning relate to study strategy choice. Cognitive Psychology, 115, 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhbandner, C., & Emmerdinger, K. J. (2019). Do students really prefer repeated rereading over testing when studying textbooks? A reexamination. Memory, 27(7), 952–961. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2019.1610177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1993). How do we know that we know? The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing. Psychological Review, 100(4), 609–639. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: a cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(4), 349–370. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.126.4.349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (2008). Easy comes, easy goes? The link between learning and remembering and its exploitation in metacognition. In Memory & Cognition (pp. 416–428).

    Google Scholar 

  • McCann, & Johnston. (1992). Locus of the single-channel bottleneck in secondary-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 471–484.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M., & Einstein, G. O. (2020). Training learning strategies to promote self-regulation and transfer: the knowledge, belief, commitment, and planning framework. In Perspectives in Psychological Sciences (p. 174569162092072). https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620920723.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Merriam-Webster. (n.d.). Effort. In Merriam-Webster.com dictionary Retrieved September 1, 2020, from https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/effort.

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1976). On the analysis of performance operating characteristics. Psychological Review, 83(6), 508–510. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.83.6.508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(3), 358–377. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.10.3.358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K. A. (2004). Exploring automaticity in text processing: syntactic ambiguity as a test case. Cognitive Psychology, 49(4), 333–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rivers, M. (2020). Metacognition of practice testing: a review of learners’ beliefs, monitoring, and control of test-enhanced learning. Resubmission under review.

  • Ryan, Petty, & Wenzlaff. (1982). Motivated remembering efforts during tip-of-the-tongue states. Acta Psychologica, 51(2), 137–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12(2), 257–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22(2), 123–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Dunlosky.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dunlosky, J., Badali, S., Rivers, M.L. et al. The Role of Effort in Understanding Educational Achievement: Objective Effort as an Explanatory Construct Versus Effort as a Student Perception. Educ Psychol Rev 32, 1163–1175 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09577-3

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09577-3

Keywords

Navigation