Skip to main content
Log in

A Meta-Analytic Review of the Benefit of Spacing out Retrieval Practice Episodes on Retention

  • Meta-Analysis
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

This article has been updated

Abstract

Spaced retrieval practice consists of repetitions of the same retrieval event distributed through time. This learning strategy combines two “desirable difficulties”: retrieval practice and spacing effects. We carried out meta-analyses on 29 studies investigating the benefit of spacing out retrieval practice episodes on final retention. The total dataset was divided into two subsets to investigate two main questions: (1) Does spaced retrieval practice induce better memory retention than massed retrieval practice? (subset 1); (2) Is the expanding spacing schedule superior to the uniform spacing schedule when learning with retrieval practice? (subset 2). Using meta-regression with robust variance estimation, 39 effect sizes were aggregated in subset 1 and 54 in subset 2. Results from subset 1 indicated a strong benefit of spaced retrieval practice in comparison with massed retrieval practice (g = 0.74). Results from subset 2 indicated no significant difference between expanding and uniform spacing schedules of retrieval practice (g = 0.034). Moderator analyses on this subset showed that the number of exposures of an item during retrieval practice explains inconsistencies between studies: the more learners are tested, the more beneficial the expanding schedule is compared with the uniform one. Overall, these results support the advantage of spacing out the retrieval practice episodes on the same content, but do not support the widely held belief that inter-retrieval intervals should be progressively increased until a retention test.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Change history

  • 29 June 2021

    Springer Nature’s version of this paper was updated to correct the word "retrieval" in the Abstract section that was mistakenly spelled as "retrieBjorkval".

Notes

  1. https://automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer/.

References

References Marked with an Asterisk Indicate Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis

  • Adesope, O. O., Trevisan, D. A., & Sundararajan, N. (2017). Rethinking the use of tests: a meta-analysis of practice testing. Review of Educational Research, 0034654316689306, 87(3), 659–701. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654316689306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, H. P. (1979). Maintenance of knowledge: questions about memory we forgot to ask. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108(3), 296–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Balota, D. A., Duchek, J. M., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2006). Does expanded retrieval produce benefits over equal-interval spacing? Explorations of spacing effects in healthy aging and early stage Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.19

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D.A., Duchek, J. M., & Logan, J. M. (2007). Is expanded retrieval practice a superior form of spaced retrieval? A critical review of the extant literature. In The foundations of remembering: essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger, III (p. 83-105). Psychology Press.

  • Bangert-Drowns, R. L., Kulik, C.-L. C., Kulik, J. A., & Morgan, M. (1991). The instructional effect of feedback in test-like events. Review of Educational Research, 61(2), 213–238. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543061002213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, A. S., & Tullis, J. (2010). What makes distributed practice effective? Cognitive Psychology, 61(3), 228–247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.05.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2011). Making things hard on yourself, but in a good way: Creating desirable difficulties to enhance learning. In M. A. Gernsbacher, R. W. Pew, L. M. Hough, & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Psychology and the real world: Essays illustrating fundamental contributions to society (pp. 56–64). New York: Worth Publishers.

  • Bjork, R. A., & Allen, T. W. (1970). The spacing effect: consolidation or differential encoding? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9(5), 567–572. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(70)80103-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470743386.

  • Braun, K., & Rubin, D. C. (1998). The spacing effect depends on an encoding deficit, retrieval, and time in working memory: evidence from once-presented words. Memory (Hove, England), 6(1), 37–65. https://doi.org/10.1080/741941599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P. C., Roediger, H. L., & McDaniel, M. A. (2014). Make it stick. Harvard University Press.

  • * Caple, C. (1996). The effects of spaced practice and spaced review on recall and retention using computer assisted instruction. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • * Carpenter, S. K., & DeLosh, E. L. (2005). Application of the testing and spacing effects to name learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19(5), 619–636. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. K., Cepeda, N. J., Rohrer, D., Kang, S. H. K., & Pashler, H. (2012). Using spacing to enhance diverse forms of learning: review of recent research and implications for instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 24(3), 369–378. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9205-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cecilio-Fernandes, D., Cnossen, F., Jaarsma, D. A. D. C., & Tio, R. A. (2018). Avoiding surgical skill decay: a systematic review on the spacing of training sessions. Journal of Surgical Education, 75(2), 471–480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsurg.2017.08.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cepeda, N. J., Coburn, N., Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. T., Mozer, M. C., & Pashler, H. (2009). Optimizing distributed practice: theoretical analysis and practical implications. Experimental Psychology, 56(4), 236–246. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.56.4.236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: a review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(3), 354–380. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, J. C. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2006). Retrieval-induced facilitation: initially nontested material can benefit from prior testing of related material. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 135(4), 553–571. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cull, W. L. (2000). Untangling the benefits of multiple study opportunities and repeated testing for cued recall. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14(3), 215–235. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(200005/06)14:3<215::AID-ACP640>3.0.CO;2-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cull, W. L., Shaughnessy, J. J., & Zechmeister, E. B. (1996). Expanding understanding of the expanding-pattern-of-retrieval mnemonic: toward confidence in applicability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2(4), 365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dail, T. K., & Christina, R. W. (2004). Distribution of practice and metacognition in learning and long-term retention of a discrete motor task. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 75(2), 148–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delaney, P. F., Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L., & Spirgel, A. (2010). Spacing and testing effects: a deeply critical, lengthy, and at times discursive review of the literature. In Ross, B. H. (Éd.), Psychology of learning and motivation: advances in research and theory, vol 53 (pp. 63–147). Elsevier Academic Press Inc.

  • * Dobson, J. L. (2012). Effect of uniform versus expanding retrieval practice on the recall of physiology information. Advances in Physiology Education, 36(1), 6–12. https://doi.org/10.1152/advan.00090.2011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Dobson, J. L. (2013). Retrieval practice is an efficient method of enhancing the retention of anatomy and physiology information. Advances in Physiology Education, 37(2), 184–191. https://doi.org/10.1152/advan.00174.2012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Dobson, J. L., Perez, J., & Linderholm, T. (2016). Distributed retrieval practice promotes superior recall of anatomy information. Anatomical Sciences Education, n/a-n/a., 10(4), 339–347. https://doi.org/10.1002/ase.1668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donovan, J. J., & Radosevich, D. J. (1999). A meta-analytic review of the distribution of practice effect: now you see it, now you don’t. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(5), 795–805. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.84.5.795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest: A Journal of the American Psychological Society, 14(1), 4–58. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100612453266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ [British Medical Journal], 315(7109), 629–634.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, Z., & Tipton, E. (2015). Robumeta: an R-package for robust variance estimation in meta-analysis. arXiv:1503.02220 [stat]. http://arxiv.org/abs/1503.02220. Accessed 26 July 2018.

  • * Fishman, E. J., Keller, L., & Atkinson, R. C. (1968). Massed versus distributed practice in computerized spelling drills. Jounral of Educational Psychology, 59(4), 290–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., Nolan, D., & Singleton, J. (2007). Expanding retrieval practice: an effective aid to preschool children’s learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(7), 991–1004. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210600823595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerbier, E., & Koenig, O. (2012). Influence of multiple-day temporal distribution of repetitions on memory: a comparison of uniform, expanding, and contracting schedules. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(3), 514–525. Scopus. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.600806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerbier, E.. (2011). Effet du type d’agencement temporel des répétitions d’une information Sur la récupération explicite. Lyon 2. http://www.theses.fr/2011LYO20029. Accessed 30 Sept 2016.

  • Gerbier, E., Toppino, T. C., & Koenig, O. (2015). Optimising retention through multiple study opportunities over days: the benefit of an expanding schedule of repetitions. Memory, 23(6), 943–954. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.944916.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M. (1979). Component-levels theory of the effects of spacing of repetitions on recall and recognition. Memory & Cognition, 7(2), 95–112. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gluckman, M., Vlach, H. A., & Sandhofer, C. M. (2014). Spacing simultaneously promotes multiple forms of learning in children’s science curriculum. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(2), 266–273. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2997.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godbole, N. R., Delaney, P. F., & Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L. (2014). The spacing effect in immediate and delayed free recall. Memory, 22(5), 462–469. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.798416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greving, S., & Richter, T. (2018). Examining the testing effect in university teaching: retrievability and question format matter. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02412.

  • * Grote, M. G. (1995). Distributed versus massed practice in high school physics. School Science and Mathematics, 95(2), 97–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrer, M., Cuijpers, P., Furukawa, T. A., & Ebert, D. D. (2019). Doing meta-analysis in R: a hands-on guide: vol. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.2551803https://bookdown.org/MathiasHarrer/Doing_Meta_Analysis_in_R/. Accessed 15 April 2020.

  • Hattie, J. (2008). Visible learning: a synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses relating to achievement. Routledge.

  • Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Academic Press.

  • Hedges, L. V., Tipton, E., & Johnson, M. C. (2010). Robust variance estimation in meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Research Synthesis Methods, 1(1), 39–65. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hempel, S., Miles, J. N., Booth, M. J., Wang, Z., Morton, S. C., & Shekelle, P. G. (2013). Risk of bias: a simulation study of power to detect study-level moderator effects in meta-analysis. Systematic Reviews, 2(1), 107. https://doi.org/10.1186/2046-4053-2-107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, J. P. T., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ [British Medical Journal], 327(7414), 557–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • * Hopkins, R. F., Lyle, K. B., Hieb, J. L., & Ralston, P. A. S. (2016). Spaced retrieval practice increases college students’ short- and long-term retention of mathematics knowledge. Educational Psychology Review, 28(4), 853–873. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9349-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horvath, J. C., Lodge, J. M., & Hattie, J. (2016). From the laboratory to the classroom: translating science of learning for teachers. Routledge.

  • Janiszewski, C., Noel, H., & Sawyer, A. G. (2003). A meta-analysis of the spacing effect in verbal learning: implications for research on advertising repetition and consumer memory. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(1), 138–149. https://doi.org/10.1086/374692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Kalenberg, K. (2017). Spaced and expanded practice: an investigation of methods to enhance retention. Journal of Undergraduate Research at Minnesota State University, Mankato, 17, 18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kang, S. H. K. (2016). Spaced repetition promotes efficient and effective learning. Policy Insights From the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2372732215624708, 3(1), –19. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732215624708.

  • * Kang, S. H. K., Lindsey, R. V., Mozer, M. C., & Pashler, H. (2014). Retrieval practice over the long term: should spacing be expanding or equal-interval? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(6), 1544–1550. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0636-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapler, I. V., Weston, T., & Wiseheart, M. (2015). Spacing in a simulated undergraduate classroom: long-term benefits for factual and higher-level learning. Learning and Instruction, 36, 38–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpicke, J. D., & Aue, W. R. (2015). The testing effect is alive and well with complex materials. Educational Psychology Review, 27(2), 317–326. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9309-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Karpicke, J. D., & Bauernschmidt, A. (2011). Spaced retrieval: absolute spacing enhances learning regardless of relative spacing. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 37(5), 1250–1257. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpicke, J. D., Blunt, J. R., & Smith, M. A. (2016). Retrieval-based learning: positive effects of retrieval practice in elementary school children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 350. https://doi.org/10.3380/fpsyg.2015.00350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpicke, J. D., & Grimaldi, P. J. (2012). Retrieval-based learning: a perspective for enhancing meaningful learning. Educational Psychology Review, 24(3), 401–418. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9202-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2007). Expanding retrieval practice promotes short-term retention, but equally spaced retrieval enhances long-term retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 33(4), 704–719. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.4.704.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2010). Is expanding retrieval a superior method for learning text materials? Memory & Cognition, 38(1), 116–124. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.1.116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klingbeil, D. A., Renshaw, T. L., Willenbrink, J. B., Copek, R. A., Chan, K. T., Haddock, A., Yassine, J., & Clifton, J. (2017). Mindfulness-based interventions with youth: a comprehensive meta-analysis of group-design studies. Journal of School Psychology, 63, 77–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2017.03.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Küpper-Tetzel, C. E., Kapler, I. V., & Wiseheart, M. (2014). Contracting, equal, and expanding learning schedules: the optimal distribution of learning sessions depends on retention interval. Memory and Cognition, 42(5), 729–741. Scopus. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0394-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, D. P. (2018). Planning education for long-term retention: the cognitive science and implementation of retrieval practice. Seminars in Neurology, 38(4), 449–456. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0038-1666983.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, T. D., & Genovese, E. D. (1989). Distribution of practice in motor skill acquisition: different effects for discrete and continuous tasks. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 60(1), 59–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsey, R., Mozer, M., Cepeda, N. J., & Pashler, H. (2009). Optimizing memory retention with cognitive models. International conference on computing and Mission, 6.

  • Lindsey, R. V., Shroyer, J. D., Pashler, H., & Mozer, M. C. (2014). Improving students’ long-term knowledge retention through personalized review. Psychological Science, 25(3), 639–647. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613504302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Logan, J. M., & Balota, D. A. (2008). Expanded vs. equal interval spaced retrieval practice: exploring different schedules of spacing and retention interval in younger and older adults. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 15(3), 257–280. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825580701322171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Maddox, G. B., Balota, D. A., Coane, J. H., & Duchek, J. M. (2011). The role of forgetting rate in producing a benefit of expanded over equal spaced retrieval in young and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 26(3), 661–670. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Agarwal, P. K., Huelser, B. J., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2011). Test-enhanced learning in a middle school science classroom: the effects of quiz frequency and placement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(2), 399–414. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Fadler, C. L., & Pashler, H. (2013a). Effects of spaced versus massed training in function learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(5), 1417–1432. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Thomas, R. C., Agarwal, P. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2013b). Quizzing in middle-school science: successful transfer performance on classroom exams. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27(3), 360–372. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * McGregor, K. K. (2014). What a difference a day makes: change in memory for newly learned word forms over 24 hours. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(5), 1842–1850. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Mettler, E., Massey, C. M., & Kellman, P. J. (2016). A comparison of adaptive and fixed schedules of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 145(7), 897–917. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine, 6(7), e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moreira, B., Pinto, T., Starling, D., & Jaeger, A. (2019). Retrieval practice in classroom settings: a review of applied research. Frontiers in Education, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2019.00005.

  • Moss, V. D. (1996). The efficacy of massed versus distributed practice as a function of desired learning outcomes and grade level of the student. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 56(9-B), 5204.

  • Mozer, M. C., Pashler, H., Cepeda, N., Lindsey, R. V., & Vul, E. (2009). Predicting the optimal spacing of study: a multiscale context model of memory. NIPS.

  • Mumford, M. D., Costanza, D. P., Baughman, W. A., Threlfall, K. V., & Fleishman, E. A. (1994). Influence of abilities on performance during practice: effects of massed and distributed practice. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 134–144. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.86.1.134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Nakata, T. (2015). Effects of expanding and equal spacing on second language vocabulary learning. does gradually increasing spacing increase vocabulary learning? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(4), 677–711. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263114000825.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, S. C., & Rickard, T. C. (2018). Transfer of test-enhanced learning: meta-analytic review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 144(7), 710–756. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., Bain, P. M., Bottge, B. A., Graesser, A., Koedinger, K., McDaniel, M., & Metcalfe, J. (2007). Organizing Instruction and Study to Improve Student Learning. IES Practice Guide. NCER 2007–2004. National Center for Education Research. http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED498555. Accessed 28 Aug 2016.

  • Pavlik, P. I., & Anderson, J. R. (2005). Practice and forgetting effects on vocabulary memory: an activation-based model of the spacing effect. Cognitive Science, 29(4), 559–586. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlik, P. I., & Anderson, J. R. (2008). Using a model to compute the optimal schedule of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 14(2), 101–117. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.14.2.101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Pyc, M. A., & Rawson, K. A. (2007). Examining the efficiency of schedules of distributed retrieval practice. Memory and Cognition, 35(8), 1917–1927 Scopus.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2003). Spacing and repetition effects in human memory: application of the SAM model. Cognitive Science, 27(3), 431–452. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2703_5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., & Butler, A. C. (2011). The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(1), 20–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17(3), 249–255. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01693.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger III, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2011). Intricacies of spaced retrieval: a resolution. In Successful remembering and successful forgetting: a festschrift in honor of Robert A. Bjork (pp. 23–47). Psychology Press.

  • Rohrer, D., & Taylor, K. (2006). The effects of overlearning and distributed practise on the retention of mathematics knowledge. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(9), 1209–1224. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenshine, B. (2010). Principles of instruction. Educational practices series-21. UNESCO International Bureau of Education.

  • Rowland, C. A. (2014). The effect of testing versus restudy on retention: a meta-analytic review of the testing effect. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 1432–1463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seabrook, R., Brown, G. D. A., & Solity, J. E. (2005). Distributed and massed practice: from laboratory to classroom. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19(1), 107–122. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sense, F., Behrens, F., Meijer, R. R., & van Rijn, H. (2016). An individual’s rate of forgetting is stable over time but differs across materials. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(1), 305–321. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smolen, P., Zhang, Y., & Byrne, J. H. (2016). The right time to learn: mechanisms and optimization of spaced learning. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 17(2), 77–88. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2015.18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, H. S., Cepeda, N. J., & Kapler, I. V. (2011). Spacing effects in real-world classroom vocabulary learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 763–767. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • * Storm, B. C., Bjork, R. A., & Storm, J. C. (2010). Optimizing retrieval as a learning event: when and why expanding retrieval practice enhances long-term retention. Memory & Cognition, 38(2), 244–253. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.2.244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabibian, B., Upadhyay, U., De, A., Zarezade, A., Schölkopf, B., & Gomez-Rodriguez, M. (2019). Enhancing human learning via spaced repetition optimization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(10), 3988–3993. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1815156116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanner-Smith, E. E., & Tipton, E. (2014). Robust variance estimation with dependent effect sizes: practical considerations including a software tutorial in Stata and SPSS. Research Synthesis Methods, 5(1), 13–30. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1091.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terenyi, J., Anksorus, H., & Persky, A. M. (2018). Impact of spacing of practice on learning brand name and generic drugs. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 82(1), 6179. https://doi.org/10.5688/ajpe6179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tipton, E., & Pustejovsky, J. E. (2015). Small-sample adjustments for tests of moderators and model fit using robust variance estimation in meta-regression. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 40(6), 604–634. https://doi.org/10.3102/1076998615606099.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toppino, T. C., & Gerbier, E. (2014). About practice: repetition, spacing, and abstraction. In Ross, B. H. (Éd.), Psychology of learning and motivation, vol 60 (pp. 113–189). Elsevier Academic Press Inc.

  • Toppino, T. C., Phelan, H.-A., & Gerbier, E. (2018). Level of initial training moderates the effects of distributing practice over multiple days with expanding, contracting, and uniform schedules: evidence for study-phase retrieval. Memory & Cognition, 46(6), 969–978. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0815-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, L. (1927). The relation of retention to the distribution of relearning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10(1), 30–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uchihara, T., Webb, S., & Yanagisawa, A. (2019). The effects of repetition on incidental vocabulary learning: a meta-analysis of correlational studies. Language Learning, 69(3), 559–599. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valentine, J. C., Pigott, T. D., & Rothstein, H. R. (2010). How many studies do you need?: A primer on statistical power for meta-analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 35(2), 215–247. https://doi.org/10.3102/1076998609346961.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vlach, H. A., & Sandhofer, C. M. (2012). Distributing learning over time: the spacing effect in children’s acquisition and generalization of science concepts. Child Development, 83(4), 1137–1144. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01781.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, Y., Madan, C. R., & Sumeracki, M. A. (2018). Teaching the science of learning. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 3(1), 2. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-017-0087-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitten, W. B., & Bjork, R. A. (1977). Learning from Tests : Effects of Spacing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16(4).

  • Wiseheart, M., Kim, A. S. N., Kapler, I. V., Foot-Seymour, V., & Küpper-Tetzel, C. E. (2019). Enhancing the quality of student learning using distributed practice. In J. Dunlosky & K. A. Rawson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of cognition and education (1re ed., pp. 550–584). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108235631.023.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Availability of Data and Material

The data from the included studies are available on OSF with the following link: https://osf.io/jbmq4/?view_only=c05f2fcb93bc4d77b68fb11b1561d220.

Funding

We acknowledge funding from Programme d’investissements d’avenir (Efran programme), Agence National de la Recherche (ANR-17-EURE-0017, ANR-10-IDEX-0001-02 PSL).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

A.L.: conceptualization, methodology, data curation, investigation, formal analysis, writing—original draft preparation. H.P.: methodology, formal analysis, writing—reviewing-editing. F.R.: conceptualization, methodology, supervision, writing—reviewing-editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alice Latimier.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Code Availability

R script for analyses with robumeta package is available on OSF with the following link: https://osf.io/jbmq4/?view_only=c05f2fcb93bc4d77b68fb11b1561d220.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix 1

Appendix 1

Sensitivity analysis for subset 1 and subset 2. This consists in varying the assumed within-study effect size correlation (ρ) and observing the impact on the mean effect size (Hedges’ g) and on the estimated between-study variance (Tau2)

Subset 1

ρ = 0

ρ = 0.2

ρ = 0.4

ρ = 0.6

ρ = 0.8

ρ = 1

Mean effect size

1.009

1.009

1.010

1.010

1.010

1.011

Standard error

0.148

0.148

0.148

0.149

0.149

0.149

Tau2

0.204

0.206

0.208

0.211

0.213

0.215

Subset 2

ρ = 0

ρ = 0.2

ρ = 0.4

ρ = 0.6

ρ = 0.8

ρ = 1

Mean effect size

0.0343

0.0343

0.0343

0.0343

0.0343

0.0343

Standard error

0.0626

0.0626

0.0626

0.0626

0.0626

0.0626

Tau2

0.0000

0.0000

0.0000

0.0000

0.0000

0.0000

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Latimier, A., Peyre, H. & Ramus, F. A Meta-Analytic Review of the Benefit of Spacing out Retrieval Practice Episodes on Retention. Educ Psychol Rev 33, 959–987 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09572-8

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09572-8

Keywords

Navigation