Skip to main content
Log in

Four Ways of Considering Emotion in Cognitive Load Theory

  • REVIEW ARTICLE
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We discuss four ways in which emotion may relate to cognitive load during learning. One perspective describes emotions as extraneous cognitive load, competing for the limited resources of working memory by requiring the processing of task-extra or task-irrelevant information. Another perspective shows that encoding, storage, and retrieval of information are affected by emotion even before awareness of the material, and that emotion may directly affect memory by broadening or narrowing cognitive resources, and by mechanisms such as mood-dependent and mood-congruent processing. A third perspective describes how emotion may affect intrinsic cognitive load, such as when emotion regulation is part of the learning outcomes. We also discuss a dual-channel assumption for emotions. A final perspective is that emotion affects motivation, and, in turn, mental effort investment. These four ways of considering emotion as part of CLT are best understood when taking an interval view of cognitive load.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Panitz, D., De Rosa, E., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2003). Neural correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial signals. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(13), 5627–5633.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2012). How does emotion influence working memory? In S. Masmoudi, D. Y. Dai, & A. Naceur (Eds.), Attention, representation, and human performance: integration of cognition, emotion, and motivation (pp. 3–18). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., Kulp, C. A., Holt, L. E., & Carr, T. H. (2004). More on the fragility of performance: choking under pressure in mathematical problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(4), 584–600.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., & Fiedler, K. (2006). Mood and regulation of information processing and behavior. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Affect in social thinking and behavior (pp. 65–84). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., Clore, G., Schwarz, N., Golisano, V., Rabe, C., & Wolk, M. (1996). Mood and the use of scripts: does a happy mood really lead to mindlessness? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(4), 665–679.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., Petry, M. C., & Lang, P. J. (1992). Remembering pictures: pleasure and arousal in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(2), 379–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brand, S., Reimer, T., & Opwis, K. (2007). How do we learn in a negative mood? Effects of a negative mood on transfer and learning. Learning and Instruction, 17(1), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, M. J. (1998). The impact of computer anxiety and self-efficacy upon performance. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 14(3), 223–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brünken, R., Plass, J. L., & Moreno, R. (2010). Current issues and open questions in cognitive load research. In J. Plass, R. Moreno, & R. Brünken (Eds.), Cognitive load theory (pp. 253–272). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. (2003). Pleasure as a sign you can attend to something else: placing positive feelings within a general model of affect. Cognition and Emotion, 17(2), 241–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarkson, J. J., Hirt, E. R., Austin Chapman, D., & Jia, L. (2011). The impact of illusory fatigue on executive control: do perceptions of depletion impair working memory capacity? Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(3), 231–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 87–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunha, C., Brambilla, R., & Thomas, K. L. (2010). A simple role for BDNF in learning and memory? Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience, 3, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curci, A., Lanciano, T., Soleti, E., & Rimé, B. (2013). Negative emotional experiences arouse rumination and affect working memory capacity. Emotion, 13(5), 867–880.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Mello, S., & Graesser, A. (2012). Dynamics of affective states during complex learning. Learning and Instruction, 22(2), 145–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Mello, S., Lehman, B., Pekrun, R., & Graesser, A. (2014). Confusion can be beneficial for learning. Learning and Instruction, 29, 153–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalgleish, T. (2004). The emotional brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(7), 583–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error and the future of human life. Scientific American, 271(4), 144–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 45–60). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H. C., & Ashbrook, P. W. (1988). Resource allocation model of the effects of depressed mood states. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition and social behavior (pp. 25–43). Toronto: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erez, A., & Isen, A. M. (2002). The influence of positive affect on the components of expectancy motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(6), 1055–1067.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102(2), 211–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Staszewski, J. J. (1989). Skilled memory and expertise: mechanisms of exceptional performance. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing: the impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 235–267). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erk, S., Martin, S., & Walter, H. (2005). Emotional context during encoding of neutral items modulates brain activation not only during encoding but also during recognition. Neuroimage, 26(3), 829–838.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: the processing efficiency theory. Cognition & Emotion, 6(6), 409–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraser, K., Ma, I., Teteris, E., Baxter, H., Wright, B., & McLaughlin, K. (2012). Emotion, cognitive load and learning outcomes during simulation training. Medical Education, 46(11), 1055–1062.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraser, K., Huffman, J., Ma, I., Sobczak, M., McIlwrick, J., Wright, B., & McLaughlin, K. (2014). The emotional and cognitive impact of unexpected simulated patient death: a randomized controlled trial. Chest, 145(5), 958–963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraser, K. L., Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (2015). Cognitive load theory for the design of medical simulations. Simulation in Healthcare, 10(5), 295–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 300–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2003). The value of positive emotions: the emerging science of positive psychology is coming to understand why it’s good to feel good. American Scientist, 91(4), 330–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2007). Educating the evolved mind: conceptual foundations for an evolutionary educational psychology. In J. S. Carlson & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Educating the evolved mind: conceptual foundations for an evolutionary educational psychology (pp. 1–99). Greenwich: Information Age.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginns, P. (2005). Meta-analysis of the modality effect. Learning and Instruction, 15(4), 313–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, E. T., Sadoski, M., Stricker, A. G., White, T. S., & Wang, Z. (2007). The role of imagery in the production of written definitions. Reading Psychology, 28(3), 241–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harp, S. F., & Mayer, R. E. (1998). How seductive details do their damage: a theory of cognitive interest in science learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(3), 414–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, J. A. (2017). Feelings and emotion-based learning: a new theory. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidig, S., Müller, J., & Reichelt, M. (2015). Emotional design in multimedia learning: differentiation on relevant design features and their effects on emotions and learning. Computers in Human Behavior, 44, 81–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Homer, B. D., Plass, J. L., Rose, M. C., MacNamara, A., Pawar, S., & Ober, T. M. (2018). Activating adolescents’ “hot” executive functions in a digital game to train cognitive skills: the effects of age and prior abilities. Cognitive Development, 49, 20–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., & Levin, P. F. (1972). Effect of feeling good on helping: Cookies and kindness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21(3), 384–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes and social behaviour. In L. Berkowitiz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 203–253). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., & Means, B. (1983). The influence of positive affect on decision-making strategies. Social Cognition, 2(1), 18–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., & Reeve, J. (2005). The Influence of Positive Affect on Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation: Facilitating Enjoyment of Play, Responsible Work Behavior, and Self-Control. Motivation and Emotion, 29(4), 295–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Shalker, T. E., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behavior: A cognitive loop? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(1), 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1122–1131.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19(4), 509–539.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory: how many types of load does it really need? Educational Psychology Review, 23(1), 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S. (2015). Instructional guidance: a cognitive load perspective. Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., & Singh, A. M. (2016). Rethinking the boundaries of cognitive load theory in complex learning. Educational Psychology Review, 28(4), 831–852.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2000). Incorporating learner experience into the design of multimedia instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(1), 126–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleinsmith, L. J., Kaplan, S., & Trate, R. D. (1963). The relationship of arousal to short- and long-term verbal recall. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 17(4), 393–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knörzer, L., Brünken, R., & Park, B. (2016). Facilitators or suppressors: effects of experimentally induced emotions on multimedia learning. Learning & Instruction, 44, 97–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kühl, T., & Zander, S. (2017). An inverted personalization effect when learning with multimedia: the case of aversive content. Computers and Education, 108, 71–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(1), 54–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laird, J. D., Wagener, J. J., Halal, M., & Szegda, M. (1982). Remembering what you feel: the effects of emotion on memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(4), 646–657.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lange, C. G. (1885). The mechanism of the emotions. In B. Rand (Ed.), The classical psychologist (pp. 672–685). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Densmore, M., Boksman, K., Neufeld, R. W., Gati, J. S., & Menon, R. S. (2004). The nature of traumatic memories: a 4-T FMRI functional connectivity analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(1), 36–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion. American Psychologist, 46(4), 352–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 23(4-5), 727–738.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E., & Brown, R. (2017). A higher-order theory of emotional consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(10), E2016–E2025.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levens, S. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2008). Emotion processing effects on interference resolution in working memory. Emotion, 8(2), 267–280. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, P. A., & Critchley, H. D. (2003). Mood-dependent memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(10), 431–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, V. E., & Williams, R. N. (1989). Mood-congruent vs. mood-state-dependent learning: implications for a view of emotion. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 4(2), 157–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loderer, K., Pekrun, R., & Plass, J. L. (2019). Emotional foundations of game-based learning. In J. L. Plass, R. E. Mayer, & B. D. Homer (Eds.), Handbook of game-based learning. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2001). Multimedia learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (Ed.). (2005). Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2009). Multimedia learning (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., & Estrella, G. (2014). Benefits of emotional design in multimedia instruction. Learning and Instruction, 33, 12–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 43–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLean, P. D. (1949). Psychosomatic disease and the "visceral brain"; recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosomatic Medicine, 11, 338–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • McVee, M. B., Dunsmore, K., & Gavelek, J. R. (2005). Schema theory revisited. Review of Educational Research, 75(4), 531–566.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (Eds.). (1999). Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, R. (2005). Instructional technology: promise and pitfalls. In L. PytlikZillig, M. Bodvarsson, & R. Bruning (Eds.), Technology-based education: bringing researchers and practitioners together (pp. 1–19). Greenwich: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, R. (2006). Does the modality principle hold for different media? A test of the method-affects-learning hypothesis. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 22(3), 149–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, R. (2010). Cognitive load theory: more food for thought. Instructional Science, 38(2), 135–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. E. (2004). Personalized messages that promote science learning in virtual environments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 165–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. (2007). Interactive multimodal learning environments. Educational Psychology Review, 19(3), 309–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navratil, S. D., & Kühl, T. (2018). Learning with elaborative interrogations and the impact of learners’ emotional states. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcal.12324.

  • Navratil, S., Kühl, T., & Heidig, S. (2018). Why the cells look like that—the influence of learning with emotional design and elaborative interrogations. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1653.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford, M., Morris, F., Grainger, B., & Williams, J. M. G. (1996). Mood, reasoning, and central executive processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(2), 476–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., & Sweller, J. (2012). An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 24(1), 27–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2004). Cognitive load theory: instructional implications of the interaction between information structures and cognitive architecture. Instructional Science, 32(1–2), 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., Tuovinen, J. E., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Darabi, A. A. (2005). A motivational perspective on the relation between mental effort and performance. Educational Technology, Research and Development, 53(3), 25–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1990). Mental representations: a dual coding approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (2007). Mind and its evolution: a dual coding theoretical approach. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (2013). Dual coding theory, word abstractness, and emotion: a critical review of Kousta et al. (2011). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 282–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, B., Moreno, R., Seufert, T., & Brünken, R. (2011). Does cognitive load moderate the seductive details effect? A multimedia study. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(1), 5–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, B., Flowerday, T., & Brünken, R. (2015a). Cognitive and affective effects of seductive details in multimedia learning. Computers in Human Behavior, 44, 267–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, B., Knörzer, L., Plass, J. L., & Brünken, R. (2015b). Emotional design and positive emotions in multimedia learning: an eyetracking study on the use of anthropomorphisms. Computers & Education, 86, 30–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R. (2000). A social-cognitive, control-value theory of achievement emotions. In J. Heckhausen (Ed.), Advances in psychology, 131. Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 143–163). New York: Elsevier Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educational Psychology Review, 18(4), 315–341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., & Stephens, E. J. (2010). Achievement Emotions: A Control-Value Approach. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(4), 238–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (2012). Academic emotions and student engagement. In Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 259–282). Boston: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., & Perry, R. P. (2014). Control-value theory of achievement emotions. International handbook of emotions in education, 120–141.

  • Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(2), 148–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, L., & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(3), 193–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. A. (2004). Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 198–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plancher, G., Massol, S., Dorel, T., & Chainay, H. (2018). Effect of negative emotional content on attentional maintenance in working memory. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plass, J. L., & Kaplan, U. (2016). Emotional design in digital media for learning. In S. Tettegah & M. Gartmeier (Eds.), Emotions, technology, design, and learning (pp. 131–162). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plass, J. L., Moreno, R., & Brünken, R. (Eds.). (2010). Cognitive load theory. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plass, J. L., Heidig, S., Hayward, E. O., Homer, B. D., & Um, E. (2014). Emotional design in multimedia learning: effects of shape and color on affect and learning. Learning and Instruction, 29, 128–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plass, J. L., Homer, B. D., MacNamara, A., Ober, T., Rose, M. C., Hovey, C. M., Pawar, S., & Olsen, A. (2019). Emotional design for digital games for learning: the affective quality of expression, color, shape, and dimensionality of game characters. Learning and Instruction. (in press).

  • Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 322–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poropat, A. E. (2014). Other-rated personality and academic performance: Evidence and implications. Learning and Individual Differences, 34, 24–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rey, G. D., & Steib, N. (2013). The personalization effect in multimedia learning: the influence of dialect. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(5), 2022–2028.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rikers, R. M. J. P., Schmidt, H. G., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (2000). Knowledge encapsulation and the intermediate effect. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(2), 150–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabourin, J., Rowe, J. P., Mott, B. W., & Lester, J. C. (2011). When off-task is on-task: the affective role of off-task behavior in narrative-centered learning environments. In International Conference on Artificial Intelligence in Education (pp. 534–536). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G. (1984). Television is “easy” and print is “tough”: the differential investment of mental effort in learning as a function of perceptions and attributions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(4), 647–658.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69(5), 379–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz-Friesel, M. (2007). Sprache und emotion. Tübingen: Francke.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seibert, P. S., & Ellis, H. C. (1991). Irrelevant thoughts, emotional mood states, and cognitive task performance. Memory & Cognition, 19(5), 507–513.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seipp, B. (1991). Anxiety and academic performance: A meta-analysis of findings. Anxiety Research, 4(1), 27–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharot, T., & Phelps, E. A. (2004). How arousal modulates memory: disentangling the effects of attention and retention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4(3), 294–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stark, L., Brünken, R., & Park, B. (2018a). Emotional text design in multimedia learning: a mixed-methods study using eye tracking. Computers & Education, 120, 185–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stark, L., Malkmus, E., Stark, R., Brünken, R., & Park, B. (2018b). Learning-related emotions in multimedia learning: an application of control-value theory. Learning & Instruction, 58, 42–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797–811.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12(2), 257–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learning and Instruction, 4(4), 295–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2003). Evolution of human cognitive architecture. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 43, pp. 215–266). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2004). Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture. Instructional Science, 32(1/2), 9–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22(2), 123–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., & Sweller, S. (2006). Natural information processing systems. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 434–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Kirschner, P. A., & Clark, R. E. (2007). Why minimally guided teaching techniques do not work: a reply to commentaries. Educational Psychologist, 42(2), 115–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Um, E., Plass, J. L., Hayward, E. O., & Homer, B. D. (2012). Emotional design in multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(2), 485–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., & Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(1), 411–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeidner, M. (1998). Test anxiety: The state of the art. Springer Science & Business Media.

  • Zlomuzica, A., Preusser, F., Totzeck, C., Dere, E., & Margraf, J. (2016). The impact of different emotional states on the memory for what, where and when features of specific events. Behavioural Brain Research, 298, 181–187.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jan L. Plass.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Plass, J.L., Kalyuga, S. Four Ways of Considering Emotion in Cognitive Load Theory. Educ Psychol Rev 31, 339–359 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09473-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09473-5

Keywords

Navigation