Skip to main content
Log in

Beyond Logical Errors: Preliminary Evidence for the “Looming Vulnerability Distortions Questionnaire” of Cognitive-Perceptual Distortions in Anxiety

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Background

In cognitive models, faulty threat appraisals that are associated with threat cognitions in anxiety are frequently seen as the outcome of logical errors. The looming vulnerability model expands upon such views by emphasizing the role of perceptual and phenomenological distortions in threat estimation. It assumes that anxiety is associated with cognitive-perceptual distortions of time, space, and movement (e.g., space and time compression) that heighten the subjective impression that threats are rapidly approaching, even when they aren’t. The present study was undertaken to develop an easy-to-administer and implement self-report measure to assess such perceptual distortions.

Methods

University students (N = 751; 71% female) completed a battery of online questionnaires that included the Looming Vulnerability Distortions Questionnaire (LVDQ) and measures of the looming cognitive style (LCS), cognitive distortions, social desirability, anxiety, worry, intolerance of uncertainty, and depression.

Results

A bifactor ESEM model displayed excellent fit indices and reliability for the LVDQ. Although the results provided strongest support for the use of a general score over specific subscales, they also support the secondary use of some specific scores for some types of distortions. The LVDQ uniquely predicted variance in LCS, anxiety, worry, intolerance of uncertainty, and depression. Moreover, both the LVDQ and LCS also uniquely predicted scores on a face-valid lab-based task, not explained by logical reasoning distortions.

Conclusions

These results support the idea that the LVDQ is a valid measure of cognitive-perceptual distortions associated with anxiety and indicate that it predicts unique variance in anxiety and other emotional distress not explained by a typical measure of logical errors. Clinical implications and future directions are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adler, A. D., & Strunk, D. R. (2010). Looming maladaptive style as a moderator of risk factors for anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 34, 59–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altan-Atalay, A., & Ayvaşık, H. B. (2019). Antecedents of looming cognitive style: Associations with reported perceived parenting and attachment. Psychological Reports, 122, 96–107. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294117750631

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2009). Exploratory structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 16(3), 397–438. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510903008204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ball, W., & Tronick, E. (1971). Infant response to impending collision: Optical and real. Science, 171, 818–820.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Kerem, A., Lamy, D., & Zakay, D. (2010). When time slows down: The influence of threat on time perception in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 255–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Basanovic, J., Dean, L., Riskind, J. H., & MacLeod, C. (2019). High spider-fearful and low spider-fearful individuals differentially perceive the speed of approaching, but not receding, spider stimuli. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 43, 514–521. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-018-9970-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 893–897. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7967(01)00092-4

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boulet, J., & Boss, M. W. (1991). Reliability and validity of the brief symptom inventory. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 3(3), 433–437. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.3.3.433

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buhr, K., & Dugas, M. J. (2002). The intolerance of uncertainty scale: Psychometric properties of the english version. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 931–945. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00092-4

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calvete, E., Gamez-Guadix, M., & Garcia-Salvador, S. (2015). Social information processing in child-to-parent aggression: Bidirectional associations in a 1-year prospective study. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24, 2204–2216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calvete, E., Riskind, J. H., Orue, I., & Gonzalez-Diez, Z. (2016). Recursive associations among maladaptive cognitions and symptoms of social anxiety and depression: Implications for sex differences. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 35, 807–821.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Card, G., & Dickenson, M. H. (2008). Visually mediated motor planning in the escape response of Drosophila. Current Biology, 18, 1300–1307.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carnahan, N. D., Carter, M. M., & Sbrocco, T. (2022). Intolerance of uncertainty, looming cognitive style, and avoidant coping as predictors of anxiety and depression during COVID-19: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 15, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41811-021-00123-9

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, A. T. (1974). Compulsive neurosis: A review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 311–318.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(3), 464–504. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 233–255. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (2010). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders: Science and practice. Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clerkin, E., Cody, M. W., Stefanucci, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Teachman, B. A. (2009). Imagery and fear influence height perception. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 381–386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, S., Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2013). Affective signals of threat increase perceived proximity. Psychological Science., 24(1), 34–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612446953

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Comrey, A. L., & Lee, H. B. (1992). A first course in factor analysis. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Covin, R., Dozois, D. J. A., Ogniewicz, A., & Seeds, P. M. (2011). Measuring cognitive errors: Initial development of the cognitive distortions scale (CDS). International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 4, 297–322. https://doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2011.4.3.297

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, B. J., Cohen, E., Direnfeld, D. M., & Swinson, R. P. (1996). Does the beck anxiety inventory measure anything beyond panic attack symptoms? Behavior Research and Therapy, 34, 949–954.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67(3), 993–1002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, J. I., Gross, J. J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2011). Psychological distance and emotional experience: What you see is what you get. Emotion, 11, 438–444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derogatis, L. R. (1992). The brief symptom inventory (BSI): Administration, scoring & procedures manual-II. Towson: Clinical Psychometric Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derogatis, L. R., & Melisaratos, N. (1983). The brief symptom inventory: An introductory report. Psychological Medicine, 13(3), 595–605. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291700048017

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dill, L. M. (1990). Distance-to-cover and the escape decisions of an African cichlid fish, Melanochromis chipokae. Environmental Biology of Fishes, 27, 147–152. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00001944

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyer-Oren, S. E., Clerkin, E. M., Edwards, C. B., Teachman, B. A., & , Steinman, S.A. (2019). Believing is seeing: Changes in visual perception following treatment for height fear. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 62, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.08.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Droit-Volet, S., Fayolle, S. L., & Gil, S. (2011). Emotion and time perception: effects of film-induced mood. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2011.00033

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Effron, D. A., Niedenthal, P. M., Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2006). Embodied temporal perception of emotion. Emotion, 6, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.1

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elwood, L. S., Riskind, J. H., & Olatunji, B. O. (2011). Looming vulnerability: Incremental validity of a fearful cognitive distortion in contamination fears. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 40–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Etkin, J., Evangelidis, I., & Aaker, J. (2015). Pressed for time? Goal conflict shapes how time is perceived, spent, and valued. Journal of Marketing Research, 52, 394–406. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.14.0130

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fayolle, S., Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2015). Fear and time: Fear speeds up the internal clock. Behavioural Processes, 120, 135–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feusner, J. D., Hembacher, E., Moller, H., & Moody, T. D. (2011). Abnormalities of object visual processing in body dysmorphic disorder. Psychological Medicine, 41, 2385–2397. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711000572

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 20–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franconeri, S. L., & Simons, D. J. (2003). Moving and looming stimuli capture attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 65, 999–1010. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03194829

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeston, M. H., Rhéaume, J., Letarte, H., Dugas, M. J., & Ladouceur, R. (1994). Why do people worry? Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 791–802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Givon-Benjio, N., & Okon-Singer, H. (2020). Biased estimations of interpersonal distance in non-clinical social anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 69, 102171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2019.102171

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • González Díez, Z., Calvete, E., Riskind, J. H., & Orue, I. (2015). Test of an hypothesized structural model of the relationships between cognitive style and social anxiety: A 12-month prospective study. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 30, 59–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenblatt-Kimron, L., & Cohen, M. (2020). The role of cognitive processing in the relationship of posttraumatic stress symptoms and depression among older Holocaust survivors: A moderated-mediation model. Anxiety Stress and Coping, 33, 59–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gwilliam, G. F. (1963). The mechanism of the shadow reflex in Cirripedia I: Electrical activity in the supraesophageal ganglion and ocellar nerve. The Biological Bulletin, 125, 470–485. https://doi.org/10.2307/1539360

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haeffel, G. J., Gibb, B. E., Abramson, L. Y., Alloy, L. B., Metalsky, G. I., Joiner, T., Hankin, B. L., & Swendsen, J. (2008). Measuring cognitive vulnerability to depression: Development and validation of the cognitive style questionnaire. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 824–836.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hammer J. & Toland M. (2016). Bifactor analysis in Mplus [Video file]. Retrieved from http://sites.education.uky.edu/apslab/upcoming-events/

  • Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 81–91. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.81

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, E. A., & Mathews, A. (2010). Mental imagery in emotion and emotional disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 349–362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.001. Epub 2010 Jan 18 PMID: 20116915.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ji, J. L., Heyes, S. B., MacLeod, C., & Holmes, E. A. (2016). Emotional mental imagery as simulation of reality: Fear and beyond-a tribute to Peter Lang. Behaviour Therapy, 47, 702–719. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2015.11.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Judd, A., Sim, J., Cho, J., Muhlenen, A., & Lleras, A. (2004). Motion perception, awareness and attention effects with looming motion. Journal of Vision, 4, 608–608. https://doi.org/10.1167/4.8.608

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, D., Rector, N. A., & Riskind, J. (2017). Reduction in looming cognitive style in cognitive-behavioral therapy: Effect on post-treatment symptoms across anxiety disorders and within generalized anxiety disorder. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 10(4), 346–358. https://doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2017.10.4.346

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiman, E. M., & Riskind, J. H. (2012). Cognitive vulnerability to comorbidity: Looming cognitive style and depressive cognitive style as synergistic predictors of anxiety and depression symptoms. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43(4), 1109–1114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiman, E. M., & Riskind, J. H. (2014). Negative cognitive style and looming cognitive style synergistically predict stress generation. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 27, 347–369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J., Wapner, S., & Werner, H. (1961). The effect of danger upon the perception of time. American Journal of Psychology, 74, 94–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J., Werner, H., & Wapner, S. (1965). Apparent speed of walking under conditions of danger. Journal of General Psychology, 73, 291–298.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leibovich, T., Cohen, N., & Henik, A. (2016). Itsy bitsy spider? Valence and self-relevance predict size estimation. Biological Psychology, 121, 138–145.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, T., Ode, S., Moeller, S. K., & Robinson, M. D. (2013). Neuroticism as distancing: Perceptual sources of evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(5), 907–920. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031969

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Markon, K. E. (2019). Bifactor and hierarchical models: Specification, inference, and interpretation. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15, 51–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, A., Ciersdorff, A., Gillath, O., et al. (2021). Effects of cognitive load and type of object on the visual looming bias. Attention, Perception and Psychophysiology, 83, 1508–1517. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-021-02271-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn state worry questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 487–495. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milovanović, T., Ajanović, A., & Živčić-Bećirević, I. (2022). The role of mother’s looming cognitive style, mother’s behaviour and stressful life events in children’s anxiety and depression. Psychological Topics, 31(2), 259–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mobbs, D., Yu, R., Rowe, J. B., Eich, H., FeldmanHall, O., & Dalgleish, T. (2010). Neural activity associated with monitoring the oscillating threat value of a tarantula. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science U S a., 107, 20582–20586. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1009076107. Epub 2010 Nov 8 PMID: 21059963.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morin, A. J., Arens, A. K., & Marsh, H. W. (2016). A bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling framework for the identification of distinct sources of construct-relevant psychometric multidimensionality. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 23(1), 116–139. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2014.961800

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muhlberger, A., Neumann, R., Wieser, M. J., & Pauli, P. (2008). The impact of changes in spatial distance on emotional responses. Emotion, 8, 192–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2021). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.

    Google Scholar 

  • Özdel, K., Taymur, I., Guriz, S. O., Tulaci, R. G., Kuru, E., & Türkçapar, M. H. (2014). Measuring cognitive errors using the cognitive distortions scale (CDS): Psychometric properties in clinical and non-clinical samples. PLoS ONE, 9(8), e105956–e105956. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105956

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, A., & Alais, D. (2007). A bias for looming stimuli to predominate in binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 47, 2661–2674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2007.06.019

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus, D. L. (1988). Assessing self-deception and impression management in self-reports: The balanced inventory of desirable responding. Retrieved from D. L. Paulhus, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Y7

  • Paulhus, D. L., & Lim, D. T. K. (1994). Arousal and evaluative extremity in social judgements: A dynamic complexity model. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 89–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pilz, K. S., Vuong, Q. C., Bülthoff, H. H., & Thornton, I. M. (2011). Walk this way: Approaching bodies can influence the processing of faces. Cognition, 118, 17–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.09.004

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. J., & Cuk, M. (1992). Fearful distortions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 583–589.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapee, R. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (1997). A cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 741–756.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reise, S. P., Scheines, R., Widaman, K. F., & Haviland, M. G. (2013). Multidimensionality and structural coefficient bias in structural equation modeling: A bifactor perspective. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 73(1), 5–26. https://doi.org/10.3138/jcfs.34.1.41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Abreu, K., Strauss, M., & Holt, R. (1997). Looming vulnerability to spreading contamination in subclinical OCD. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 405–414.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Black, D., & Shahar, G. (2010). Cognitive vulnerability to anxiety in the stress generation process: Interaction between the looming cognitive style and anxiety sensitivity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 124–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Calvete, E., & Black, D. (2017a). Effects of looming cognitive style and time course on anxiety about an impending speech. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 36, 814–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Calvete, E., Gonzalez, Z., Orue, I., Kleiman, E. M., & Shahar, G. (2013). Direct and mediated pathways of looming cognitive style on social anxiety, depression, and hostility: An SEM study. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 1, 73–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Kelly, K., Moore, R., Harman, W., & Gaines, H. (1992). The looming of danger: Does it discriminate focal phobia and general anxiety from depression? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Kleiman, E. M., Seifritz, E., & Neuhoff, J. (2014). Influence of anxiety, depression and looming cognitive style on auditory looming perception. Journal of Anxiety Disorders., 28, 45–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Moore, R., & Bowley, L. (1995). The looming of spiders: The fearful perceptual distortion of movement and menace. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 171–178.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., & Rector, N. A. (2007). Beyond belief: Incremental prediction of OCD by looming vulnerability illusions. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 21, 243–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., & Rector, N. A. (2018). Looming vulnerability: Theory, research and practice. Springer Nature.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Rector, N. A., & Taylor, S. (2012). Looming cognitive vulnerability to anxiety and its reduction in psychotherapy. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 22, 137–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Sagliano, L., Trojano, L., & Conson, M. (2016). Dysfunctional freezing responses to approaching stimuli in persons with a looming cognitive style for physical threats. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00521

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Sica, C., Bottesi, G., Ghisi, M., & Kashdan, T. D. (2017b). Cognitive vulnerabilities in parents as a risk factor for anxiety symptoms in young adult offspring: An exploration of looming cognitive style. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 54, 229–238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.08.010

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Sica, C., Caudek, C., Bottessi, G., Disabato, D. J., & Ghia, M. (2021). Looming cognitive style more consistently predicts anxiety than depressive symptoms: Evidence from a 3-wave yearlong study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 45, 747–758. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-020-10189-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Tzur, D., Williams, N., Mann, B., & Shahar, G. (2007). Short-term predictive effects of the looming cognitive style on anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1765–1777.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., & Williams, N. L. (1999). Cognitive case conceptualization and the treatment of anxiety disorders: Implications of the looming vulnerability model. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 13, 295–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Williams, N. L., Gessner, T., Chrosniak, L. D., & Cortina, J. (2000). The looming maladaptive style: Anxiety, danger, and schematic processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 837–852.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rodriguez, A., Reise, S. P., & Haviland, M. G. (2016). Evaluating bifactor models: Calculating and interpreting statistical indices. Psychological Methods, 21(2), 137–150. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000045

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shafir, T., Taylor, S. F., Atkinson, A. P., Langenecker, S. A., & Zubieta, J.-K. (2013). Emotion regulation through execution, observation, and imagery of emotional movements. Brain and Cognition, 82, 219–227.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shiban, Y., Fruth, M. B., Pauli, P., Kinateder, M., Reichenberger, J., & Mühlberger, A. (2016). Treatment effect on biases in size estimation in spider phobia. Biological Psychology, 121, 146–152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sica, C., Caudek, C., Chiri, L. R., Ghisi, M., & Marchetti, I. (2012). “Not just right experiences” predict obsessive-compulsive symptoms in non-clinical Italian individuals: A one-year longitudinal study. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 1, 159–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teachman, B. A., Stefanucci, J. K., Clerkin, E. M., Cody, M. W., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). A new mode of fear expression: Perceptual bias in height fear. Emotion, 8(2), 296–301. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.296

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tolin, D. F., Worhunsky, P., & Maltby, N. (2004). Sympathetic magic in contamination-related OCD. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35, 193–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vagnoni, E., Lourenco, S. F., & Longo, M. R. (2012). Threat modulates perception of looming visual stimuli. Current Biology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.053. PMID: 23058796.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Wassenhove, V., Wittmann, M., Craig, A. D., & Paulus, M. P. (2011). Psychological and neural mechanisms of subjective time dilation. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5, 56. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2011.00056

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vasey, M. W., Vilensky, M. R., Heath, J. H., Harbaugh, C. N., Buffington, A. G., & Fazio, R. H. (2012). It was as big as my head, I swear! Biased spider size estimation in spider phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26, 20–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.08.009

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waldman, A., Loomes, R., Mountford, V. A., et al. (2013). Attitudinal and perceptual factors in body image distortion: An exploratory study in patients with anorexia nervosa. Journal of Eating Disorders, 1, 17. https://doi.org/10.1186/2050-2974-1-17

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, M. W. (2013). Omega. [Computer software]. Phoenix, AZ: Ed & Psych Associates.

  • Watts, F. N., & Sharrock, R. (1984). Fear and time estimation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 59(2), 597–598.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Westby, G. W. M., Keay, K. A., Redgrave, P., Dean, P., & Bannister, M. (1990). Output pathways from the rat superior colliculus mediating approach and avoidance have different sensory properties. Experimental Brain Research, 81, 626–638. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02423513

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witt, J. K., & Sugovic, M. (2013). Spiders appear to move faster than non-threatening objects regardless of one’s ability to block them. Acta Psychologica, 143(3), 284–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeo, G. C., Hong, R. Y., & Riskind, J. H. (2020). Looming cognitive style and its associations with anxiety and depression: A meta-analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 44, 445–467. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-020-10089-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zamani, E., Akbari, M., Mohammadkhani, S., Riskind, J. H., Drake, C. L., & Palagini, L. (2021). The relationship of neuroticism with sleep quality: The mediating role of emotional. Cognitive and Metacognitive Factors, Behavioral Sleep Medicine. https://doi.org/10.1080/15402002.2021.1888730

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Julian Basanovic for his helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. This research was supported by a grant from the Basque Country (Ref. IT1532-22).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John H. Riskind.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

John H. Riskind and Esther Calvete declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Informed Consent

All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (national and institutional).

Animal Rights Statements

No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 27 KB)

Appendix: LVDQ Questionnaire

Appendix: LVDQ Questionnaire

Instructions:

In this questionnaire, we are studying different types of thinking that people have when are anxious or worry about situations. For every type of thinking listed below, there will be three examples–one dealing with social relationships, one dealing with personal achievement (in jobs, school, or career), and one dealing with physical threats. We would like you to do your best to read and understand what each of these types of thinking is like. You will be asked to estimate how often you engage in each type of thinking in the three categories described above (social, achievement, and physical). Please consider each of your answers carefully.

  1. 1.

    *Size Distortion

When people see something or someone that is possibly threatening or makes them uncomfortable (e.g., an angry boss, a spider, a stain on their shirt when in public), they sometimes perceive the threat to be physically larger or bigger in size than it is.

  1. A.

    Social Situation Example. Clark sees a stain on his shirt when in public and sees the stain as much bigger than it actually is.

Please estimate how often you engage in size distortion when in social situations (like when you’re with friends, partners, family or in public).

figure a
  1. B.

    Achievement Situation Example. Jack is called into his boss’ office to talk about a recent mistake that he just made. Jack sees his boss is much bigger in size than is true. Please estimate how often you engage in size distortion when in achievement situations (such as in school or work).

figure b
  1. C.

    Physical Threat Situation. When Kelly sees a spider on the other side of the room, the spider seems much bigger in size than is true.

Please estimate how often you engage in size distortion when in when in situations that seem physically threatening.

figure c
  1. 2.

    *Space Compression

People sometimes perceive the amount of distance or physical space between themselves and something or someone that is threatening as being smaller and closer than it is.

To illustrate this, please read the following passages:

  1. A.

    Social Situation Example. While at a party, Neil sees the romantic partner that he just broke-up with glaring angrily at him from 10 feet away. She looks like she is ready to start a fight and Neil sees her as standing much closer than is true.

Please estimate how often you engage in Space Compression when in social situations (like when you’re with friends, partners, family or in public)).

figure d
  1. B.

    Achievement Situation Example. Phil gets called into his boss’ office to talk about Phil’s recent mistakes. He experiences boss as sitting closer than is true.

Please estimate how often you engage in Space Compression when in achievement situations (such as in school or work).

figure e
  1. C.

    Physical Threat Situation Example. When leaving her tent while camping, Nora sees a large snake. The snake is several yards away, but she sees it as being much closer than is true.

Please estimate how often you engage in Space Compression when in situations that seem physically threatening.

figure f
  1. 3.

    *Misperceiving Threats as Physically Moving Closer

People may see potentially threatening things as moving closer and physically approaching them when this is not the case.

  1. A.

    Social Example. While grocery shopping, Beth sees someone who recently said ugly and insulting things to her. She sees the person as physically moving in her direction even though they aren’t.

Please estimate how often you engage in the misperceiving threats as approaching distortion when in social situations (like when you’re with friends, partners, family or in public)).

figure g
  1. B.

    Achievement Example. Rick’s boss moves into the hallway by his office as Rick is playing a game on his computer. Rick sees her as walking towards him more than is true.

Please estimate how often you engage in the misperceiving threats as approaching distortion when in achievement situations (such as in school or work).

figure h
  1. C.

    Potential Physical Threat Example. When visiting a neighborhood park, Brad notices a large stray dog and is unsure whether it is dangerous. Brad sees it as moving closer to him more than is true.

Please estimate how often you engage in the misperceiving threats as approaching when in situations that seem physically threatening.

figure i
  1. 4.

    *Time Compression

People sometimes experience the closeness (or distance) in time between themselves and an event that is threatening to them as or that makes them uncomfortable as smaller than it is.

  1. A.

    Social Situation Example. Monty just remembers that he has forgotten to get his good friend a gift for his wedding that is coming up in three weeks. Despite the fact that Monty has plenty of time, he experiences the wedding as if it were going to occur more soon than is true.

Please estimate how often you engage in time compression when in social situations (like when you’re with friends, partners, family or in public)).

figure j
  1. B.

    Achievement Situation Example. Erica has weeks to finish a project for work that is important to her career. Despite the fact that she has plenty of time, she experiences the deadline as it were only a week away.

Please estimate how often you engage in time compression when in achievement situations (such as in school or work).

figure k
  1. C.

    Potential Physical Threat Example. Julia is terrified of flying and has an airplane flight ticket to fly in three weeks. Despite the three weeks, she experiences the date of her flight as if it were much closer than is true.

Please estimate how often you engage in time compression when in situations that seem physically threatening.

figure l
  1. 5.

    *ALL-at-Once Distortion

People may experience a number of independent or unique things that they are worried about in the future, as though they will come all at once.

  1. A.

    Social Situation Example. Sarah has an important date with someone new next week, a party she needs to plan for her friend’s graduation in a month, and a difficult visit with family members in six months. DESPITE the distance in time between the different events Sarah experiences them as if she has to deal with them all- at- once.

  2. B.

    Achievement Situation Example. Larry has a work project due next week, a job review he’s concerned about in a month, and is getting a new boss he doesn’t know in six months. Despite the distance in time between the events, Larry feels as if he has to deal with all of the events at the same time.

  3. C.

    Physical Threat Situation. Jonah will be going on a difficult fitness test he is nervous about during the next two weeks, a long and uncomfortable trip in a month, and major surgery in six months. In spite of the distance in time between the events, Jonah experiences the events as if he has to deal with them all at the same time.

  4. 6.

    *Experiencing Times as Moving Faster Than It Is

When confronted with a potentially threatening or unpleasant situation, people often experience time as passing far more quickly than is actually the case.

  1. A.

    Social Situation Example. Jim agreed to speak at his best friend’s wedding despite a fear of public speaking. As the date approaches, he feels like time is going by much faster than it is.

  2. B.

    Achievement Situation Example. Jane could fail to make the course grade she needs if she doesn’t get a good grade on an exam that is coming up. As she thinks about the coming exam, she experiences time as passing by faster than is true.

  3. C.

    Physical Threat Situation. Jake is in the middle of the street and sees a car coming. Despite the fact that Jake has plenty of time to get across the street to safety, he experiences time as passing by much faster than it is.

  4. 7.

    *Odds Rising Too Rapidly

People sometimes see the odds (or chances) of feared or dreaded things occurring as increasing faster than is true as the events approach.

  1. A.

    Achievement Situation Example. Shelly waits for someone to arrive for their first date which starts in 20 min. As each minute passes, she feels more and more convinced that the date will probably go badly.

  2. B.

    Becky is about to take a big exam in 30 min that she must pass to get a job. She has the skill, but as each minute passes she gets more convinced that she will probably fail.

  3. C.

    Dan is waiting for test results that will determine whether or not he needs to go to the hospital for a painful surgery. As each minute passes while he waits, he becomes increasingly convinced that he will probably need the surgery.

  4. 8.

    *Time Moving Too Slowly

When facing threatening or uncomfortable situations, people may experience time as passing more slowly than appears natural, causing them to perceive time as practically stationary and standing still.

  1. A.

    Social Situation Example. Diana has delivered some painful news to a friend. While waiting for the friend to respond, she experiences time as if is moving is standing still and moving in extreme slow motion.

  2. B.

    Achievement Situation Example. While Andrew is waiting for a job interview to begin, he experiences time as if it is standing still and each moment as it is taking forever.

  3. C.

    Physical Threat Situation. As Allison finds that her car is skidding and sliding on a wet road, see sees that she is just about to hit a bus. As she watches, she experiences time as if it is moving in extreme slow motion.

  4. 9.

    *Minimizing Intervening Events

When people worry that an event could happen in the future, they sometimes overlook many other factors that could affect the outcome.

  1. A.

    Social Situation Example. Mark hears the beginnings of a rumor that could cause harm to his reputation. He doesn’t consider that others may not hear the rumor or believe the rumor or keep spreading the rumor.

  2. B.

    Achievement Situation Example. After failing her first-class exam, Anne feels she is sure to fail the whole class. She overlooks the fact that she would need to fail all her other exams and well as neglect all her future homework and final project to fail the course.

  3. C.

    Physical Threat Situation. After Bailey hears about a house robbery nearby, she fears sure that the robber will break into her house. She doesn’t consider that the there are many other houses in the neighborhood that the robber can break into, and that the robber can move on to other neighborhoods or caught soon.

  4. 10.

    *Minimizing Coping Time

People sometimes perceive that they don’t have enough time to influence events when, in fact, they do.

  1. A.

    Social Situation Example. Sally will host her sister’s graduation party in a month. She has plenty of time but experiences herself as having less time than is true.

  2. B.

    Achievement Situation Example. Sean has to pass an upcoming exam. Although he has plenty of time, he experiences self as having too little time to prepare.

  3. C.

    Physical Threat Situation. Carla sees a large wasp at rest on the wall on the other side of the room. She has plenty of time to get away, but she experiences herself as not having enough time to do this.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Riskind, J.H., Calvete, E. Beyond Logical Errors: Preliminary Evidence for the “Looming Vulnerability Distortions Questionnaire” of Cognitive-Perceptual Distortions in Anxiety. Cogn Ther Res 47, 802–822 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-023-10370-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-023-10370-z

Keywords

Navigation