Introduction

In recent years, the field of psychotherapy has seen a rise of novel transdiagnostic treatments that have the potential to provide easier access to evidence-based mental health care by addressing a wider range of disorders and lowering barriers to treatment (e.g., Dalgleish et al., 2020; Sauer-Zavala, et al., 2017a, 2017b; Schaeuffele, et al., 2021b). Transdiagnostic treatments show promising effects when delivered face-to-face and over the internet (Newby et al., 2015; Păsărelu et al., 2017; Pearl & Norton, 2017), with equivalence to gold-standard single disorder protocols (e.g., Barlow et al., 2017; Pearl & Norton, 2017). However, overall — across single disorder and transdiagnostic treatments — there remains a need to improve effectiveness and response rates. A better understanding of the active ingredients of psychotherapy can help to create more effective treatments and lower non-response rates (Kazdin, 2007). With the emergence of personalized and modularized treatments, knowledge on change mechanisms can also help to create a better fit between therapy and patient (Hofmann & Hayes, 2019). However, after years of research and advancements, the factors underlying change are still poorly understood (Cuijpers et al., 2019; Kazdin, 2007). This may be due to the fact that investigating mechanisms of change is a complicated endeavor. Determining whether a factor constitutes a (causal) mechanism of change requires a theoretical foundation of putative mechanisms to guide the investigation, the establishment of temporal precedence (change occurs first in the mediator followed by change in symptoms), the inclusion of several mediators, and ideally a manipulation of the mediating variable (Cuijpers et al., 2019; Kazdin, 2007).

A common distinction in mechanisms of change is between common and specific factors. Common factors are active across virtually all therapies and settings, whereas specific factors, as the name suggests, are more treatment-specific (Cuijpers et al., 2019; Wampold, 2015). The most robust evidence on common factors has been found for therapeutic alliance and treatment expectancy (Baier et al., 2020; Constantino et al., 2018; Flückiger et al., 2018). Therapeutic alliance and treatment expectancy have also been shown to predict and mediate outcomes in the internet-based setting (Berger, 2017; Boettcher et al., 2013; El Alaoui et al., 2015, 2016; Flückiger et al., 2018; Probst et al., 2019). In comparison, the body of research on relevant specific factors is less clear (e.g., Domhardt et al., 2020; Kazantzis et al., 2018). The number of potential candidate mechanisms impedes the study of specific factors, with every disorder-specific model encompassing specific processes that cause or maintain the disorder. Evidence on how these specific processes are related to outcome is difficult to aggregate and summarize across disorders. In contrast, the transdiagnostic approach, with its emphasis on shared mechanisms that should be targeted in treatment, provides an attractive framework for investigating mediating variables (Sauer-Zavala, et al., 2017b).

The Unified Protocol (UP) is one example of a theory-driven transdiagnostic treatment for emotional disorders (Barlow et al., 2016; Ellard et al., 2010). The authors hypothesize that individuals with emotional disorders share a tendency to frequently and intensely experience negative affect and exhibit maladaptive reactions to these high negative emotions (Barlow et al., 2014; Bullis et al., 2019). The UP aims at changing these dysfunctional reactions towards emotions by increasing mindful emotion awareness as well as cognitive flexibility and decreasing avoidance (Barlow et al., 2004). These processes are increasingly recognized as transdiagnostic mediators of change across CBT treatments (e.g., Eustis et al., 2016; Goldin et al., 2016; Kocovski et al., 2015).

First studies started to investigate transdiagnostic processes in relation to outcome within the UP framework. As an important prerequisite, the modules of psychoeducation, mindful emotion awareness, cognitive flexibility, and countering emotional behaviors led to changes in these putative mechanisms (Sauer-Zavala, et al., 2017a). Single case experimental design studies found that mindfulness, cognitive flexibility, and anxiety sensitivity were associated with outcome (Boswell, 2013; Boswell et al., 2014; Brake et al., 2016). Findings on avoidance are mixed. In single case experimental design studies, experiential avoidance did not exhibit substantial changes following treatment (Boswell et al., 2014; Brake et al., 2016) and did not predict or proceed changes in anxiety or depression in an individual with comorbid depression and generalized anxiety (Boswell et al., 2014). In a larger RCT, Eustis et al. (2019) investigated the mediating effect of experiential avoidance across the UP and single-disorder treatment protocols for mixed anxiety disorders. In contrast to these single case studies, experiential avoidance mediated and preceded changes across the UP and single-disorder protocols, suggesting that experiential avoidance is a general transdiagnostic mechanism in CBT (Eustis et al., 2019). One meta-analysis aggregated the UP’s effects on emotion regulation (Sakiris & Berle, 2019). They differentiated between adaptive (e.g., mindfulness, reappraisal) and maladaptive (e.g. experiential avoidance, thought suppression) emotion regulation skills (Sakiris & Berle, 2019). Overall, they found preliminary evidence that adaptive strategies increased moderately over the course of treatment and that there were moderate to large effects on experiential avoidance and negligible effects on thought suppression.

Taken together, while there is preliminary evidence for the putative transdiagnostic mechanisms in the UP, the majority of findings are correlational, limited to anxiety disorders, and stem from small sample sizes. Only Eustis et al. (2019) investigated mediating effects in a larger sample, but their investigation was limited to one transdiagnostic process.

Our primary goal for this study was to extend previous findings on the relevance of transdiagnostic processes, specifically mindfulness, cognitive flexibility, anxiety sensitivity, and avoidance, in the UP in an internet-based setting for emotional disorders beyond anxiety disorders. We were interested whether these transdiagnostic processes were malleable by treatment and to which extent they mediated treatment effects — both in isolation and in a multiple mediator model taking into account their interrelation.

Method

We collected data within a randomized controlled trial examining the efficacy of an internet-based version of the UP for anxiety, depressive, and somatic symptom disorders (Schaeuffele et al., 2020). For this purpose, treatment was compared to waitlist in a 1:1 ratio (N = 129). The treatment yielded large effects on symptom distress in comparison to waitlist. 69% of participants in the treatment group were considered responders (as determined based on the reliable change index (RCI = 1.96)). The proportion of responders would shift to a more conservative estimate of 45% if we considered all participants that did not report post-treatment data as non-responders. Following the waiting period of 10 weeks, participants of the waitlist received access to the intervention.

Participants

We recruited participants by self-selection in online mental health forums, on social media platforms, as well as over targeted ads on a popular search engine. We included participants with an anxiety, depressive, or somatic symptom disorder as a primary diagnosis in the trial. While the UP has primarily been applied in the treatment of anxiety disorders (e.g., Cassiello-Robbins et al., 2020), the rationale should be expandable to other disorders with strong emotional components like depression and somatic symptom disorders (Farchione et al., 2012). Participants were diagnosed with a structured clinical interview based on DSM-5 via telephone prior to randomization (Diagnostic interview for mental disorders (DIPS); Margraf et al., 2017). We excluded participants if they (1) currently underwent psychotherapy, (3) were under 18, (4) changed their medication dosage in the preceding three months, (5) showed indications of acute suicidality, (6) were diagnosed with a substance use disorder, (7) experienced symptoms of mania, or (8) psychosis. See Fig. 1 for a detailed flow of participants through the inclusion process.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Consort flow chart

Intervention

The intervention is a 10-week adaptation of the UP for internet-based use. For this purpose, the UP workbook was translated one-to-one from English into German. In a next step, the content was divided into ten modules. In several editing loops, the paragraphs were shortened, simplified, and summarized, while retaining the core content and exercises. Figure 2 shows core concepts of the 10 final modules. Participants had to complete a module in order to start with a new module. To complete a module, participants had to complete a reflection on how this module helped them work towards their goals. We recommended that participants work on one module per week, but participants were free to complete the modules at their own pace. In theory, participants could skip exercises and leave the reflection blank to start a new module, but this only affected a negligible proportion of modules and participants. Participants on average completed 7.22 modules. 73% of participants completed the modules corresponding to the core treatment targets of the UP (mindfulness, cognitive flexibility, avoidance) and 42% completed all ten modules. Online therapists were the first and last author as well as 16 advanced students of clinical psychology who were trained in a 1-day workshop and supervised. Online therapists provided written feedback once per week on a fixed day over the course of 10 weeks. Therapists based their feedback on the record sheets that participants completed within the online therapy program. The therapists focused their written feedback on increasing engagement and motivation as well as providing guidance on how to work and advance with the therapeutic materials. Online therapists, for example, provided guidance when they felt that participants did not engage with the exercises in the intended manner or struggled with adaptive solutions (e.g. therapists would guide participants to come up with alternative adaptive behaviors or offer personalized examples of possible alternative thoughts).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Core concepts of the intervention and measurement points of mediators and outcome. SMQ Southampton Mindfulness Questionnaire, BSI Brief Symptom Distress Inventory, ERQ Emotion Regulation Questionnaire, ASI Anxiety Sensitivity Index, BEAQ Brief Experiential Avoidance Questionnaire, BADS Behavioral Activation for Depression Scale

Ethics and Trial Registration

The ethics committee of the Department of Education and Psychology at Freie Universitaet Berlin, Germany, approved the trial (186/2018). The trial was registered at the German Clinical Trial Registry DRKS (DRKS00014820).

Measures

All assessments were self-rated on the online platform. Figure 2 shows an overview of measurement time points. Outcome and mediators were assessed at baseline (t0). All putative mediators were assessed immediately following the module that targeted the construct to ensure participants had received the dosage of treatment (t1). For mechanisms targeted early in treatment (mindfulness and cognitive flexibility), we additionally administered the measures at post-treatment to explore further changes (t2). We did not assess anxiety sensitivity and avoidance at post-treatment due to temporal proximity to the module 9/week 9 assessment. We assumed that one module was completed per week. Accordingly, we assessed the transdiagnostic processes in the waitlist group in the week that corresponded to the number of the module (e.g., we assessed mindfulness after module 3 in the treatment and after 3 weeks in the waitlist group). The percentage of missing values was comparatively high (see Table 2). Technical problems at the beginning of the study led to low response rates in the waitlist control group. As many participants in the intervention group did not complete all modules, missingness rates were higher for measures administered in later modules.

Symptom Distress

We assessed symptom distress with the 18-item Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI-18) (Derogatis, 2000; German version: Franke et al., 2011). The BSI-18 is internally reliable and demonstrates satisfactory psychometric properties (Derogatis & Fitzpatrick, 2004; Franke et al., 2011; Prinz et al., 2013).

Mindfulness

We assessed mindfulness with the 16-item Southampton Mindfulness Questionnaire (SMQ) (Chadwick et al., 2008; German version: Böge et al., 2020). The SMQ assesses mindfulness as the reaction to distressing thoughts and images. Example items include “Usually when I experience distressing thoughts and images, I judge the thought/image as good or bad” and “Usually when I experience distressing thoughts and images, I just notice them and let them go”. The SMQ is unidimensional and internally reliable (Böge et al., 2020; Chadwick et al., 2008).

Cognitive Flexibility

We assessed cognitive flexibility with the 6-item reappraisal subscale of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) (Gross & John, 2003; German version: Abler & Kessler, 2009). Example items include “When I want to feel less negative emotion, I change the way I’m thinking about the situation.” and “I control my emotions by changing the way I think about the situation I’m in”. The ERQ has satisfactory psychometric properties (Abler & Kessler, 2009; Gross & John, 2003).

Anxiety Sensitivity

We assessed anxiety sensitivity with the 18-item Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (ASI-3) (Taylor et al., 2007; German version: Kemper et al., 2009). The scale assesses three facets of anxiety sensitivity: Physical, cognitive, and social concerns. Respective example items include “When my chest feels tight, I get scared that I won’t be able to breathe properly”, “When my mind goes blank, I worry there is something terribly wrong with me.”, and “When I begin to sweat in a social situation, I fear people will think negatively of me.”. Psychometric properties are satisfactory (Kemper et al., 2009; Taylor et al., 2007).

Avoidance

The UP targets avoidance by countering avoidance tendencies and engaging in emotional exposures. For depressed patients specifically, targeting avoidance relates to overcoming tendencies of withdrawal and engaging more actively with valued activities which in turn should increase activation (Boswell et al., 2018). In order to capture different aspects of avoidance and be more reflective of the proportion of depressed participants in our sample, we assessed both experiential avoidance, which has been described as the unwillingness to remain in contact with distressing emotions, thoughts, memories, and physical sensations, as well as behavioral activation.

Experiential Avoidance

We assessed experiential avoidance with the 15-item Brief Experiential Avoidance Questionnaire (BEAQ) (Gámez et al., 2014; German version: Schaeuffele, et al., 2021a). The BEAQ is internally reliable (Gámez et al., 2014). Example items include “The key to a good life is never feeling any pain” and “I go out of my way to avoid uncomfortable situations”. We used our own German translation of the scale. Psychometric properties were investigated in a student and clinical population with comparable results to the English original. Our translation showed high internal reliability (.80–.81), 7 to 13-day test–retest-reliability (.77–.86), and a favorable pattern of convergent and divergent validity in comparison to other measures of experiential avoidance. Our findings indicated that the BEAQ is well represented by a bifactor structure that includes a general experiential avoidance factor and specific factors of individual facets of experiential avoidance (e.g., distress aversion or distraction). Future studies should substantiate this preliminary finding. For the purpose of this study, we relied on the total scale.

Behavioral Activation

We assessed behavioral avoidance and activation with the 9-item Behavioral Activation for Depression Scale Short Form (BADS) (Manos et al., 2011; German version: Fuhr et al., 2016). The BADS has two subscales, an activation and an avoidance subscale. While originally developed to measure changes during behavioral activation for depression, items are phrased transdiagnostically. Example items include “I engaged in a wide and diverse array of activities” or “Most of what I did was to escape from or avoid something unpleasant”. The instrument has shown good psychometric properties (Fuhr et al., 2016; Manos et al., 2011).

Statistical Analyses

The accompanying de-identified data set is available at https://osf.io/hr5wb/. We ran all statistical analyses in RStudio, version 1.2.5033 (RStudio Team, 2019). For the transdiagnostic processes, we first investigated whether the treatment led to significant changes in the putative mediators (treatment, time, and the interaction of treatment and time as fixed and individuals as random effects, mediator as outcome) with linear mixed effects models using the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2014), obtaining p values with the lmer-test package (Kuznetsova et al., 2017). We also calculated effect sizes (Hedge’s g) using estimated means and pooled standard deviations.

We then tested all putative transdiagnostic processes in separate mediator models with group as predictor and symptom distress at post-treatment as outcome (see Fig. 3). For mindfulness and cognitive flexibility two time points were included in these models, while for anxiety sensitivity, avoidance, and activation only one time point was available for the comparison. In a next step, we combined all mediators that showed a significant mediating effect in the separate mediator models in a multiple mediator model (see Fig. 4), where residuals of mediators were allowed to covary (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). Again, two time points were included for mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Mediation models were fitted as path analyses with bias-corrected bootstrapping (10,000 draws) and full maximum likelihood estimation to account for missing values using the lavaan package (Rosseel, 2012). If the bias-corrected bootstrapped confidence interval does not contain zero, then the effect of treatment is at least partially dependent on the mediator and the indirect effect can be considered significant (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). We controlled all mediators and outcomes for their baseline value, following findings from a simulation study that found this approach superior to other methods like difference scores in terms of error rates, bias, power, and confidence interval coverage (Valente & MacKinnon, 2017). To judge the magnitude of the mediating effect, PM—the ratio of the indirect effect to the total effect—was calculated (Preacher & Kelley, 2011; Wen & Fan, 2015).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Single mediator models: indirect effect of intervention on symptom distress through transdiagnostic processes. Note. Graphical depiction of the mediator models of (1) mindfulness, (2) cognitive flexibility, (3) anxiety sensitivity, (4) experiential avoidance, and (5) behavioral activation as mediators between intervention and symptom distress. Mindfulness and cognitive flexibility were measured twice, after the module that targeted the mechanism (module/week 3 and module/week 5 respectively; (t1) and at post-treatment (t2). Anxiety sensitivity, experiential avoidance, and behavioral activation were solely assessed after the module that targeted them (module/week 7 or module/week 9). Symptom distress was measured at post-treatment. Mediators and outcome were controlled by their respective baseline value. These paths were omitted from the figure for clarity. CIs represent upper and lower limit of 95% Bootstrap-corrected confidence intervals. * p < .05

Fig. 4
figure 4

Multiple mediator model. Note. Graphical depiction of the multiple mediation model including mindfulness, cognitive flexibility, experiential avoidance, and behavioral activation as mediators between intervention and symptom distress. Mindfulness and cognitive flexibility were measured twice, after the module that targeted the mechanism (module/week 3 and module/week 5 respectively; t1) and at post-treatment (t2). Experiential avoidance and behavioral activation were solely assessed after module/week 9. Symptom distress was measured at post-treatment. Mediators and outcome were controlled by their respective baseline value and mediators were allowed to covary. These paths were omitted from the figure for clarity. Indirect effect 1 and 2 refer to the indirect effects through t1 and t2. CIs represent upper and lower limit of 95% Bootstrap-corrected confidence intervals. *p < .05

Results

Demographics

Demographic variables are displayed in Table 1. Groups did not differ at baseline.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of participants at baseline

Transdiagnostic Processes over Time

Means, Hedges’ g effect sizes, as well as percentages of missing values at each measurement point are reported in Table 2. We found significant changes over time in comparison to waitlist for all transdiagnostic processes: mindfulness, cognitive flexibility, anxiety sensitivity, behavioral activation, and experiential avoidance (F = 5.30–16.40, df = 1–2, p < 0.05). The transdiagnostic processes changed in the hypothesized direction: mindfulness, cognitive flexibility, and behavioral activation increased (g = 0.73–0.99), while anxiety sensitivity and experiential avoidance decreased (g = 0.4–0.48). Between group effect sizes were large for behavioral activation and cognitive flexibility (g = 0.86—0.88), and medium for mindfulness, anxiety sensitivity, and experiential avoidance (g = 0.59–0.76).

Table 2 Means, standard deviations/errors, percentage of missing values and effect sizes

Transdiagnostic Processes as Mediators

Unstandardized and standardized parameter estimates as well as fit indices of the full models can be found in the supplementary material.

Single Mediator Models

Single mediator models with transdiagnostic processes as the mediator between group and outcome are depicted in Fig. 3. For mindfulness and cognitive flexibility, two measurement points were available for the comparison between treatment and waitlist and the indirect effects through both time points were significant, as shown in Fig. 3. For mindfulness, the indirect effect through the first time point was 0.93, 90% CI [0.18, 2.19] and for the second − 2.57, 90% CI [− 4.86, -0.67]. For cognitive flexibility, the indirect effect through the first time point was − 0.59, 90% CI [− 1.71, 0.33] and for the second − 1.06, 90% CI [− 2.74, − 0.06]. The indirect effect through anxiety sensitivity was not significant, while the indirect path through experiential avoidance and behavioral activation were. The treatment effect, thus, was partially mediated by mindfulness, cognitive flexibility, experiential avoidance, and behavioral activation, but not anxiety sensitivity. The effect size of the mediating effect was PM = .13 for mindfulness (indirect effect through t1 and t2), PM = .06 for cognitive flexibility (indirect effect through t1 and t2), PM = .09 for experiential avoidance, and PM = .15 for behavioral avoidance.

Multiple Mediator Models

Figure 4 depicts the multiple mediator model that includes all significant mediators from the single mediator models (all except anxiety sensitivity). In the combined multiple mediator model, the bootstrapped 95% confidence interval of the indirect path through mindfulness and cognitive flexibility (through both time points) did not contain zero: The treatment effect was partially mediated by mindfulness and cognitive flexibility, but not by the other putative processes. The indirect effect through mindfulness showed an effect size of PM = .09 and through cognitive flexibility PM = .06. The total indirect effect (all indirect effects combined) was − 4.58, corresponding to an effect size of PM = .48.

Discussion

This study investigated transdiagnostic processes in an internet-based intervention based on the Unified Protocol for anxiety, depressive, and somatic symptom disorders. All transdiagnostic processes were malleable by treatment with medium to large effect sizes. In single mediator models, we found that cognitive flexibility, mindfulness, experiential avoidance, and behavioral activation partially mediated the relationship between treatment and symptom distress. When all significant mediators were collapsed into one model, the multiple mediation model revealed the indirect paths through mindfulness and cognitive flexibility as significant.

Results from the single mediator models are in line with previous research on the UP that found mindfulness, cognitive flexibility, and experiential avoidance predictive and associated with symptom change (Boswell et al., 2014; Brake et al., 2016; Eustis et al., 2019). Anxiety sensitivity — while increasingly conceptualized as a transdiagnostic process and found to be associated with symptom change in the UP (Boettcher et al., 2016; Boswell et al., 2013) — might play a lesser role in depressive disorders and thus, may not be related to outcome to the same extent in our fairly depressed sample. Future studies should investigate whether these findings on transdiagnostic processes are dependent on diagnosis, as suggested by a recent study on mindfulness as a predictor of outcome in the UP (Woods et al., 2020). The non-significant finding on anxiety sensitivity also raises the question of whether all participants benefit from the interoceptive exposure module that targets anxiety sensitivity. This highlights the potential of a flexible, modular application of the UP, where an individualized sequence of treatment components could be provided based on transdiagnostic processes (Fisher et al., 2019; Sauer-Zavala et al., 2018).

To our knowledge, transdiagnostic processes have not been investigated jointly in one mediation model for the UP as of yet. Mindfulness and cognitive flexibility, as measured by the SMQ and the reappraisal subscale of the ERQ, might capture the overarching aim of the UP best with their focus on how individuals react to distressing thoughts and images (and by that probably also emotions). These findings also tap into the debate whether it is more beneficial to decrease maladaptive or increase adaptive emotion regulation strategies in treatment (e.g., Southward et al., 2021). In our study, the adaptive emotion regulation processes — mindfulness, cognitive flexibility, and behavioral activation — seemed to show more stable indirect effects than experiential avoidance (and anxiety sensitivity).

The disentanglement of transdiagnostic processes seems quite complex, given they are all related, overlapping, and may share a common underlying basis (e.g., Mansell & McEvoy, 2017; Spinhoven et al., 2017). For instance, mindfulness training seems to facilitate exposure and decrease avoidance in some participants treated with the UP, highlighting the interplay and carryover effects between processes (Curreri et al., 2020). CBT treatments have also shown to be mediated by transdiagnostic processes without explicitly targeting them, for example by mindfulness (e.g., Goldin et al., 2016; Kocovski et al., 2015) or experiential avoidance (e.g., Eustis et al., 2016). Thus, based on our results, we cannot conclude that other treatment elements, besides mindfulness training and cognitive restructuring, are obsolete, since modules targeting avoidance may also enhance mindful emotion awareness and cognitive flexibility. These findings also challenge our theory-driven decision to measure mechanisms following the module that target them. Instead, additional research on the structure and hierarchy of transdiagnostic processes as well as dismantling component studies are needed to get a better understanding of the interplay of the processes in therapy and isolate effects.

Our findings need to be interpreted in the light of several limitations. First, the trial suffered from a high rate of missing values. Besides participants in the treatment group failing to fill out post-treatment assessments, dropout from the intervention affected the assessment of the transdiagnostic processes in the later modules. In order to ensure that participants received the proper dosage of treatment, we measured the transdiagnostic processes following the module that targeted them. But since the treatment was self-paced and the mean number of modules completed was M = 7.22 (Schaeuffele et al., 2020), this naturally meant that the assessments after completion of module 7 and 9 were limited to participants who progressed to this stage of treatment. Thus, our findings are preliminary and should be replicated with larger samples. Our sample size also did not permit us to analyze mediation in different subsamples, e.g. responders versus non-responders. However, we believe that examining mechanisms of change particularly in those patients experiencing change is a promising route of future research (DeRubeis et al., 2014). The dropout also limits the generalizability of our results. We have several hypotheses as to why participants might have not completed the intervention. The self-paced setup and “one size fits all” transdiagnostic approach of the intervention may have been overwhelming and lacked personalization for participants, especially in a guided self-help context. In addition, the lack of adherence-fostering features like personalized reminders and progress reinforcement, as they are implemented in other guided internet-based interventions, may have been detrimental to adherence. Future applications could address these concerns by delivering the intervention in a personalized, modular fashion and including adherence-fostering features. Another limitation concerns the lack of temporal precedence that has been described as an important prerequisite for causation (Kazdin, 2007). When comparing treatment and waitlist, temporal precedence could not be established with how measurements were set up. While several other of the quality criteria of mediation studies outlined by Lemmens et al. (2016) could be established (RCT with control group, n > 40 per group, assessment of several mediators), the lack of temporal precedence does not allow to draw any conclusions on causation. However, internet-based treatments are shorter in comparison to face-to-face and it seems to take time for the effect on transdiagnostic processes to unfold as the delayed changes in mindfulness and cognitive flexibility indicate. These circumstances complicate the establishment of temporality. Our results were obtained within an internet-based delivery of the UP. While the face-to-face and internet-based setting generally seem to share findings on relevant mechanisms of change (e.g., for depression: Domhardt et al., 2020; Lemmens et al., 2016), we cannot rule out that setting-specific influences affected our findings and they, thus, may not translate to the face-to-face setting to the full extent. Direct comparisons between face-to-face and internet-based deliveries and their accompanying change mechanisms are sparse and should be subject of future studies. Linear mediation models, as employed in our study, may not be suitable to capture the complexity of therapeutic processes, since the relationship between outcome and mediator are dynamic, bidirectional, and non-linear (Hofmann et al., 2020). More frequent and parallel assessments of mediators and outcomes are needed to understand the dynamic and agents of change in a more fine-grained manner. To facilitate this, shorter and change sensitive measures should be utilized.

Identifying the active ingredient of treatments and moving towards personalized treatments might benefit response rates. This also applies to our study where non-response rates of 31% (or 55% as a more conservative estimate) — while in line with meta-analytic findings on response rates in internet-based settings (Rozental et al., 2019) — may be improved by providing more effective and personalized treatments. How could this be achieved? Our findings suggest that (internet-based) applications of the UP should especially emphasize and increase a mindful stance towards emotions as well as regulating emotions by cognitive reappraisal in order to enhance effects. Future studies should substantiate our findings with larger sample sizes and more frequent and parallel assessments.