Skip to main content
Log in

When Are We More Ethical? A Review and Categorization of the Factors Influencing Dual-Process Ethical Decision-Making

  • Review Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The study of ethical decision-making has made significant advances, particularly with regard to the ways in which different types of processing are implicated. In recent decades, much of this advancement has been driven by the influence of dual-process theories of cognition. Unfortunately, the wealth of findings in this context can be confusing for management scholars and practitioners who desire to know how best to encourage ethical behavior. While some studies suggest that deliberate reflection leads to more ethical behavior, other studies find, in contrast, that intuitive decision-making leads to more ethical results. The goal of this integrative conceptual review is to help make sense of such apparently contradictory findings by identifying the moderating influences that lead to more versus less ethical decisions, whether they are made via intuitive or deliberative processes. Based on our integrative review of moderators from different disciplines and eras, we develop a taxonomy that can aid researchers in the task of identifying when similar constructs have been studied under different names. We organize our findings concerning these influences in accordance with four emergent moderator categories—psychological, situational, social, and physiological. This work helps us identify patterns of moderating factors across both intuitive and deliberative ethical decision-making, gaps that suggest future research directions and practical implications.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. While questions regarding contested issues in ethics remain crucial to the creation and maintenance of healthy societies, we believe that it is similarly essential to understand why unethical behavior persists even when the ethics of a situation are clear. We propose that scandals such as emissions issues at Volkswagen (Hotten 2015) or fraudulent account issues at Wells Fargo (Wattles et al. 2018) did not occur because the ethics of these situations were unclear but rather because such ethics were ignored. As a work of behavioral rather than normative ethics, these latter questions are the focus of this study.

  2. We assign the social consensus aspect of moral intensity to our social moderator category and describe its impact in the associated section of this review.

  3. We included this study because ego depletion was specifically tied to dual-process theory by suggesting that depleted individuals would not have the mental resources to engage in System 2 processing.

  4. Note that we generally categorized samples in studies using Amazon Mechanical Turk as U.S. samples when the relevant rewards were paid in U.S. dollars. In reality, these participants could have been located anywhere, and we have little insight regarding the steps that researchers may have taken to restrict their studies to participants in any particular country. A total of 21 such studies were included in the group of 107 studies that were attributed to the U.S.

  5. The European countries in our review studies were all located in Western Europe and specifically included Germany (12), The Netherlands (11), Sweden (2), The UK (2), Italy(1) and Norway(1), listed in order of frequency.

References

  • Aydinli, A., Bender, M., Chasiotis, A., Cemalcilar, Z., & van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2014). When does self-reported prosocial motivation predict helping? The moderating role of implicit prosocial motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 38(5), 645–658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 364–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Self-control—The moral muscle. Psychologist, 25(2), 112–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252–1265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bereby-Meyer, Y., Hayakawa, S., Shalvi, S., Corey, J. D., Costa, A., & Keysar, B. (2020). Honesty speaks a second language. Topics in Cognitive Science, 12(2), 632–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bereby-Meyer, Y., & Shalvi, S. (2015). Deliberate honesty. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 195–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialek, M., & Terbeck, S. (2016). Can cognitive psychological research on reasoning enhance the discussion around moral judgments? Cognitive Processing, 17(3), 329–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, B. M., Geniole, S. N., Procyshyn, T. L., Ortiz, T. L., Carré, J. M., & Watson, N. V. (2019). Effect of exogenous testosterone on cooperation depends on personality and time pressure. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(3), 538–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cappelen, A. W., Sørensen, E. Ø., & Tungodden, B. (2013). When do we lie? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 93, 258–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, R. W., Aknin, L. B., & Liotti, M. (2016). When is giving an impulse? An ERP investigation of intuitive prosocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(7), 1121–1129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, F., & Fischbacher, U. (2020). Cognitive processes underlying distributional preferences: A response time study. Experimental Economics, 23(2), 421–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, F., & Krajbich, I. (2018). Biased sequential sampling underlies the effects of time pressure and delay in social decision making. Nature Communications, 9(1), 3557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, N.-W., Chang, H.-T., & Huang, H.-L. (2015). Can personality traits and daily positive mood buffer the harmful effects of daily negative mood on task performance and service sabotage? A self-control perspective. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 131, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cojuharenco, I., Marques, T., & Patient, D. (2017). Tell me who, and i’ll tell you how fair: A model of agent bias in justice reasoning. Group & Organization Management, 42(5), 630–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cushman, F. (2013). Action, outcome, and value: A dual-system framework for morality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17(3), 273–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Dreu, C. K. W. (2003). Time pressure and closing of the mind in negotiation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91(2), 280–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Dreu, C. K. W., & Boles, T. L. (1998). Share and share alike or winner take all?: The influence of social value orientation upon choice and recall of negotiation heuristics. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76(3), 253–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Dreu, C. K. W., Dussel, D. B., & Ten Velden, F. S. (2015). In intergroup conflict, self-sacrifice is stronger among pro-social individuals, and parochial altruism emerges especially among cognitively taxed individuals. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derfler-Rozin, R., Moore, C., & Staats, B. R. (2016). Reducing organizational rule breaking through task variety: How task design supports deliberative thinking. Organization Science, 27(6), 1361–1379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive—Experiential and analytical–rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 390–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, A. M., Dillon, K. D., & Rand, D. G. (2015). Fast but not intuitive, slow but not reflective: Decision conflict drives reaction times in social dilemmas. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(5), 951–966.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (1984). Heuristic and analytic processes in reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 451–468. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1984.tb01915.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 255–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J., & Stanovice, K. E. (2013). Dual-Process theories of higher cognition. Pespectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223–241. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Everett, J. A. C., Ingbretsen, Z., Cushman, F., & Cikara, M. (2017). Deliberation erodes cooperative behavior—Even towards competitive out-groups, even when using a control condition, and even when eliminating selection bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 73, 76–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, E., Maltby, J., Bibby, P. A., & Lawrence, C. (2014). Fast to forgive, slow to retaliate: Intuitive responses in the ultimatum game depend on the degree of unfairness. PLoS ONE, 9(5), e96344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Filippin, A., & Guala, F. (2017). Group identity as a social heuristic: An experiment with reaction times. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 10(4), 153–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fong, S. S., Navarrete, C. D., Perfecto, S. E., Carr, A. R., Jimenez, E. E., & Mendez, M. F. (2017). Behavioral and autonomic reactivity to moral dilemmas in frontotemporal dementia versus Alzheimer’s disease. Social Neuroscience, 12(4), 409–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärtner, M. (2018). The prosociality of intuitive decisions depends on the status quo. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 74, 127–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gęsiarz, F., & Crockett, M. J. (2015). Goal-directed, habitual and Pavlovian prosocial behavior. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 135–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gore, J., & Sadler-Smith, E. (2011). Unpacking intuition: A process and outcome framework. Review of General Psychology, 15(4), 304–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D. (2007). Why are VMPFC patients more utilitarian? A dual-process theory of moral judgment explains. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 322–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D. (2017). The rat-a-gorical imperative: Moral intuition and the limits of affective learning. Cognition, 167, 66–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron, 44(2), 389–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293(5537), 2105–2108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grizzard, M., Shaw, A. Z., Dolan, E. A., Anderson, K. B., Hahn, L., & Prabhu, S. (2017). Does repeated exposure to popular media strengthen moral intuitions?: Exploratory evidence regarding consistent and conflicted moral content. Media Psychology, 20(4), 557–583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossmann, I., Brienza, J. P., & Bobocel, D. R. (2017). Wise deliberation sustains cooperation. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(3), 0061.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunia, B. C., Wang, L., Huang, L., Wang, J., & Murnighan, J. K. (2018). Contemplation and conversation: Subtle influences on moral decision making. Academy of Management Journal, 55(1), 13–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316(5827), 998–1002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, A., Young, A., Hughson, L., Green, D., Doan, S. N., Hughson, E., & Reed, C. L. (2020). Perceived relative social status and cognitive load influence acceptance of unfair offers in the Ultimatum Game. PLoS ONE, 15(1), 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haruno, M., Kimura, M., & Frith, C. D. (2014). Activity in the nucleus accumbens and amygdala underlies individual differences in prosocial and individualistic economic choices. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(8), 1861–1870.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most People are not WEIRD: To understand human psychology, behavioral scientists must stop doing most of their experiments on Westerners. Nature, 466(7302), 29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogarth, R. M. (2010). Intuition: A challenge for psychological research on decision making. Psychological Inquiry, 21(4), 338–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hotten, R. (2015). Volkswagen: The scandal explained. BBC News. http://www.bbc.com/news/business-34324772. Accessed 1 May 2017.

  • Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. Academy of Management Review, 16(2), 366–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. J., & Rand, D. G. (2020). Signaling when no one is watching: A reputation heuristics account of outrage and punishment in one-shot anonymous interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118(1), 57–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2003). Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. The American Economic Review, 93(5), 1449–1475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, J., & Sadler-Smith, E. (2019). Paradoxes and dual processes: A review and synthesis. International Journal of Management Reviews, 21(2), 162–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kern, M. C., & Chugh, D. (2009). Bounded ethicality: The perils of loss framing. Psychological Science, 20(3), 378–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kieslich, P. J., & Hilbig, B. E. (2014). Cognitive conflict in social dilemmas: An analysis of response dynamics. Judgment and Decision Making, 9(6), 510–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiyonari, T., Tanida, S., & Yamagishi, T. (2000). Social exchange and reciprocity: Confusion or a heuristic? Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(6), 411–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Köbis, N. C., Verschuere, B., Bereby-Meyer, Y., Rand, D., & Shalvi, S. (2019). Intuitive honesty versus dishonesty: Meta-analytic evidence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(5), 778–796.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive developmental approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 347–480). Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krosch, A. R., Tyler, T. R., & Amodio, D. M. (2017). Race and recession: Effects of economic scarcity on racial discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(6), 892–909.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kvarven, A., Strømland, E., Wollbrant, C.E.-P., Andersson, D., Johannesson, M., Tinghög, G., Västfjäll, D., & Myrseth, K. O. R. (2020). The intuitive cooperation hypothesis revisited: A meta-analytic examination of effect-size and between-study heterogeneity. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 6(1), 26–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lapsley, D. K., & Hill, P. L. (2008). On dual processing and heuristic approaches to moral cognition. Journal of Moral Education, 37(3), 313–332. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240802227486

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leavitt, K., Zhu, L., & Aquino, K. (2016). Good without knowing it: Subtle contextual cues can activate moral identity and reshape moral intuition. Journal of Business Ethics, 137(4), 785–800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindström, B., Jangard, S., Selbing, I., & Olsson, A. (2018). The role of a “common is moral” heuristic in the stability and change of moral norms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(2), 228–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ma, Y., Liu, Y., Rand, D. G., Heatherton, T. F., & Han, S. (2015). Opposing oxytocin effects on intergroup cooperative behavior in intuitive and reflective minds. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(10), 2379–2387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mischkowski, D., Glöckner, A., & Lewisch, P. (2018). From spontaneous cooperation to spontaneous punishment—Distinguishing the underlying motives driving spontaneous behavior in first and second order public good games. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 149, 59–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C. (2015). Moral disengagement. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 199–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C., & Gino, F. (2015). Approach, ability, aftermath: A psychological process framework of unethical behavior at work. Academy of Management Annals, 9(1), 235–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moussaïd, M., & Trauernicht, M. (2016). Patterns of cooperation during collective emergencies in the help-or-escape social dilemma. Scientific Reports, 6, 33417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mrkva, K. (2017). Giving, fast and slow: Reflection increases costly (but not uncostly) charitable giving. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(5), 1052–1065.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nishi, A., Christakis, N. A., & Rand, D. G. (2017). Cooperation, decision time, and culture: Online experiments with American and Indian participants. PLoS ONE, 12(2), e0171252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Reilly, J., Aquino, K., & Skarlicki, D. (2016). The lives of others: Third parties’ responses to others’ injustice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101(2), 171–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paharia, N., Vohs, K. D., & Deshpandé, R. (2013). Sweatshop labor is wrong unless the shoes are cute: Cognition can both help and hurt moral motivated reasoning. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 121(1), 81–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, J. H., & Schaller, M. (2005). Does attitude similarity serve as a heuristic cue for kinship? Evidence of an implicit cognitive association. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(2), 158–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peysakhovich, A., & Rand, D. G. (2016). Habits of virtue: Creating norms of cooperation and defection in the laboratory. Management Science, 62(3), 631–647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfattheicher, S., Keller, J., & Knezevic, G. (2017). Sadism, the intuitive system, and antisocial punishment in the public goods game. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(3), 337–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rand, D. G. (2016). Cooperation, fast and slow: Meta-analytic evidence for a theory of social heuristics and self-interested deliberation. Psychological Science, 27(9), 1192–1206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rand, D. G. (2017). Social dilemma cooperation (unlike dictator game giving) is intuitive for men as well as women. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 73, 164–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rand, D. G., Brescoll, V. L., Everett, J. A. C., Capraro, V., & Barcelo, H. (2016). Social heuristics and social roles: Intuition favors altruism for women but not for men. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(4), 389–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rand, D. G., Greene, J. D., & Nowak, M. A. (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature, 489(7416), 427–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rand, D. G., & Kraft-Todd, G. T. (2014). Reflection does not undermine self-interested prosociality. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rand, D. G., Newman, G. E., & Wurzbacher, O. M. (2015). Social context and the dynamics of cooperative choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 28(2), 159–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rand, D. G., Peysakhovich, A., Kraft-Todd, G. T., Newman, G. E., Wurzbacher, O., Nowak, M. A., & Greene, J. D. (2014). Social heuristics shape intuitive cooperation. Nature Communications, 5(1), 3677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawn, C. D., & Vohs, K. D. (2011). People use self-control to risk personal harm: An intra-interpersonal dilemma. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(3), 267–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and theory. Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J. (2006). A neurocognitive model of the ethical decision-making process: Implications for study and practice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(4), 737–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J., Leavitt, K., & DeCelles, K. A. (2010). Automatic ethics: The effects of implicit assumptions and contextual cues on moral behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(4), 752–760.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Righetti, F., Finkenauer, C., & Finkel, E. J. (2013). Low self-control promotes the willingness to sacrifice in close relationships. Psychological Science, 24(8), 1533–1540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, J. A., & Wasieleski, D. M. (2012). Moral reasoning in computer-based task environments: Exploring the interplay between cognitive and technological factors on individuals’ propensity to break rules. Journal of Business Ethics, 110(3), 355–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roch, S. G., Lane, J. A. S., Samuelson, C. D., Allison, S. T., & Dent, J. L. (2000). Cognitive load and the equality heuristic: A two-stage model of resource overconsumption in small groups. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 83(2), 185–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowley, D. A., Rogish, M., Alexander, T., & Riggs, K. J. (2018). Counter-intuitive moral judgement following traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neuropsychology, 12(2), 200–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruedy, N. E., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2010). In the moment: The effect of mindfulness on ethical decision making. Journal of Business Ethics, 95(S1), 73–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samuelson, C. D., & Allison, S. T. (1994). Cognitive factors affecting the use of social decision heuristics in resource-sharing tasks. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 58(1), 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. S. (2016). Ethical decision-making theory: An integrated approach. Journal of Business Ethics, 139(4), 755–776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shalvi, S., Eldar, O., & Bereby-Meyer, Y. (2012). Honesty requires time (and lack of justifications). Psychological Science, 23(10), 1264–1270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shi, R., Qi, W., Ding, Y., Liu, C., & Shen, W. (2020). Under what circumstances is helping an impulse? Emergency and prosocial traits affect intuitive prosocial behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 159, 109828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1987). Making management decisions: The role of intuition and emotion. Academy of Management Perspectives, 1(1), 57–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(2), 108–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sonenshein, S. (2007). The role of construction, intuition, and justification in responding to ethical issues at work: The sensemaking-intuition model. Academy of Management Review, 32(4), 1022–1040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soutschek, A., Weinreich, A., & Schubert, T. (2018). Facial electromyography reveals dissociable affective responses in social and non-social cooperation. Motivation and Emotion, 42(1), 118–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stead, W. E., Worrell, D. L., Spalding, J. B., & Stead, J. G. (1987). Unethical decisions: The impact of reinforcement contingencies and managerial philosophies. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 2(1), 105–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strombach, T., Margittai, Z., Gorczyca, B., & Kalenscher, T. (2016). Gender-specific effects of cognitive load on social discounting. PLoS ONE, 11(10), e0165289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sundar, A., & Kellaris, J. J. (2017). How logo colors influence shoppers’ judgments of retailer ethicality: The mediating role of perceived eco-friendliness. Journal of Business Ethics, 146(3), 685–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ten Velden, F. S., Daughters, K., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2017). Oxytocin promotes intuitive rather than deliberated cooperation with the in-group. Hormones and Behavior, 92, 164–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Chugh, D. (2015). Behavioral ethics: A story of increased breadth and depth. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 205–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Smith-Crowe, K. (2008). Ethical decision making: Where we’ve been and where we’re going. Academy of Management Annals, 2(1), 545–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teper, R., Zhong, C.-B., & Inzlicht, M. (2015). How emotions shape moral behavior: Some answers (and questions) for the field of moral psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(1), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinghög, G., Andersson, D., Bonn, C., Johannesson, M., Kirchler, M., Koppel, L., & Västfjäll, D. (2016). Intuition and moral decision-making—The effect of time pressure and cognitive load on moral judgment and altruistic behavior. PLoS ONE, 11(10), e0164012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2014). Assessing miserly information processing: An expansion of the cognitive reflection test. Thinking & Reasoning, 20(2), 147–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevino, L. K. (1986). Ethical decision making in organizations: A person-situation interactionist model. Academy of Management Review, 11(3), 601–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2008). The duality of virtue: Deconstructing the moral hypocrite. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(5), 1334–1338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Bavel, J. J., FeldmanHall, O., & Mende-Siedlecki, P. (2015). The neuroscience of moral cognition: From dual processes to dynamic systems. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 167–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Cruyssen, I., D’Hondt, J., Meijer, E., & Verschuere, B. (2020). Does honesty require time? Two preregistered direct replications of experiment 2 of Shalvi, Eldar, and Bereby-Meyer (2012). Psychological Science, 31(4), 460–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vieira, J. B., Almeida, P. R., Ferreira-Santos, F., Barbosa, F., Marques-Teixeira, J., & Marsh, A. A. (2014). Distinct neural activation patterns underlie economic decisions in high and low psychopathy scorers. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(8), 1099–1107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Y., Highhouse, S., Lake, C. J., Petersen, N. L., & Rada, T. B. (2017). Meta-analytic investigations of the relation between intuition and analysis. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(1), 15–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, S. J., & King, L. A. (2018). Individual differences in reliance on intuition predict harsher moral judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(5), 825–849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wattles, J., Geier, B., Egan, M., & Wiener-bronner, D. (2018). Wells Fargo’s 20-month nightmare. CNN Business.

  • Weaver, G. R., Reynolds, S. J., & Brown, M. E. (2014). Moral Intuition: Connecting current knowledge to future organizational research and practice. Journal of Management, 40(1), 100–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weller, J. A., Moholy, M., Bossard, E., & Levin, I. P. (2015). Preadolescent decision-making competence predicts interpersonal strengths and difficulties: A 2-year prospective study. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 28(1), 76–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiteside, D. B., & Barclay, L. J. (2018). When wanting to be fair Is not enough: The effects of depletion and self-appraisal gaps on fair behavior. Journal of Management, 44(8), 3311–3335. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206316672531

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winterich, K. P., Morales, A. C., & Mittal, V. (2015). Disgusted or happy, it is not so bad: Emotional mini-max in unethical judgments. Journal of Business Ethics, 130(2), 343–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yam, K. C. (2018). The effects of thought suppression on ethical decision making: Mental rebound versus ego depletion. Journal of Business Ethics, 147(1), 65–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yam, K. C., Chen, X.-P., & Reynolds, S. J. (2014a). Ego depletion and its paradoxical effects on ethical decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 124(2), 204–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yam, K. C., Reynolds, S. J., & Hirsh, J. B. (2014b). The hungry thief: Physiological deprivation and its effects on unethical behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 125(2), 123–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamagishi, T., Matsumoto, Y., Kiyonari, T., Takagishi, H., Li, Y., Kanai, R., & Sakagami, M. (2017). Response time in economic games reflects different types of decision conflict for prosocial and proself individuals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(24), 6394–6399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamagishi, T., Takagishi, H., Fermin, A. D. S. R., Kanai, R., Li, Y., & Matsumoto, Y. (2016). Cortical thickness of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex predicts strategic choices in economic games. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(20), 5582–5587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamagishi, T., Terai, S., Kiyonari, T., Mifune, N., & Kanazawa, S. (2007). The social exchange heuristic: Managing errors in social exchange. Rationality and Society, 19(3), 259–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, Z. J. (2016). Altruism during ebola: Risk perception, issue salience, cultural cognition, and information processing. Risk Analysis, 36(6), 1079–1089.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yetmar, S. A., & Eastman, K. K. (2000). Tax practitioners’ ethical sensitivity: A model and empirical examination. Journal of Business Ethics, 26(4), 271–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yudkin, D. A., Rothmund, T., Twardawski, M., Thalla, N., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2016). Reflexive intergroup bias in third-party punishment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(11), 1448–1459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhong, C.-B. (2011). The ethical dangers of deliberative decision making. Administrative Science Quarterly, 56(1), 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Support through the ANR Labex IAST is gratefully acknowledged. The authors would like to thank Laurie Barclay, Rellie Derfler-Rozin, Mario Gollwitzer, Gijs van Houwelingen, Caroline Manville, Sean Martin and Kristin Smith-Crowe for reviews of earlier drafts of the manuscript. We are also grateful to the three anonymous reviewers who provided valuable feedback on our manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Clark H. Warner.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Human and animal participations

This work did not involve human or animal participants; hence, no informed consent was necessary.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix A: Search Criteria

Clarivate: Web of Science Search String

You searched for: ((TS = (("Automatic Processing" OR "System 1" OR "Controlled Processing" OR "Dual Cognition" OR "Intuition" OR Dual-System OR "Deliberate Processing" OR Heuristic OR "Conation" OR "Hot Cognition" OR "Cold Cognition" OR EGO-Depletion OR "Cognitive Load") AND (Ethic OR "Moral Behavior" OR Stealing OR Cheating OR Lying OR Justice OR "Cooperation" OR "Selfish Behavior" OR Deviance OR Altruistic OR "Moral Reasoning" OR Honesty OR "Ethical Behavior "OR "Ethical Decision-Making" OR "Unethical Decision-Making" OR Pro-Social OR Prosocial OR Justice OR "Fairness" OR "Ethical" OR Moral)) AND WC = (PSYCHOLOGY DEVELOPMENTAL OR COMPUTER SCIENCE ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE OR ETHICS OR PSYCHOLOGY MULTIDISCIPLINARY OR PSYCHOLOGY OR MATHEMATICS INTERDISCIPLINARY APPLICATIONS OR SOCIAL SCIENCES INTERDISCIPLINARY OR PSYCHOLOGY BIOLOGICAL OR MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES OR SOCIAL ISSUES OR PSYCHOLOGY SOCIAL OR MEDICAL ETHICS OR PSYCHOLOGY EXPERIMENTAL OR MANAGEMENT OR INFORMATION SCIENCE LIBRARY SCIENCE OR OPERATIONS RESEARCH MANAGEMENT SCIENCE OR COMPUTER SCIENCE INTERDISCIPLINARY APPLICATIONS OR BUSINESS OR BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES OR ECONOMICS OR SOCIAL SCIENCES MATHEMATICAL METHODS OR SOCIOLOGY OR EDUCATION EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH OR CRIMINOLOGY PENOLOGY OR PHILOSOPHY OR PSYCHOLOGY CLINICAL OR SOCIAL SCIENCES BIOMEDICAL OR PSYCHIATRY OR PSYCHOLOGY APPLIED))) AND LANGUAGE: (English) AND DOCUMENT TYPES: (Article OR Book Chapter OR Data Paper OR Early Access OR Meeting Abstract OR Proceedings Paper OR Review).

Timespan: 1980–2018. Indexes: SCI-EXPANDED, SSCI, CPCI-S, CPCI-SSH, ESCI.

After the initial search above, the search was conducted weekly to capture articles beyond the initial time period.

Proquest Search String

all(("Automatic Processing" OR "System 1" OR "Controlled Processing" OR "Dual Cognition" OR Intuition OR Dual-System OR "Deliberate Processing" OR Heuristic OR Conation OR "Hot Cognition" OR "Cold Cognition" OR Induction OR EGO-Depletion OR "Cognitive Load") AND (Ethics OR "Moral Behavior" OR Stealing OR Cheating OR Lying OR Justice OR Cooperation OR "Selfish Behavior" OR Deviance OR Altruistic OR "Moral Reasoning" OR Honesty OR "Ethical Behavior " OR "Ethical Decision-Making" OR "Unethical Decision-Making" OR Pro-Social OR presocial OR Justice OR Fairness OR Ethical OR Moral)) AND cc("25??" OR "26??").

This search was conducted in the Business Premium Collection.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Warner, C.H., Fortin, M. & Melkonian, T. When Are We More Ethical? A Review and Categorization of the Factors Influencing Dual-Process Ethical Decision-Making. J Bus Ethics 189, 843–882 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-022-05281-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-022-05281-0

Keywords

Navigation