Skip to main content
Log in

Tax Fairness: Conceptual Foundations and Empirical Measurement

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Prior research shows taxpayers’ perceptions of fairness leads to greater cooperation and compliance with tax authorities. Yet our understanding of tax fairness has been hampered by its general reliance upon models and measures of fairness developed by organizational fairness research, even though fairness is a perception subject to contextual influences. Accordingly, we attempt to gain insight into the influence of contextual factors on fairness through the development of a theoretically based and empirically derived model of tax fairness, grounded in organizational literature, but empirically tested in the tax context. Our analysis shows the extent to which items and dimensions found in the organizational fairness literature reflect taxpayers’ perceptions of tax fairness. Most importantly, our findings contribute to fairness research by identifying the role of relevant comparison groups in affecting perceptions of fairness, and thereby extends our understanding of the role of context in affecting fairness perceptions. Furthermore, by identifying the dimensions and items incorporated in perceptions of tax fairness, our study provides a foundation for tax researchers and policy makers to determine how best to promote voluntary taxpayers’ compliance and in so doing, mitigate tax avoidance and tax evasion.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Throughout the manuscript, we specifically refer to “tax compliance” to encompass the range of outcomes, from positive to negative, that can be associated with tax fairness (i.e., tax compliance) and a lack of tax fairness (i.e., tax avoidance and tax evasion).

  2. Fairness and justice are terms that often are used interchangeably (Colquitt 2005, p. 4). In this paper, we use the terms interchangeably, and use the term fairness to include fairness perceptions.

  3. For example, some researchers consider tax fairness to be a singular overarching perception of general fairness (Etzioni 1986; Mason and Calvin 1978; Spicer and Lundstedt 1976), while others consider it to have multiple facets. Further, in the case of the latter, there does not appear to be agreement about the facets that apply. For instance, Christensen et al. (1994), Gerbing (1988), Maroney et al. (2002), Trivedi et al. (2003), and Hartner-Tiefenthaler et al. (2012) all consider general and distributive fairness while Eichfelder and Kegels (2014) as well as Van Dijke and Verboon (2010) consider procedural and informational fairness.

  4. More specifically in the tax context, exchange equity reflects the extent to which taxpayers’ tax dollars are effectively used by the government.

  5. “Preferred tax rate structure” encompasses two concepts—namely, that taxpayers with similar incomes should be taxed at the same rate while taxpayers with higher incomes should be taxed at higher rates than those at lower incomes because of their greater ability to pay.

  6. The organizational fairness papers included Avery and Quinones (2004), Colquitt (2001), De Boer et al. (2002), De Cremer and Tyler (2007), Fondacaro et al. (2002), Magner and Johnson (1995), and Moorman (1991).

  7. The IRS is the Internal Revenue Service, the United States’ tax authority.

  8. 44 of 50 states were represented in the sample.

  9. Our use of 7-point scales is consistent with prior research (see Diefenbach et al. 1993).

  10. The Tax Policy Center (2015) reports that 17.3% of tax units (i.e., households) pay no taxes because they have no income/earnings. Given the reported strong positive relationship between education and income, it is likely that individuals who are not well-educated make up the majority of this reported 17.3% of households.

  11. We also asked subjects to answer two yes/no questions for which it is not possible to gauge representativeness with the U.S. population: (1) Have you ever contacted the IRS? and (2) Has the IRS ever contacted you? Of the sample, 43.7% (41.1%) answered “yes” to the first (second) question.

  12. Our use of EFA is predicated on three assumptions. First, EFA is appropriate, in cases like ours, where researchers wish to identify a set of latent constructs that underlie a set of measured variables (Fabrigar et al. 1999). Second, our sample size of 501 is sufficiently large to conduct EFA (Fabrigar et al. 1999; Field 2009). And, third, our data pass all validity metrics for use of EFA. For instance, we check for reliability of the items that comprise the initial fairness factors (Fabrigar et al. 1999) and find that all have Cronbach alphas more than 0.7, which indicates acceptable reliability (Nunnally 1978). Further, the KMO (Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin) statistic is 0.94, which indicates that patterns of correlations are compact, and the factor analysis should yield distinct and reliable factors (Field 2009). Finally, Bartlett’s test of sphericity is significant (p < 0.001), indicating that the factors are correlated, and therefore factor analysis is appropriately conducted (Field 2009).

  13. Hair et al. (1998) recommend a scree test to determine the number of factors (i.e., plotting the eigenvalues against the number of possible factors, and examining the shape of the resulting curve). The point at which the curve begins to flatten indicates the maximum number of factors to extract. Our data show a drastic flattening after the fourth data point, which also suggests a three-factor solution (Costello and Osborne 2005).

  14. Given these results, the exploratory factor analysis has implicitly culled 16 of the original 30 items we started with, as these 16 items did not load on the three primary factors. Of the 16 items, five relate to general fairness, three relate to horizontal equity, one was a negatively worded item about exchange equity, and the remaining seven were originally used to capture something related to procedural, interpersonal, and/or informational fairness.

  15. Oblique rotation allows for the possibility of correlated factors and is appropriate when the goal of the factor analysis is to obtain several theoretically meaningful factors (Conway and Huffcutt 2003). Factor loadings of 0.4 are considered important, and factor loadings of 0.5 or higher are practically significant. Most importantly however, in a sample size of at least 250, a factor loading must be at least 0.35 to have a statistical significance at the 0.05 level (Hair et al. 1998).

  16. According to Byrne (2009) GFI, AGFI, and CFI should be at least 0.90, while RMSEA should not exceed 0.08.

  17. As sensitivity analysis, we examined the correlations between the dimensions and demographic items. For any significant correlation with any dimension and any demographic item (age, work experience, gender, tax preparer, and self-employment income), we checked to see if the underlying factor structure changed. The factor structure was identical, regardless whether taxpayers had no self-employment income or some self-employment income; whether they were male or female; and whether they prepared their own tax return or whether someone else did. As well, we conducted a similar analysis using a median split for both age and work experience, and found that the factor structure was identical.

  18. We measure intentional non-compliance, which is tax evasion. In particular, the focus of this research is on intentional non-compliance where taxpayers have opportunities to be non-compliant with minimal risk of detection (i.e., the underground economy).

  19. It may also explain the lack of taxpayers’ consideration of “off shore taxation policies” in their perceptions of tax fairness, as this group fails to be a relevant comparison group for individual taxpayers. Future research is needed to investigate this possibility.

  20. Bolded statements are items included in Table 2; [bracketed] designations at the end of the bolded statements correspond to the labels in Table 2 and Fig. 1.

References

  • Adams, J. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Cherry, T., Jones, M., & McKee, M. (2010). Taxpayer information assistance services and tax compliance behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(4), 577–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Kirchler, E., & Muehlbacher, S. (2012). Combining psychology and economics in the analysis of compliance: From enforcement to cooperation. Economic Analysis & Policy, 42(2), 133–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Avery, D., & Quinones, M. (2004). Individual differences and the voice effect. Group & Organization Management, 29(1), 106–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernardi, R., Bean, D., & Massey, D. (2004). The influence of political ideology on DIT scores: Fact or artifact? Research on Professional Responsibility and Ethics in Accounting, 9, 21–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernasconi, M., Corazzini, L., & Seri, R. (2014). Reference dependent preferences, hedonic adaptation and tax evasion: Does the tax burden matter? Journal of Economic Psychology, 40(1), 103–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bies, R., & Moag, J. (1986). Interactional justice: communication criteria of fairness. In R. Lewicki, B. Sheppard & M. Bazerman (Eds.), Research on negotiations in organizations (Vol. 1, pp. 43–55). Greenwich: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanthorne, C., & Kaplan, S. (2008). An egocentric model of the relations among the opportunity to underreport, social norms, ethical beliefs, and underreporting behavior. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 33(7–8), 684–703.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanthorne, C., & Roberts, M. (2015). Cognitive responses to partitioned pricing of consumption taxes: Consequences for state and local tax revenues. The Journal of the American Taxation Association, 37(1), 183–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobek, D., Hageman, A., & Kehhlier, C. (2013). Analyzing the role of social norms in tax compliance behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 115(3), 451–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobek, D., Hageman, A., & Kelliher, C. (2011). The role of social norms in tax compliance: Scale development, social desirability, and presentation effects. Advances in Accounting Behavioral Research, 15, 37–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braithwaite, V. (2003). A new approach to tax compliance. In V. Braithwaite (Ed.), Taxing democracy: Understanding tax avoidance and evasion (pp. 1–11). Aldershot: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B. (2009). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming (2nd edn.). New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chemerinsky, E. (1998). Substantive due process. Touro Law Review., 15, 1501–1534.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, A., Weihrich, S., & Newman, M. D. G. (1994). The impact of education on perceptions of tax fairness. Advances in Taxation, 6, 63–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colquitt, J. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(3), 386–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colquitt, J., Greenberg, J., & Scott, B. (2005). Organizational justice: Where do we stand? In J. Greenberg & J. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice (pp. 589–619). Mahwah: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colquitt, J., & Shaw, J. (2005). How should organizational justice be measured? In J. Greenberg & J. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice (pp. 113–152). Mahwah: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, J., & Huffcutt, A. (2003). A review and evaluation of exploratory factor analysis practices in organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 6(2), 147–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costello, A., & Osborne, J. (2005). Best practices in exploratory factory analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 10(5), 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Boer, E., Bakker, A., Syroit, J., & Schaufeli, W. (2002). Unfairness at work as a predictor of absenteeism. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 23(2), 181–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Cremer, D., & Tyler, T. (2007). The effects of trust in authority and procedural fairness on cooperation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(3), 639–649.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, M. (1975). Equity, equality, and need: What determines which value will be used as the basis for distributive justice? Journal of Social Issues, 31, 137–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diefenbach, M., Weinstein, N., & O’Reilly, J. (1993). Scales for assessing perceptions of health hazard susceptibility. Health Education Research, 8, 181–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doherty, D., & Wolak, J. (2012). When do the ends justify the means? Evaluating procedural fairness. Political Behavior, 34(2), 301–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doyle, E., Gallery, K., & Coyle, M. (2009). Procedural justice principles and tax compliance in Ireland: A preliminary exploration in the context of reminder letters. Journal of Finance and Management in Public Services, 8(1), 49–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eichfelder, S., & Kegels, C. (2014). Compliance costs caused by agency action? Empirical evidence and implications for tax compliance. Journal of Economic Psychology, 40, 200–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Etzioni, A. (1986). Tax evasion and perceptions of tax fairness: A research note. Journal of Applied Behavioral Sciences, 22(2), 177–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabrigar, L., Wegener, D., MacCallum, R., & Strahan, E. (1999). Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 4(3), 272–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrar, J., Donnelly, M., & Dhaliwal, S. (2013). Procedural aspects of tax fairness: A content analysis of Canadian tax jurisprudence. Journal of Legal Tax Research, 11(2), 21–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS (3rd edn.). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (2001). Fairness theory: Justice as accountability. In J. Greenberg & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in organizational justice (pp. 1–55). Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fondacaro, M., Jackson, S., & Luescher, J. (2002). Toward the assessment of procedural and distributive justice in resolving family disputes. Social Justice Research, 15(4), 341–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garver, M., & Mentzer, J. (1999). Logistics research methods: Employing structural equation modeling to test for construct validity. Journal of Business Logistics, 20(1), 33–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerbing, M. D. (1988). An empirical study of taxpayer perceptions of fairness. Ph.D. thesis, The University of Texas at Austin.

  • Gilliland, S., & Steiner, D. (2001). Causes and consequences of applicant perceptions of unfairness. In R. Cropanzano (Ed.), Justice in the workplace: From theory to practice (Vol. 2, pp. 175–195). Mahwah: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J. (1993). The social side of fairness. Interpersonal and information classes of organizational justice. In R. Cropanzano (Ed.), Justice in the workplace: Approaching fairness in human resource management (pp. 79–103). Hillsdale: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hair, J., Tatham, R., Anderson, R., & Black, W. (1998). Multivariate data analysis (5th edn.). Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartner, M., Kirchler, E., Poschalko, A., & Rechberger, S. (2010). Taxpayers’ compliance by procedural and interactional fairness perceptions and social identity. Journal of Psychology & Economics, 3(1), 12–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartner, M., Rechberger, S., Kirchler, E., & Schabmann, A. (2008). Procedural Fairness and Tax Compliance. Economic Analysis & Policy, 38(1), 137–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartner-Tiefenthaler, M., Kubicek, B., Kirchler, E., Rechberger, S., & Wenzel, M. (2012). Perceived distributive fairness of European transfer payments and EU-taxes in Austria, the Czech Republic, and the United Kingdom. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 61(3), 454–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasseldine, J., Kaplan, S., & Fuller, L. (1994). Characteristics of New Zealand tax evaders: A note. Accounting and Finance, 34(2), 79–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodge, S. (2012). Who are America’s Millionaires? Tax Foundation. June 15. Available at: https://taxfoundation.org/who-are-americas-millionaires/ (last accessed: 09/12/17).

  • Howden, L., & Meyer, J. (2011). Age and sex composition: 2010. 2010 Census Briefs. May: U.S. Census Bureau. Retrieved from https://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-03.pdf. Accessed Sep 24, 2017.

  • Jacoby, J. (1978). Consumer research: A state of the art review. Journal of Marketing, 42(2), 87–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judge, T., & Colquitt, J. (2004). Organizational justice and stress: The mediating role of work/family conflict. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(3), 395–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, A., & Dean, P. N. (1980). Moral evaluations of tax evasion. Social Policy and Administration, 14(3), 209–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerlinger, F. (1986). Foundations of behavioral research (2nd edn.). Fort Worth: Holt, Rinehard and Winston, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E., Niemirowski, A., & Wearing, A. (2006). Shared subjective views, intent to cooperate and tax compliance: Similarities between Australian taxpayers and tax officers. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(4), 502–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knopff, R., & Morton, F. L. (1992). Charter politics. Toronto: Nelson Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konow, J. (2001). Fair and square: The four sides of distributive justice. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 46(2), 137–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kwok, W., & Sharp, D. (1998). A review of construct measurement issues in behavioral accounting research. Journal of Accounting Literature, 17, 137–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leventhal, G. (1980). What should be done with equity theory? New approaches to the study of fairness in social relationships. In K. Gergen, M. Greenberg & R. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in theory and research (pp. 27–55). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lind, E. (2001). Fairness heuristic theory: Justice judgments as pivotal cognitions in organizational relations. In I. Greenberg & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in organizational justice (pp. 56–88). Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magner, N., & Johnson, G. (1995). Municipal officials’ reactions to justice in budgetary resource allocation. Public Administration Quarterly, 18(4), 439–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maroney, J., Rupert, T., & Wartick, M. (2002). The perceived fairness of taxing social security benefits: The effect of explanations based on different dimensions of tax equity. The Journal of the American Taxation Association, 24(2), 79–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, R., & Calvin, L. D. (1978). A study of admitted tax evasion. Law & Society Review, 13(1), 73–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moorman, R. (1991). Relationship between organizational justice and organizational citizenship behaviors: Do fairness perceptions influence employee citizenship? Journal of Applied Psychology, 76(6), 845–855.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K. (2003). Procedural justice and tax compliance. Australian Journal of Social Issues, 38(3), 379–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K. (2004). The role of trust in nurturing compliance: A study of accused tax avoiders. Law and Human Behavior, 28(2), 187–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K. (2005). Regulating more effectively: The relationship between procedural justice, legitimacy and tax non-compliance. Journal of Law and Society, 32(4), 562–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K. (2009). Procedural justice and affect intensity: Understanding reactions to regulatory authorities. Social Justice Research, 22(1), 1–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K., & Tyler, T. (2008). Procedural justice and compliance behavior: The mediating role of emotions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(4), 652–668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niemirowski, P., Baldwin, S., & Wearing, A. J. (2003). Tax related behaviors, beliefs, attitudes and values and taxpayer compliance in Australia. Journal of Australian Taxation, 6(1), 132–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pew Research Center. (2014). Distribution of the ideological consistency scale. Political Polarization in the American Public. Retrieved from http://www.people-press.org/2014/06/12/appendix-a-the-ideological-consistency-scale/pp-2014-06-12-polarization-a1-02. Accessed Sep 24, 2017.

  • Proctor, B., Semega, J., & Kollar, M. (2016). Income and Poverty in the United States: 2015. U.S. Census Bureau. September, Report Number: P60-256. Retrieved from https://www.census.gov/content/dam/Census/library/publications/2016/demo/p60-256.pdf. Accessed Sep 24, 2017.

  • Richardson, G. (2005a). A preliminary study of the impact of tax fairness perception dimensions on tax compliance behavior in Australia. Australian Tax Forum, 20, 407–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, G. (2005b). An exploratory cross-cultural study of tax fairness perceptions and tax compliance behavior in Australia and Hong Kong. The International Tax Journal, 31(1), 11–24 & 65–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, G. (2006). The impact of tax fairness dimensions on tax compliance behavior in an Asian jurisdiction: the case of Hong Kong. International Tax Journal, 32(1), 29–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saad, N. (2009). Fairness perceptions and compliance behavior: The case of salaried taxpayers in Malaysia after implementation of the self-assessment system. eJournal of Tax Research, 8(1), 32–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saad, N. (2011). Fairness perceptions and compliance behavior: The New Zealand evidence. New Zealand Journal of Taxation Law & Policy, 17(1), 33–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweitzer, M., & Gibson, D. (2008). Fairness, feelings, and ethical decision-making: Consequences of violating community standards of fairness. Journal of Business Ethics, 77(3), 287–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spicer, M., & Lundstedt, S. (1976). Understanding tax evasion. Public Finance, 31(2), 295–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweeney, B., Arnold, D., & Pierce, B. (2010). The impact of perceived ethical culture of the firm and demographic variables on auditors’ ethical evaluation and intention to act decisions. Journal of Business Ethics, 93(4), 531–551.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tax Policy Center. (2015). T15-0138tax units with zero or negative income tax. Tax Policy Center, Preliminary Results 2-Oct-15. Retrieved from http://www.taxpolicycenter.org/model-estimates/tax-units-zero-or-negative-income-tax/tax-units-zero-or-negative-income-tax. Accessed Dec 09, 2017.

  • Thibault, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treviño, L. (1992). The social effects of punishment: A justice perspective. Academy of Management Review, 17(4), 647–676.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivedi, V., Shehata, M., & Lynn, B. (2003). Impact of personal and situational factors on taxpayer compliance: An experimental analysis. Journal of Business Ethics, 47(3), 175–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T., & Lind, E. (1992). A relational model of authority in groups. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 115–191). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Census Bureau. (2017). Educational attainment—People 18 years old and over, by total money earnings, work experience, age, race, Hispanic origin, and sex. Current Population Survey Tables. U. S. Census Bureau. Report PINC-04. Available at: https://www.census.gov/data/tables/time-series/demo/income-poverty/cps-pinc/pinc-04.html. Accessed Sep 24, 2017.

  • Valentine, S., & Fleischman, G. (2003). Ethical reasoning in an equitable relief innocent spouse context. Journal of Business Ethics, 45(4), 325–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valentine, S., & Rittenberg, T. (2004). Spanish and American business professionals’ ethical evaluations in global situations. Journal of Business Ethics, 51(1), 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K., Peters, S., Bobocel, D., & Ybema, J. (2006). On preferences and doing the right thing: Satisfaction with advantageous inequity when cognitive processing is limited. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(3), 273–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijke, M., & Verboon, P. (2010). Trust in authorities as a boundary condition to procedural fairness: Effects on tax compliance. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(1), 80–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verboon, P., & Goslinga, S. (2009). The role of fairness in tax compliance. Netherlands Journal of Psychology, 65, 136–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verboon, P., & Van Dijke, M. (2007). A self-interest analysis of justice and taxpayers’ compliance: How distributive justice moderates the effect of outcome favorability. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28(6), 704–727.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallschutzky, I. G. (1984). Possible causes of tax evasion. Journal of Economic Psychology, 5(4), 371–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wärneryd, K. E., & Walerud, B. (1982). Taxes and economic behavior: Some interview data on tax evasion in Sweden. Journal of Economic Psychology, 2(3), 187–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M. (2002). The impact of outcome orientation and justice concerns on tax compliance: The role of taxpayers’ identity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(4), 629–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M. (2006). A letter from the tax office: Compliance effects of informational and interpersonal justice. Social Justice Research, 19(3), 345–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M., & Taylor, N. (2004). An experimental evaluation of tax-reporting schedules: A case of evidence-based tax administration. Journal of Public Economics, 88(12), 2785–2799.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors gratefully acknowledge financial support from the Dolan School of Business at Fairfield University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dawn W. Massey.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in this study, which involved human participants, were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committees of the authors’ institutions.

Appendix: Survey Instrument

Appendix: Survey Instrument

The following survey asks for your opinion about the fairness of income taxes paid by individual taxpayers and Internal Revenue Service (IRS) practices. Your opinion is important to developing a better understanding of the fairness of the United States’ tax system. Please remember your responses, which should not take more than about 15–20 min, will be kept entirely confidential.

Part I

Please read the following statements and indicate your level of agreement by clicking on the appropriate response, where 1 = strongly agree, and 7 = strongly disagree.Footnote 20

  1. 1.

    It is fair that taxpayers earning the same income pay a similar share of income tax.

  2. 2.

    It is fair for taxpayers with similar amounts of income to pay a similar amount of income tax.

  3. 3.

    Two taxpayers who earn the same income should pay the same amount of tax.

  4. 4.

    It is fair that high-income earners are subject to a higher tax rate than middle-income earners. [VE1]

  5. 5.

    It is fair that middle-income earners are taxed at a lower rate than high-income earners. [VE2]

  6. 6.

    High-income earners should pay a greater share of income tax than low-income earners. [VE3]

  7. 7.

    When compared to the taxes paid, taxpayers receive too few public services.

  8. 8.

    The amount taxpayers pay in income tax is equal to the amount they receive in government services. [EE1]

  9. 9.

    Taxpayers receive social services equivalent to the income taxes they pay. [EE2]

  10. 10.

    The IRS offers taxpayers ways to express their grievances and complaints. [PF1].

  11. 11.

    At the IRS, taxpayers’ complaints are taken seriously. [PF2]

  12. 12.

    IRS procedures are based on accurate information provided by taxpayers

  13. 13.

    The IRS treats all taxpayers similarly.

  14. 14.

    The IRS processes all taxpayers’ returns the same way. [PF3]

  15. 15.

    The IRS correctly uses the information provided by taxpayers in calculating taxes owed.

  16. 16.

    The IRS communicates in a timely manner. [INF1]

  17. 17.

    IRS correspondence is concise.

  18. 18.

    IRS correspondence is easy to understand. [INF2].

  19. 19.

    The IRS answers taxpayers’ questions carefully. [INF3]

  20. 20.

    The IRS is candid in its communication.

  21. 21.

    The IRS expresses regret for possible negative effects on taxpayers. [INT1]

  22. 22.

    The IRS treats taxpayers with courtesy. [INT2]

  23. 23.

    The IRS refrains from improper comments. [INT3]

  24. 24.

    The IRS treats taxpayers politely.

  25. 25.

    IRS correspondence is polite.

  26. 26.

    The IRS tries to be fair.

  27. 27.

    Overall, the IRS is fair.

  28. 28.

    The IRS is fair in all aspects of its operations.

  29. 29.

    The US tax system is fair.

  30. 30.

    Overall, the tax system in the US is fair.

[Compliance Questions]

  1. 1.

    Taxpayers will omit income from their tax returns if the party paying them does not report the income to the IRS.

  2. 2.

    Taxpayers will omit income from their tax returns if they receive it in cash.

  3. 3.

    Taxpayers will overstate their tax deductions if the amounts are paid in cash.

Part II

Please read the following statements and indicate your response.

figure a

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Farrar, J., Massey, D.W., Osecki, E. et al. Tax Fairness: Conceptual Foundations and Empirical Measurement. J Bus Ethics 162, 487–503 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-018-4001-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-018-4001-4

Keywords

Navigation