Introduction

Invasive non-native plants (INNP) are a significant driver, as well as a product of, global environmental change (Simberloff et al. 2013; Blackburn et al. 2014). INNP often pose major risks to the environment, ecosystem services and human well-being (Pejchar and Mooney 2009; Vilà et al. 2011; Jeschke et al. 2014). These risks are likely to be greatly exacerbated as overall anthropogenic pressures on the natural environment increase (Banks et al. 2014).

The risks posed by INNP are managed and mitigated by people. However, the details and extent of the perception of such risks are inherently highly variable between individuals (Slimak and Dietz 2006; Vanderhoeven et al. 2011; Gozlan et al. 2013), are largely species dependent (Sharp et al. 2011; Gozlan et al. 2013; Verbrugge et al. 2013), and do not always correlate with actual ecological risk (Andreu et al. 2009; Gozlan et al. 2013). People are generally poor at assessing risks (Wachinger et al. 2013), frequently exaggerating some whilst downplaying others (Clayton and Myers 2009). The processes that lead to development of perceptions of risk are complex (Slovic 1999). Whilst there is debate over the levels of rationality and subjectivity involved (Slovic 1999; Sjoberg 1999), certain drivers have recurrently been found to influence their development (Slimak and Dietz 2006). These include, for example, direct or indirect experience of a risk, proximity to the risk, and certain socio-demographic variables (e.g. age, education and gender; Kasperson et al. 1988; Flynn et al. 1994; Gustafson 1998; Slovic 1999; Carlton and Jacobson 2013; Wachinger et al. 2013).

How an individual perceives the risks of a specific INNP is central to determining their attitudes towards it, and subsequently their behaviour (Fischer and van der Wal 2007; Estévez et al. 2014). For example, divergent perceptions about the risks from INNP might result in conflict over management approaches, priorities, or even in opinions regarding whether they should be controlled at all (McDaniels et al. 1997; Estévez et al. 2014). In domestic gardens, where the management of INNP is largely the responsibility of the owner or tenant of a given garden (Qvenild et al. 2014), the consequences of variation in perceptions of risks of INNP are likely to result in spatial heterogeneity in how INNP are managed therein.

INNP in domestic gardens can pose serious ecological risks, both within the garden and, if they escape, in the wider environment (Groves et al. 2005; Sullivan et al. 2005). Furthermore, the risks posed by INNP in domestic gardens can result in large economic costs (McDermott et al. 2013), and can cause high levels of anxiety (Eskridge and Alderman 2010). Mismanagement of INNP in domestic gardens could increase the ecological and socio-economic impacts INNP have, encourage their spread (van Heezik et al. 2013), and be detrimental to the wellbeing, biodiversity and ecosystem services that gardens can provide. The wellbeing benefits gardens can deliver, such as providing a space for leisure and social activities (Bhatti and Church 2004), opportunities to connect with nature (Restall and Conrad 2015) and opportunities to gain ecological knowledge and skills (Barthel et al. 2010), will only become more important in an increasingly urbanised world (UN 2010). Similarly, the significant contribution domestic gardens make to urban ecosystem functioning and habitat connectivity in many westernised countries, due to the large proportion of urban land they cover, will also become increasingly important as urbanisation increases. For example, private gardens account for over 20% of land cover in some UK cities (Loram et al. 2007) and over 35% in New Zealand (Mathieu et al. 2007).

Research into INNP in domestic gardens is relatively scarce (Qvenild et al. 2014). This is especially true of studies considering the perceptions of INNP. Studies that have examined perceptions of INNP in domestic gardens have largely focused on their categorisation as native or non-native, and their perceived level of invasiveness (e.g. Zagorski et al. 2004; Qvenild et al. 2014), rather than the perception of risks that specific INNP pose.

In this paper, we employ a survey approach to determine the drivers of people’s perception of the risks from INNP in domestic gardens, and which risks concern them most. The variables selected for the survey as potentially influencing perception of risk of INNP were chosen a priori based on knowledge of how they influence perceptions of other environmental risks. The results of this analysis help to reveal whether and why people might develop over- or under- inflated perceptions of risk. Furthermore, identifying drivers of perception of risk can assist in informing the design and targeting of risk communication, education and awareness strategies to reduce the ecological and socio-economic impacts of INNP. We use Japanese knotweed Fallopia japonica in the UK as a case study, as it exemplifies many of the risks surrounding INNP in domestic gardens, as well as having a number of additional risks when present on domestic property (e.g. it might devalue property; van Ham et al. 2013). Data were collected in Cornwall, a county in the southwest of the UK.

Method

Japanese knotweed

Introduced as a desirable garden plant in c.1850 (Shaw et al. 2011), Japanese knotweed has since become widespread in much of the UK (Engler et al. 2011); it is prevalent in the study region, Cornwall (present in 38.6% (n = 1517) of 3932 1 × 1 km grid-cells covering the county; Cornwall Council pers. comm.; NBN 2015). The ecological traits of Japanese knotweed make it a particularly difficult INNP to control or eradicate. For example, it can regrow from a small fragment of rhizome (Colleran and Goodall 2014), it can grow fast (Beerling et al. 1994), and its roots extend far both vertically and horizontally (EA 2013). Ecological risks of Japanese knotweed include outcompeting native plants, directly for light and water and indirectly via alleopathy (Dommanget et al. 2014), and changing habitat structure for animals (Engler et al. 2011). On domestic property it can have a number of socio-economic risks. For example, it can cause undesirable changes to gardens by outcompeting other plants, it can have a negative aesthetic impact, and be costly to eradicate or control. If it is present in a garden, or even on nearby land, it might reduce the value of the property/land and might cause complications in obtaining a mortgage (RICS 2012; Taylor et al. 2013; van Ham et al. 2013). If it spreads from a garden to adjacent land it could result in legal proceedings by neighbours as it is considered a ‘private nuisance’ (Payne and Hoxley 2012). More recent legislation grants environmental agencies (e.g. Environment Agency) the authority to access the property to control it if required, and failure to comply could result in prosecution (Home Office 2014; House of Lords 2014).

Selection of variables potentially influencing perception of risk

We used drivers demonstrated as influencing perceptions of a broad range of environmental risks (e.g. flooding, earthquakes, volcanic eruptions and landslides) to inform those included in the survey that might influence the perception of risk of Japanese knotweed in domestic gardens (Table 1); not all of these variables consistently predict perceptions of risk. The perception of risk of Japanese knotweed in domestic gardens was measured as perceived (a) frequency of this plant and (b) severity of impacts (Kasperson et al. 1988).

Table 1 Summary of variables chosen a priori that might be influencing perception of risk of INNP on domestic property

Direct experience

Research suggests that direct experience of a risk will likely result in greater clarity, persistence and strength of perception of that risk compared with indirect experience (Whitmarsh 2008). Here we consider two types of direct experience:

  1. 1.

    Direct professional experience If participants’ have or had an occupation where they are more likely to encounter Japanese knotweed. In this study we define two possible categories of such professions: (1) working in the housing market sector, including as estate agents, solicitors, architects, building surveyors or mortgage advisors; and (2) work involving ecology, including as ecological consultants, working for a UK environmental/conservation organisation (e.g. Natural England or National Trust), or as an academic whose research involves ecology. All other occupations were grouped as ‘other’.

  2. 2.

    Direct domestic experience If participants have or have had Japanese knotweed in the garden of a property they have owned or rented, or on land they have managed.

Indirect experience

When people do not have direct experience of an event they base their perceptions of risk on information from secondary sources, for example friends, family or media (Kasperson et al. 1988). Mass media has been found to be the most common way of obtaining information on INNP (McNeely 2001). Theoretical and empirical research suggests that when people gain information about a risk from secondary sources, and combine it with perceptions of closely related risks, it can result in social amplification of that risk (Pidgeon et al. 2003). Resulting behavioral responses can have secondary social and economic consequences (Renn et al. 1992). Determining whether survey participants who only receive information about Japanese knotweed via the mass media have under- or over-inflated perceptions of risk would help understand if its media portrayal is contributing to social amplification of risk. We define mass media as TV, radio and newspapers.

Proximity to risk

If an individual is closer to a risk, either geographically, or in a way that increases their liability to the impacts, the consequences will likely appear greater; in our survey we considered both of these:

  1. 1.

    Geographically closer This was measured in terms of whether participants know of Japanese knotweed within 5 km of home, either in a garden or on other land.

  2. 2.

    Increased liability One way in which proximity to the liability of certain risks can increase, and that has been proven in some studies to influence perception of risk, is by owning rather than renting property (Burningham et al. 2008; Wachinger et al. 2013). The assumption is that if someone owns property they might be more concerned about certain environmental risks as they are usually responsible for resulting economic costs.

Socio-demographics

We selected three socio-demographic variables that are easily and accurately measurable.

  1. 1.

    Gender the socio-demographic variable perhaps most commonly examined as a driver in perception of risk is gender (Slovic 1999), with multiple studies finding that women generally perceive risks as more problematic than do men (Flynn et al. 1994; Gustafson, 1998; Karanci et al. 2005; Barberi et al. 2008; Miceli et al. 2008; Armaş and Avram 2009; Kellens et al. 2011).

  2. 2.

    Level of education Education is also frequently found to be significant in explaining perceptions of risks (Karanci et al. 2005; Barberi et al. 2008), with those with lower levels of qualifications usually having greater perception of risk (Armaş and Avram 2009).

  3. 3.

    Age Many studies explore the influence of age, often finding that older people have a higher perception of risk (Kellens et al. 2011), however, this is usually a weaker relationship than with other socio-demographic variables (e.g. Karanci et al. 2005; Lindell and Hwang 2008; Miceli et al. 2008).

Sampling regime

To reduce the biases associated with each in isolation, survey responses were gathered by three methods between July 2014 and February 2015. First, passers-by in Truro city centre, one of the largest urban areas in Cornwall, were asked to participate. Participants were selected at random and those who did not have time to complete the survey were given a flyer promoting the online version. Second, a press release was issued advertising the online version of the survey, in which INNP were not mentioned to avoid creating a bias in participants. Third, participants identified as likely to come across Japanese knotweed in their occupation through online searches (e.g. estate agents) and through email distribution lists, were emailed the link to the online survey. All participants were Cornwall residents.

Survey design

The survey was designed following guidance from Bernard (2011). All questions analysed here were closed, response options to which were randomised where possible. The survey was piloted several times to refine wording and order of questions.

There were three sections in the survey (see Appendix S1 in Supplementary Information for full list of questions). The first asked about perception of risk of Japanese knotweed, split into two questions addressing (1) perception of frequency, and (2) perception of severity of impacts. To put this into context, questions were also asked about perception of risk of other potential concerns on domestic property: ivy (Hedera helix), large trees close to the property, gulls, bats, subsidence, damp, flooding, dry rot, mundic (deterioration of concrete structures due to inappropriate materials used), and radon (a natural gas which can have elevated levels inside some buildings and has associated health concerns). These potential concerns were derived from semi-structured interviews with estate agents (see Appendix S2 for details).

The second section focused solely on Japanese knotweed. Participants were asked if they had heard of this INNP. If they answered no, they moved straight to the final section. Questions were asked to determine if participants had had Japanese knotweed on a property they owned or rented, or on land they managed (direct domestic experience). Then two questions were asked to explore perceptions of particular risks (these were compiled based on results from analysis of internet discourse on the subject and semi-structured interviews with housing market professionals, see S2 for details). The questions were (a) ‘what is your perception of the threat posed by the following issues associated with Japanese knotweed in domestic gardens?’ And (b) ‘what would be your primary motivation for taking action to control Japanese knotweed if present in the garden where you currently live?’.

The third section collected background data, including socio-demographics (age, gender, level of education), and asked questions that allowed us to identify whether participants worked in an occupation where they regularly came across Japanese knotweed (direct professional experience).

The sample comprised a marginally lower percentage of women than in the region (49.2 and 51.6% respectively; ONS 2011; Table S1). It comprised similar percentages to the region in all age categories: 18–29 age category was 18.5 and 20.7% respectively, 30–39 age category was 18.8 and 16.9% respectively, 40–49 age category was 19.5 and 18.6% respectively, 50–59 age category was 18.8 and 15.4% respectively, and the 60+ age category was 24.3 and 28.5% respectively (ONS 2011). The percentage of participants with the top level of education (first degree or above) was higher than for the region (52.9% and 26% respectively; ONS 2011). This was skewed by the targeting of participants with professional experience in the housing market and in ecology. A similar percentage of the sample owned property compared with the region (67.2% and 69.6% respectively; ONS 2011).

Analysis

We first determined which drivers predicted perception of risk of Japanese knotweed in domestic gardens. Two models were constructed (using R 3.1.3; R Core Team 2015) to evaluate the responses to the following questions (1) ‘how frequently do you think the following occur on domestic properties in Cornwall?’, (2) ‘if the following were identified on a property, how severe do you think the consequences could be?’. For each question participants could choose from five levels of response or respond ‘no idea/never heard of’. Responses of the last option were excluded from analyses. Explanatory variables included in the maximal models were direct professional experience of Japanese knotweed (three-level fixed factor), direct domestic experience of Japanese knotweed (two-level fixed factor), indirect experience of Japanese knotweed (two-level fixed factor), increased perceived geographical proximity to risk (two-level fixed factor), increased proximity to liability of risk, (two-level fixed factor), age category (five-level fixed factor), education (four-level fixed factor) and gender (two-level fixed factor; Table 1). As the response variable was categorical we used cumulative link models using the ‘clm’ function in the ‘ordinal’ package (Christensen 2014). Cumulative link models, also known as ordered regression models or proportional odds models, employ a modified Newton–Raphson algorithm to provide accurate maximum likelihood estimates of the parameters (Christensen 2014). To verify whether model results were not due to differences in occupation, models with only participants whose occupation did not involve Japanese knotweed were also constructed.

Following a multi-model inference approach (Grueber et al. 2011) we used the ‘MuMin’ package (Barton 2011) to determine the final averaged model and to evaluate the relative importance of each parameter. We used the natural averaging method (Burnham and Anderson 2002). All models where ΔAIC < 2 were used to produce the averaged model (Burnham and Anderson 2001).

We were particularly interested in how direct professional experience of Japanese knotweed influenced perception of risk, as it has been found to be a significant driver explaining perception of INNP more generally (Selge et al. 2011; Gozlan et al. 2013). To address this, we explored whether sub-categories differed in (1) perception of risk relative to other risks on domestic property, and (2) which specific risks concern them most. Averages, standard errors and rankings were calculated for (a) each potential risk on domestic property (again, ‘no idea/never heard of’ responses excluded) and for (b) participants’ perception of the threat to particular risks from Japanese knotweed (‘no idea’ responses excluded). The number of participants within each sub-category of direct professional experience (other, housing market and ecology) who listed a particular risk as their primary motivation for taking action to control Japanese knotweed were summed and ranks were calculated.

Results

In total 329 surveys were completed (144 in person, 185 online).

Drivers of perception of risk

Participants perceived Japanese knotweed to be less frequent on domestic property in Cornwall if their occupation involved the housing market (estimate = −1.707 ± 0.331 (SE), p < 0.001), if they did not have domestic experience of Japanese knotweed (estimate = 0.907 ± 0.327 (SE), p = 0.006), if they did not know of Japanese knotweed within 5 km of their home (estimate = 0.686 ± 0.228 (SE), p = 0.003), or if they had the top level of education (1st degree or above; estimate = −0.973 ± 0.327 (SE), p = 0.003; Table 2; see Table S2 for global models and S3 for top models). Education remained significant in the model that only carried out analysis of participants whose profession was ‘other’ (see Table S4 for details).

Table 2 Top candidate models and model averaged parameter estimates from the top models of cumulative linkmodels exploring the effect of every combination of explanatory factors for (a) how frequently people thought Japanese knotweed occurred on domestic property in Cornwall and (b) how severe people thought the consequences of having Japanese knotweed on domestic property in Cornwall could be

Participants who thought that the consequences of Japanese knotweed being present on domestic property could be more severe had occupations that involved the housing market (estimate = 0.759 ± 0.318 (SE), p = 0.017), knew of Japanese knotweed within 5 km of their home (estimate = 1.198 ± 0.235 (SE), p < 0.001), or were older (‘30–39’ estimate = 0.739 ± 0.361 (SE), p = 0.041;’40–49’ estimate = 0.760 ± 0.361; p = 0.036; ‘50–59’ estimate = 0.800 ± 0.375, p = 0.034; ‘60+’ estimate = 1.076 ± 0.39 (SE), p = 0.002; Table 2; see Appendix S2 for global models and S3 for top models). If participants owned property was also significant in the model, but only marginally (estimate = 0.502 ± 0.252 (SE), p = 0.048). The third level of education (‘further education or vocational training’) was marginally significant in the global model, however, it was not significant in the averaged model or when a model was constructed using only participants whose occupation was ‘other’ (see Table S4 for details).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Participants’ responses to a how frequently people thought certain issues occurred on domestic property in Cornwall and b how severe people thought the consequences of having these issues on domestic property in Cornwall could be. Response ‘no idea/never heard of’ excluded. Numbers represent the rank

Participants whose occupation was ‘other’ ranked their perception of how frequently Japanese knotweed occurs on domestic property in Cornwall as highest (6th) in relation to the other potential concerns on domestic property, followed by participants whose occupation involved the housing market (6th), and participants whose occupation involved ecology ranked it lower (11th; Fig. 1a). Both participants whose occupation involved the housing market and ecology ranked the potential severity of Japanese knotweed on domestic property higher (4th) in relation to other potential threats, whereas participants whose occupation was ‘other’ ranked Japanese knotweed lower (7th; Fig. 1b).

Risks of greatest concern to participants

The most common primary motivation given to control Japanese knotweed in domestic gardens by participants whose occupation was ‘other’, or involved ecology, was the potential for it to spread to adjacent land, whereas this ranked second for participants whose occupation involved the housing market (Table 3). Participants whose occupation involved the housing market reported their primary motivation to be concern about damage to building structure, which was ranked second by participants whose occupation was ‘other’, and third by participants whose occupation involved ecology.

Table 3 Response to the question: ‘What would be your primary motivation for taking action to control Japanese knotweed if present in the garden where you currently live?’ (Participants could only select one answer)

Participants whose occupation was ‘other’, housing market and ecology all ranked their perception of the threat by Japanese knotweed spreading to adjacent property as the highest (Fig. 2). Perceptions about the level of threat from other potential risks of ‘devaluing property’ and damage to the structure of the property were ranked much lower by all participants.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Survey participants’ response to the question ‘What is your perception of the threat posed by the following issues associated with Japanese knotweed in domestic gardens?’ Response ‘no idea’ excluded. Numbers represent the rank

Discussion

This study asked the questions: what are the drivers of people’s perception of the risks from INNP in domestic gardens, and which risks concern them most? Japanese knotweed in the UK was used as a case study. We found large differences in perceived risk of Japanese knotweed depending on people’s profession, their direct domestic experience, their perceived geographical proximity to the risk, and socio-demographic differences. Here we consider explanations for these results and discuss the implications for garden management decisions, policy, risk communication, and awareness strategies.

Drivers of perception of risk

Direct professional experience was significant in predicting perception of the frequency of Japanese knotweed on domestic property, as well as perception of the potential severity of the consequences. Participants whose occupation involved the housing market perceived the frequency of Japanese knotweed on domestic property as lowest, but perceived the potential severity of the consequences as highest. Housing market professionals are likely to encounter Japanese knotweed on domestic properties if it is present, and therefore are likely to have more accurate knowledge of the frequency with which it occurs therein than other participant groups. This increased likelihood of observing the problems that Japanese knotweed can cause in domestic gardens, including observation of particularly acute impacts, might inflate their perception of severity of risk, or alternatively might make it more accurate. The perceptions of those whose occupation involved ecology aligned more closely with participants who had no professional experience of Japanese knotweed. This might be because this subset included participants from professions that would not necessarily involve Japanese knotweed, or require knowledge about its impacts or management.

Participants with direct domestic experience of Japanese knotweed or increased perceived geographical proximity to risk, measured as whether participants knew of Japanese knotweed within 5 km of their home, perceived its frequency to be higher than those without these attributes. An explanation for this might be because those in these participant groups are more likely to live in areas of locally high abundance of Japanese knotweed, and therefore base their perception of frequency on their local environment. Another study found that environmental managers made decisions based on local perception of abundance and impacts of INNP (Andreu et al. 2009). If those responsible for managing domestic gardens also base management decisions regarding INNP on local frequency it perhaps means management is more appropriate for local conditions.

Our finding that the relationship between direct domestic experience and perception of severity differed to the relationship with increased perceived geographical proximity to risk, might be because observing Japanese knotweed close to home might inflate perceptions of risk due to concerns about it spreading to a respondent’s property and not knowing the level of management required to control it or the reality of the severity of threat to personal property. The consequences of having direct domestic experience, however, might not be sufficiently problematic to inflate perception of risk severity. Increased research and communication about the impacts of Japanese knotweed and other INNP could help make risk perceptions more uniform.

Two socio-demographic factors were significant drivers of perception of risk. Education had a negative relationship with perception of frequency, in alignment with studies of perception of environmental risks (e.g. Armaş and Arvam 2009). Age had a positive relationship with severity of consequences, also in line with previous research exploring risk perception of environmental risks (e.g. Kellens et al. 2011). It is difficult to determine the causal mechanism underlying these relationships. Perhaps it is because with age one accumulates viewpoints about INNP, some of which may conflict and contribute to inflation of perception of risk. Alternatively, it might not be a function of age, but rather experiential and cultural differences between generations (Bremner and Park 2007). Other studies have found education and gender to influence perceptions towards INNP. For example, one study found that older people reported greater support for control and eradication of INNP in Scotland (Bremner and Park 2007). Another study found that older people, and those with higher levels of education were more supportive of higher levels of management intervention of INNP in parks (Sharp et al. 2011). Conversely, other research has found that in south-west Spain younger people were more aware of concerns surrounding INNP (García-Llorente et al. 2008). Ensuring communications about INNP are published in outlets accessible to a variety of age groups could help distribute knowledge more evenly.

Gender was the only socio-demographic driver that was not statistically significant in predicting perception of risk of either frequency or severity. Gender may perhaps be more of a driver in emotive decisions such as control/eradication of animals, for example, lethal deer management (Dougherty et al. 2003). A study in Scotland, UK, found that men were more likely to support eradication of invasive non-native species to support conservation goals and protect rare species; however, this study does not distinguish between support for eradication of invasive non-native plants or invasive non-native animals (Bremner and Park 2007).

Additionally, whether participants had heard about Japanese knotweed only via mass media was not significant in predicting perceived frequency and at most only had a minor effect on perception of severity (see Table S4). Several studies have found a limited ability of the mass media to influence perception of other risks (Freudenburg et al. 1996; Wåhlberg and Sjöberg 2000; Brenkert-Smith et al. 2013), as well as support for management options of invasive species (Sharp et al. 2011). Despite sensationalist headlines, pictures and loaded language, factual information is contained within some articles (Freudenburg et al. 1996), which may help objectively to assess the risk. Furthermore, perhaps many doubt the credibility of some media (Sjoberg 1999), so do not base their perception of risk on it. Alternatively, it may be that other complex social processes and interactions are also producing social amplification of risk. For example, information derived through social networks (e.g. friends and family) can have a strong influence on risk perceptions of many topics including climate change (Kahan et al. 2012). To ensure accurate and useful information on invasive plants is portrayed in the media it is important authorities on invasive plants (e.g. scientists and control companies) interact regularly with the media (Barua 2010) and provide clear factual information on this topic.

Risks of greatest concern to participants

Concern about spread to adjacent land was reported as the top motivation for controlling Japanese knotweed in domestic gardens by all participant groups except those whose occupation involved the housing market, who ranked it second. Similarly, perception of the threat from Japanese knotweed spreading was ranked top by all participant groups. This is perhaps an indication of the high level of concern regarding the uncontrollability of the plant, and the consequences of not only having to control it on your land. For example, in the worst-case scenario, spread to adjacent land could result in legal proceedings (Payne and Hoxley 2012). Increased communication about practical and easy ways to stop this plant spreading could help curtail this concern, for example highlighting the need for early identification and widespread ability to identify it (Simberloff et al. 2013; Robinson et al. 2016).

Damage to the structure of a property was rated as the second highest motivation to control Japanese knotweed in a domestic garden by participants who had no professional experience of Japanese knotweed, and third by those whose occupation involved ecology. Interestingly, however, both participant groups rated their perception of risk of this threat as relatively low. A number of drivers might have influenced this. First, perhaps people perceive this threat as one they can realistically mitigate, and therefore are perhaps more likely to take preventative action if required. Second, perhaps people perceive the consequences of this threat as high, which is a reason to act to prevent it, even if the likelihood of it occurring is low. Lastly, perhaps the scientific uncertainty of this risk manifests as conflicting information, which along with variation in interpretation and communication of this risk by different secondary sources, might subsequently influence how people perceive the risk (Pidgeon et al. 2003). In-depth interviews could provide insight into why people develop the perceptions of these risks.

Conclusions

When interpreting the results of this study it is important to acknowledge that simply because a hazard is perceived to be a risk, it does not necessarily follow that the details of the risk are understood (Clayton and Myers 2009), or that perceptions logically correlate with attitudes and behaviour. There are likely to be many other drivers, such as time and money availability, impacting these complex relationships (Wåhlberg and Sjöberg 2000; Wachinger et al. 2013). The extent to which perceptions of risk are based on values influences how difficult conflicts arising from different perceptions are to resolve (Estévez et al. 2014). Furthermore, the lack of scientific consensus about how to control Japanese knotweed, or if it should always be controlled (Delbart et al. 2012), is likely contributing to the large variation in perceptions of risk about this plant in domestic gardens, even amongst those who encounter it in a professional context. Further research is needed to determine the actual levels of risk that Japanese knotweed poses both in urban and rural areas by quantifying its abundance and impact.

As perceptions of risk are important in determining what, if any, action is taken to manage INNP on domestic property, the results of this paper have several important implications. The results highlight the need for discourses communicating the risks of INNP in domestic gardens to be clear and accurate. This could be achieved by clarifying terminology used and concepts discussed (Selge et al. 2011), by providing balanced discussion of the risks, impacts and solutions, and highlighting the role and responsibility those managing INNP in domestic gardens have. As media publications cannot be fully regulated, it is particularly important for government organisations carefully to consider risk communication strategies. Furthermore, the drivers of risk perception identified in this study could be used to target awareness campaigns to reduce over or under-inflated risk perceptions developing. Implementation of these recommendations could help reduce the ecological and socio-economic impacts of INNP in domestic gardens, as well as the wider environment. There is a need for policy that provides support and resources for people in managing INNP in their local area (Bardsley and Edwards-Jones 2007). Although this study focused on Japanese knotweed in Cornwall, INNP in domestic gardens is a global issue, therefore applying some of the solutions discussed in this study to other countries could help reduce impacts of INNP globally.