Skip to main content
Log in

The Impact of Health, Wealth, and Attractiveness on Romantic Evaluation from Photographs of Faces

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A large literature suggests that men and women differ in their self-reported mate preferences such that men place greater weight on physical attractiveness than women do, whereas women value financial prospects more than men. Yet, little research has addressed how these differences generalize to other contexts, such as modern online dating in which mate selection may largely depend on visual cues. Distinct from the sex differences observed in previous studies relying on self-reports, we found that men and women both used perceptions of health and attractiveness to select hypothetical partners based on photographs of their faces. Importantly, although people reliably identified others’ wealth from their photographs, these perceptions did not influence men’s or women’s partner selections. Thus, men and women may select romantic partners similarly based on limited visual information.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Although we instructed our research assistants to download 80 female faces, they downloaded 81 faces; thus, we included them all.

  2. Although we collected photographs of individuals only within this particular bracket, we did not record each target’s specific age when downloading the stimuli and were unable to retrieve this information post hoc. Notably, all effects and significance levels remained reliable when we considered only the targets’ peers (i.e., participants who reported their age to be 18–35 years old; n = 159).

  3. According to Eastwick et al.’s (2014) recent meta-analysis, the average correlation between participants’ judgments of targets’ physical attractiveness and their interest in them at initial attraction (resembling the zero-acquaintance context examined here) was r = .59. Similarly, the correlation between participants’ evaluations of targets’ earning prospects and their interest in them at initial attraction was r = .25. To guarantee sufficient power, we (conservatively) used the latter effect size in our power analysis.

  4. We additionally examined whether people indeed evaluated physical attractiveness and not attractiveness in general. To address this, we recruited an independent sample of 61 participants (n = 28 female), who provided their ratings of physical attractiveness for the opposite sex targets. We found that people’s attractiveness ratings in the main study strongly correlated with the physical attractiveness ratings provided by the participants from the independent sample: r(156) = .95, p < .001. In other words, people indeed evaluated physical attractiveness.

  5. Because standardized estimates may be misleading in the context of multilevel modeling, we report unstandardized coefficients accompanied by their standard errors (Hox, 2010).

  6. Because we were interested in how perceptions of health, wealth, and attractiveness relate to romantic interest, we did not include actual wealth in these models.

References

  • Anderson, C., John, O. P., Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (2001). Who attains social status? Effects of personality and physical attractiveness in social groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 116–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baize, H. R., & Schroeder, J. E. (1995). Personality and mate selection in personal ads: Evolutionary preferences in a public mate selection process. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 10, 517–536.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2005). Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.98-1.

  • Burrows, K. (2013). Age preferences in dating advertisements by homosexuals and heterosexuals: From sociobiological to sociological explanations. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 203–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (1988). The evolution of human intrasexual competition: tactics of mate attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 616–628.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, D., London, O., & Reeves, K. (1968). The effects of physical attractiveness, sex, and attitude similarity on interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality, 36, 259–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, M. R., Roberts, A. R., Barbee, A. P., Druen, P. B., & Wu, C. H. (1995). “ Their ideas of beauty are, on the whole, the same as ours”: Consistency and variability in the cross-cultural perception of female physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 261–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Sousa Campos, L., Otta, E., & de Oliveira Siqueira, J. (2002). Sex differences in mate selection strategies: Content analyses and responses to personal advertisements in Brazil. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 395–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Vries, J. M. (2010). Impact of self-descriptions and photographs on mediated dating interest. Marriage & Family Review, 46, 538–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Vries, J. M., Swenson, L., & Walsh, R. P. (2008). Hot picture or great self-description: Predicting mediated dating success with parental investment theory. Marriage & Family Review, 42(3), 7–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2008). Sex differences in mate preferences revisited: Do people know what they initially desire in a romantic partner? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 245–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eastwick, P. W., Luchies, L. B., Finkel, E. J., & Hunt, L. L. (2014). The predictive validity of ideal partner preferences: A review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 623–665.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elder, G. H. (1969). Appearance and education in marriage mobility. American Sociological Review, 34, 519–533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feingold, A. (1990). Gender differences in effects of physical attractiveness on romantic attraction: A comparison across five research paradigms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 981–993.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feingold, A. (1992). Gender differences in mate selection preferences: A test of the parental investment model. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 125–139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Finkel, E. J., Eastwick, P. W., Karney, B. R., Reis, H. T., & Sprecher, S. (2012). Online dating: A critical analysis from the perspective of psychological science. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 13, 3–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, G. J., Tither, J. M., O’Loughlin, C., Friesen, M., & Overall, N. (2004). Warm and homely or cold and beautiful? Sex differences in trading off traits in mate selection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 303, 659–672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goode, E. (1996). Gender and courtship entitlement: Responses to personal ads. Sex Roles, 34, 141–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greitemeyer, T. (2010). Effects of reciprocity on attraction: The role of a partner’s physical attractiveness. Personal Relationships, 17, 317–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ha, T., van den Berg, J. E., Engels, R. C., & Lichtwarck-Aschoff, A. (2012). Effects of attractiveness and status in dating desire in homosexual and heterosexual men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 673–682.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, A. A., & Saeed, L. (1977). Let’s make a deal: An analysis of revelations and stipulations in lonely hearts advertisements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 257–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, R. (1945). Campus values in mate-selection. Journal of Home Economics, 37, 554–558.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hitsch, G. J., Hortaçsu, A., & Ariely, D. (2010). What makes you click? Mate preferences in online dating. Quantitative Marketing and Economics, 8, 393–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hox, J. J. (2010). Applied multilevel analysis (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, P. C. (2014). Extension of Nakagawa & Schielzeth’s R2GLMM to random slopes models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 5, 944–946.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., Westfall, J., & Kenny, D. A. (2012). Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: A new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 54–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalick, S. M., Zebrowitz, L. A., Langlois, J. H., & Johnson, R. M. (1998). Does human facial attractiveness honestly advertise health? Longitudinal data on an evolutionary question. Psychological Science, 9, 8–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kocsor, F., Rezneki, R., Juhász, S., & Bereczkei, T. (2011). Preference for facial self-resemblance and attractiveness in human mate choice. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 1263–1270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kraus, M. W., & Keltner, D. (2009). Signs of socioeconomic status a thin-slicing approach. Psychological Science, 20, 99–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., & Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 390–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, L., Loewenstein, G., Ariely, D., Hong, J., & Young, J. (2008). If I’m not hot, are you hot or not? Physical-attractiveness evaluations and dating preferences as a function of one’s own attractiveness. Psychological Science, 19, 669–677.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Li, N. P., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Sex similarities and differences in preferences for short-term mates: What, whether and why. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 468–489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Li, N. P., Yong, J. C., Tov, W., Sng, O., Valentine, K. A., Jiang, Y. F., & Balliet, D. (2013). Mate preferences do predict attraction and choices in the early stages of mate selection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 757–776.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, S. L., & Maner, J. K. (2012). Overperceiving disease cues: The basic cognition of the behavioral immune system. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 1198–1213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montoya, R. M. (2008). I’m hot, so I’d say you’re not: The influence of objective physical attractiveness on mate selection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1315–1331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakagawa, S., & Schielzeth, H. (2013). A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 4, 133–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nezlek, J. B. (2007). Multilevel modeling in personality research. In R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley, & R. F. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 502–522). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pawlowski, B., & Koziel, S. (2002). The impact of traits offered in personal advertisements on response rates. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 139–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pillsworth, E. G., & Haselton, M. G. (2006). Male sexual attractiveness predicts ovulatory shifts in female extra-pair attraction and male mate retention. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 247–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology, 25, 111–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ronis, D. L., & Lipinski, E. R. (1985). Value and uncertainty as weighting factors in impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 47–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rule, N. O., Bjornsdottir, R. T., Tskhay, K. O., & Ambady, N. (2016). Subtle perceptions of male sexual orientation influence occupational opportunities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 1687–1704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Satterthwaite, F. E. (1946). An approximate distribution of estimates of variance components. Biometrics Bulletin, 2, 110–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shorrocks, A. F. (1975). The age-wealth relationship: A cross-section and cohort analysis. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 57, 155–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singh, D. (1995). Female judgment of male attractiveness and desirability for relationships: Role of waist-to-hip ratio and financial status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1089–1101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spielmann, S. S., MacDonald, G., Maxwell, J. A., Joel, S., Peragine, D., Muise, A., & Impett, E. A. (2013). Settling for less out of fear of being single. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 1049–1073.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sprecher, S. (1989). The importance to males and females of physical attractiveness, earning potential, and expressiveness in initial attraction. Sex Roles, 21, 591–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stroebe, W., Insko, C. A., Thompson, V. D., & Layton, B. D. (1971). Effects of physical attractiveness, attitude similarity, and sex on various aspects of interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 79–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Todd, P. M., Penke, L., Fasolo, B., & Lenton, A. P. (2007). Different cognitive processes underlie human mate choices and mate preferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 15011–15016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. M. (1993). Sexuality and partner selection: Sex differences among college students. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 305–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. M., & Levy, G. D. (1990a). Effects of potential partners’ costume and physical attractiveness on sexuality and partner selection. Journal of Psychology, 124, 371–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. M., & Levy, G. D. (1990b). Effects of potential partners’ physical attractiveness and socioeconomic status on sexuality and partner selection. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 149–164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. M., & Roberts, L. W. (1993). Gender differences in mate preference among law students: Divergence and convergence of criteria. Journal of Psychology, 127, 507–528.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. M., & Wasserman, T. (1998). Sexual attractiveness: Sex differences in assessment and criteria. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 171–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. G. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of Man, 1871–1971 (pp. 136–179). Chicago, IL: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tskhay, K. O., & Rule, N. O. (2014). Perceptions of personality in text-based media and OSN: A meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 49, 25–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Udry, J. R., & Eckland, B. K. (1984). Benefits of being attractive: Differential payoffs for men and women. Psychological Reports, 54, 47–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Census Bureau. (2014). Income and poverty in the United States: Current population reports. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/content/dam/Census/library/publications/2014/demo/p60-249.pdf on May 8, 2015.

  • Westfall, J., Kenny, D. A., & Judd, C. M. (2014). Statistical power and optimal design in experiments in which samples of participants respond to samples of stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 2020–2045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D., & Brumbaugh, C. C. (2009). Using revealed mate preferences to evaluate market force and differential preference explanations for mate selection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1226–1244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zebrowitz, L. A. (1997). Reading faces: Window to the soul? Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zebrowitz, L. A., & Collins, M. A. (1997). Accurate social perception at zero acquaintance: The affordances of a Gibsonian approach. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 204–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zebrowitz, L. A., & Montepare, J. M. (2005). Social psychological face perception: Why appearance matters. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zentner, M., & Mitura, K. (2012). Stepping out of the caveman’s shadow: Nations’ gender gap predicts degree of sex differentiation in mate preferences. Psychological Science, 23, 1176–1185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Rebecca Zhu and other members of the Social Perception and Cognition Lab for their help with data collection. The current work was supported by a grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada to Nicholas O. Rule.

Funding

This study was funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Grant.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Konstantin O. Tskhay.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Konstantin O. Tskhay declares that he has no conflict of interest. Jerri M. Clout declares that she has no conflict of interest. Nicholas O. Rule declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tskhay, K.O., Clout, J.M. & Rule, N.O. The Impact of Health, Wealth, and Attractiveness on Romantic Evaluation from Photographs of Faces. Arch Sex Behav 46, 2365–2376 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-017-0963-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-017-0963-z

Keywords

Navigation