Introduction

In most Western countries, alcohol use typically begins during adolescence, with many young people developing an early pattern of binge drinking, i.e., drinking more than five drinks on one occasion (Kuntsche and Labhart 2012; Kuntsche et al. 2004). Young people’s alcohol use tends to accelerate in the late teens, peak in the early 20s, and decrease in the late 20s (Andrade 2019). However, university students often maintain a high level of alcohol consumption up until their late 20s (Van Hal et al. 2018; Karam et al. 2007). While most students will not experience negative consequences for later life outcomes, students who drink heavily expose themselves to immediate risks, such as low academic performance, physical injuries, traffic accidents, and unwanted or unprotected sexual encounters (Andrade and Järvinen 2017; Porter and Pryor 2007; Hingson et al. 2002; Wechsler et al. 1994).

When asked in surveys and qualitative interviews about their alcohol consumption, university students tend to refer to a distinctive drinking culture that includes two motivational factors (Lannoy et al. 2017; Järvinen et al. 2018; Measham and Brain 2005; Kuntsche et al. 2005; Demers et al. 2002). The first motivational factor is that alcohol use helps dealing with stress, coping with personal problems, and relieving boredom (Mohr et al. 2005). The second motivational factor is that alcohol facilitates socialization. As most young people experience starting university as a major transition, alcohol plays a vital role in the transition as the organizing driver for new students to meet and bond with the other students (Utpala-Kumar and Deane 2012; Dempster 2011). Many university students find it difficult to go against the dominant drinking culture (Supski and Lindsay 2017). As drinking too much—or too little—can lead to low popularity among peers, the new students quickly learn the “right” way to drink alcohol (Fjær and Pedersen 2015; Østergaard 2009).

Research suggests that young people’s drinking cultures are formed by a multiplicity of actors, including peers, family members, university authorities, policymakers, and the alcohol industry (Savic et al. 2016; Room 1992). Governmental interventions, such as increasing taxes and reducing the availability of alcohol, have previously been shown to reduce the overall level of alcohol consumption (Anderson et al. 2009; Stockwell 2001). Nevertheless, experiences from past interventions aimed at changing the student drinking show that social norms are not easily changed and that most students view binge drinking as a cornerstone of university life (Kypri et al. 2018; Anderson and Baumberg 2006). Research reveals that social norms are among the best predictors of college student drinking (Neighbors et al. 2007; Fairlie et al. 2021; Lewis and Neighbors 2006). Thus to reduce students’ alcohol consumption, it is important not only to focus on how the individual consumes alcohol but also how the consumption pattern is embedded in a network of friendships with a similar drinking culture. Previous research suggests that social norms may negatively affect students’ participation and outcome in interventions targeting behavioral strategies (Larimer et al. 2007; Barnett et al. 2007).

Extensive literature shows that web-based alcohol interventions are an effective and cost-efficient tool to successfully change alcohol-related behavior among young people (Martinez-Montilla et al. 2020; Riper et al. 2010; Donoghue et al. 2014; Bannink et al. 2014; Schulz et al. 2013; Bewick et al. 2008). However, few web-based alcohol interventions combine randomized control trials with elements that target the young people’s drinking culture (Riper et al. 2010; Haug et al. 2012; Bendtsen et al. 2012; White et al. 2010; Cooke et al. 2016). A recent web-based UK intervention targeting first-year university students is a notable exception. On the basis of the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen 1988), the researchers combined text messages and “if–then” plans to significantly reduce the frequency of the first-year university students’ binge drinking and change their cognitions about binge drinking (Norman et al. 2018).

The aim of this study was to develop, implement, and evaluate a web-based intervention aimed at reducing alcohol consumption among university students. Compared to previous web-based interventions that tend to focus on first-year students only, this study included students at all levels at the university. The intervention was hereby targeting both bachelor and master students’ engagement with and attitudes toward the dominant drinking culture at the university. We implemented the intervention in Denmark, which constitutes a highly useful case for targeting student drinking. Together with their British counterparts, Danish university students are among the heaviest drinkers in Europe (Järvinen and Room 2007). As the level of alcohol consumption among young people in most of Europe has been converging since the early 2000s (Kraus et al. 2020), additional knowledge about the Danish youth is likely valuable to peer groups in other countries.

Compared to the web-based interventions in previous studies, this intervention was relatively short (answering the questionnaire takes no more than 15 min). Drawing on the theory of planned behavior, the intervention included three different elements focusing on pre-commitment strategies, social norms, and reminders. A central element in the development of the intervention was the collaboration with the local student organizations to adapt the intervention to the current social norms of campus. We hypothesized that the participating students would report improvements in self-reported measures about their alcohol consumption.

Method

Design

In this study, we conducted a stratified randomized control experiment among students from Aarhus University in Denmark. The participants were stratified by both gender and their answer to the question “how often have you been drinking five or more standard drinks at one event during the preceding month?” and randomly assigned to either a control or an intervention group. The trial was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Danish Centre for Social Science Research on September 16, 2019, and registered prior to the implementation on September 24, 2019, in the American Economic Association’s registry for randomized trials with RCT ID: AEARCTR-0004703. No significant changes were made between the start of the trial and the registration confirmation.

Participation

The participants were recruited by the student association at Aarhus University over 2 weeks between September 11 and September 25, 2019 (see Appendix 1 for a detailed description of the recruitment channels). To be eligible for the experiment, the students had to meet four criteria: be a student at Aarhus University, drink alcohol (answer “yes” to the question “Do you ever drink alcohol?”), sign up voluntarily, and provide a valid phone number. The participants signed up online via a questionnaire that asked them for background information, including their drinking behavior, and background characteristics such as gender, age, and parents’ education. The participants received a follow-up questionnaire 2 months after the intervention had started. To increase participation, we offered a lottery in which the students could win tickets to concerts or the movies.

The study sample size was determined using an effect size of 32 g (means of 176 and 199) alcohol per week, standard deviations of 100 g per week, statistical power of 0.80 at a 2-sided significance level of an alpha of 0.05. This power calculation resulted in 310 participants (50% in treatment and 50% in control group). As the participants were registered automatically, as soon as they answered the first questionnaire, and as there were no marginal cost of including more participants, we end up oversampling participants.

The intervention

The intervention included a link to an online questionnaire and to three short text blocks followed by videos. The students in the intervention group received a text message by SMS and an e-mail with the link. Furthermore, to remind the students about the content of the intervention, three text messages were sent out by SMS. While inspired by a validated self-affirmation questionnaire and a web-based intervention in the UK (Stock et al. 2009), the online questionnaire and short text blocks and videos was adapted to the local Danish university setting. The text messages send out by SMS were developed specifically for this study. Both the questionnaire, and use of text blocks, videos, and text messages draw on a well-established technique, nudging, that alters people’s behavior in a predictable way without using either prohibition or economic incentives. We used three sets of nudging tools—pre-commitment strategies, social norms, and reminders—aimed at enhancing the students’ self-control and self-image, and thereby reduce the alcohol consumption.

Pre-commitment strategies help people reflect on their behavior and commit to a certain course of action. Following Gollwitzer (1999), Hagger et al. (2012), and Norman et al. (2018), we included pre-commitment in the intervention by focusing on implementation intentions. The students were asked to specify “if–then” plans, targeting critical situations in the process toward the desired goal. Thus, the students were asked to write a strategy for how they plan to avoid getting drunk in a specific situation.

We used social norms in the intervention by using the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen 1988), which focuses on beliefs as the primary determinant of individual behavior. Inspired by Norman et al. (2018), the three text blocks and videos focused on the extent to which the students differed from one another relative to three beliefs about drinking behaviors at the university: (a) that social events at the university always include alcohol consumption, (b) that irresponsible drinking behavior does not necessarily affect ones studies, and ©) that most university students drink large amounts of alcohol. In the intervention, each belief is presented with a text block explaining why it is not necessarily true, in combination with a short video in which fellow students talk about their contradictory beliefs and explain what they do to avoid heavy drinking.

For the first belief, the students were introduced to alternative social activities at either the university or in the city, activities that do not include alcohol consumption. For the second belief, the students were presented with fact-based information about the negative effects of alcohol consumption on academic performance. For the third belief, the students were presented with studies showing that most university students drink responsibly. The students were also given reasons for avoiding heavy drinking.

The final element in the intervention was three reminders, sent as text messages to the students’ cell phones. The text messages are intended to remind the student of the specific elements of the intervention. For example, the text message referring to the “if–then” plan reads “It can sometimes be a good idea to make a plan for how much to drink when going to a party to avoid bad experiences. Have you thought about how much you want to drink at the next party you are going to?” Reminders have proven to be a simple but effective tool for combating procrastination, laziness, and forgetfulness, and for completing obligations (Sunstein 2014). For sending out reminders, the timing is essential. In our study, the messages were sent at 2:00 p.m. on three Fridays as this is the time where most student bars begin.

The web-based intervention included insights from self-affirmation theory (Steele 1988), which posits that individuals may protect their self-integrity by ignoring messages targeting their behavior. To prevent students from ignoring our text messages, we used self-affirmation manipulation to reduce defensive processing (Harris and Epton 2009). We introduced the self-affirmation manipulation through a version of the Values in Action Strength Scale, adapted for the Danish setting, and sent it to all participants (Napper et al. 2009). We hypothesized that, as they answered the questionnaire, the students would become more familiar with their positive strengths and thus be more open to suggestions for self-improvement.

Measures

We investigated the effects of the intervention on four individual outcomes related to alcohol consumption: (a) the number of times the student drank alcohol during the preceding month, (b) the number of times the student had been binge drinking alcohol during the preceding month (i.e., drinking more than five standard drinks at one event), (c) the total number of alcoholic drinks the student consumed on a typical day when drinking during the preceding month, and the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) score based on the three questions (Higgins-Biddle and Babor 2018). We include both the total AUDIT score and three items of the AUDIT score as outcome measures, as we believe that the specific item carries valuable information about drinking behavior that is masked by total score. All outcomes are measures 2 months after starting the intervention.

Besides information on alcohol, drinking patterns, and reasons for drinking, the survey included questions about age, gender, immigrant status, region of residence, parental education, self-rated health, smoking behavior, drug use, and their study program. We collected the additional background to (1) check for balance between intervention and control group, (2) study the attrition out for the experiment, and (3) compare the intervention sample with the average student at the university.

Statistical analysis

The randomization of participants into the intervention group and the control group by strata (i.e., gender and answers to the survey question “How often have you had five or more standard drinks at one event during the preceding month?”) were performed using the RAND function in SAS. The primary analysis where conducted using the REGRESS command in Stata. We used ordinary least square regression models, where the strata are included as controls. We estimated the average effect of the intervention and applied robust standard errors for each of the four outcomes. We conduct both intention-to-treat and complete case analysis for each outcome. This amounts to a total of eight tests. P values were computed accounting for multiple hypothesis testing (eight tests) using the method proposed in List et al. (2019). We use the Stata package ice (MICE, Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations) for imputations of missing data for the intention-to-treat analysis. Details on the imputation procedure is available in Appendix 2.

Results

Participation flow

In total, 961 students signed up for participation. Figure 1 illustrates the participant flow. The intervention was sent to 480 participants of whom 225 responded to the follow-up survey. In the control group, 284 participants responded to the follow-up survey. The complete case sample comprised 458 participants, implying a drop-out rate of 0.52. The intention-to-treatment sample includes all initial 961 participants.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Participant flow diagram

Descriptive statistics

As part of the analysis, we have performed detailed descriptive statistics between various relevant subsamples in three steps. In the first step, we began by testing the balance between the treatment and control group in the complete case sample. Table S3.1 in Appendix 3 presents the descriptive statistics on the complete case sample for the treatment and control group. The control group consist of 63% (160/254) women and the average age is 23.8 (SD 2.7) years. Furthermore, for the control group, average number of times drinking alcohol during the past month is 5.4 (SD 3.9), average number of times binge drinking during the past month is 3.2 (SD 3.0), and the average number of standard drinks on a day drinking alcohol during the past month is 5.8 (SD 2.7). The treatment and control group do not differ significantly on these characteristics.

In the second step of our descriptive analysis, we examined sample selection out of the experiment. This involved testing whether the group of students who answered the follow-up surveys differed from those who did not (results can be found in Table S3.2 in Appendix 3). Among the 29 variables included in the analysis, only two yielded p values less than .10, and none had a p value below 0.05. These findings suggest that students did not systematically opt out of the experiment.

As a third and final step of our descriptive analyses, we compared the participants in the experiment to the full population of students at Aarhus University. The results, available in Table S3.3 in Appendix 3, show that students who participated in the experiment are on average two years older that the average student in the full student population and 10% points more likely to be a woman.

Fidelity

Appendix 4 includes an implementation fidelity study, the results of which show that 56.7% (272/480) of the students in the intervention group completed the online intervention: 298 students (62.1%) opened the link and answered at least one question, 287 students (59.8%) answered all questions, and 272 students (56.7%) wrote down a behavioral strategy (see Table S4.1). While we only have counts on video views for the full group, we know that for each time the video was started, 71-77% of the students watched the video to the end; 45.6% (219/480) answered question on the treatment intensity linked to the follow-up questionnaire for the intervention group. They answered that they on average read 2.4 (SD 0.8) of the three text messages: only 6 students read zero text messages, 25 students read 1 message, 44 students read 2 messages, and the majority, 127 students, read 3 messages. The intervention made the majority of the students think more seriously about their alcohol consumption (146/219, 66.7%).

Alcohol consumption

Table 1 presents the effect of the intervention on four measures of alcohol consumption: the AUDIT score, the number of times the student had drunk alcohol during the preceding month, the number of times the student had been binge drinking during the preceding month, and the typical number of drinks on a day when the student was drinking alcohol within the preceding month. The estimations in Table 1 are conditioned on the strata, i.e., baseline information on gender at answers to the question “How often have you had five or more standard drinks at one event during the preceding month?”

Table 1 Effects on (1) Audit score, (2) Number of times drinking alcohol during the past month, (3) Number of times binge drinking during the past month, and (4) the typical number of drinking on a day drinking alcohol during the past month

In Table 1, we show the outcome means for the complete case sample for the treatment and the control group. For the outcomes AUDIT score and number of times drinking alcohol during the past week the outcome means where larger for the control group than the treatment group. For the outcome “number of times binge drinking during the past month” the outcome means were the same for treatment and control group and for the outcome measuring the typical number of drinks on a day drinking alcohol, the treatment group has a higher outcome mean than the control group. The regression results in Table 1 show that the intervention had a significant effect on the number of times the student drank alcohol during the preceding month. As the control and intervention group are similar at baseline (see Table S3.1 in Appendix 3), the estimated effects in Table 1 show that the effect of the intervention was a reduction in the number of times the students drank alcohol during the preceding month by almost one time (intention-to-treat estimate: mean −0.94, P = .03; compete case estimate: mean −0.87, P = .049), corresponding to a reduction of 17.0% for the intention-to-treat estimate and 15.6% for the complete case. This calculation is based on the average number of times the control group drank alcohol during the preceding month (5.58). We find no significant effect of the intervention on the three other measures of alcohol consumption, i.e., AUDIT score, binge drinking, and number of drinks on a day drinking alcohol.

To control for the students’ baseline drinking behavior and sociodemographic background, we also ran regression with 35 variables measuring, besides baseline drinking behavior, aspects such as age, ethnicity, parental educational background, experience with illegal substances, and variables for motivational factors (for complete list of variables see Table S5.1 in Appendix 5). Results of the regression model can also be found in Table S5.1, which shows similar results to the ones presented in Table 1. Furthermore, we found no effect on harmful alcohol consumption, see Table S5.2 in Appendix 5. On the basis of cost-effectiveness calculations, which can be found in Table S6.1 in Appendix 6, our intervention is relatively inexpensive. We estimated the cost for reducing the frequency of drinking by one per month is between 14 and 28 Euros.

Discussion

Main findings of this study

The aim of this intervention study is to test whether a web-based intervention, making use of various nudging tools, can reduce alcohol consumption among university students. The intervention included both a questionnaire, text including videos of peers telling, among other things, about life at the University without drinking alcohol, a written down strategy to opt out of drinking, and reminders of the intervention. As the elements in the intervention were not randomized, we can only evaluate the total effect of the intervention, including all elements. However, we find that most of the students received the intervention and that most of the students found liked or were neutral about the intervention. The intervention was tested on Danish university students who are among the heaviest drinkers in Europe. We conducted a stratified randomized controlled trial that provided a group of students with tools that helped them to make pre-commitment strategies to avoid getting drunk and to change their views on social norms that result in excessive alcohol intake. The intervention significantly reduced the number of times the students drank alcohol during the preceding month. The intention-to-treat estimate corresponds to a reduction of 17.0% in the number of times the students drank alcohol. The result is virtually the same in the complete case analysis and robust to multiple hypothesis testing.

While the number of times the students drank alcohol was reduced, the number of drinks when drinking was not changed. One explanation for this result could be that, as the question is retrospective, it may be easy for the students to remember if they did not drink alcohol, while it may be difficult to remember how many drinks they consumed. Thus, we might expect a higher measurement error in the variables measuring the number of drinks compared to whether they drank or not. However, another explanation could be that the students continue to consume the same number of drinks when they drink, or that the times they do not drink anymore were the times when they drank few drinks. In the latter case, we would expect to see an increase in the average number of drinks when they drink. In fact, the results show a small, but insignificant, increase in the number of drinks when they drink.

The study demonstrates the cost-effectiveness of an easy-to-implement web-based intervention, targeting the individuals’ engagement with and attitudes toward the dominant drinking culture, for reducing alcohol consumption among university students. Compared to most web-based health interventions, our intervention was relatively short (answering the questionnaire takes no more than 15 min).

While the intervention had only a significant effect on one of the drinking variables, i.e., number of times drinking alcohol per month, at a 2-month follow-up, these short-term changes indicate that providing students with simple psychological tools can enhance their abilities to go against the dominant drinking culture. Implementation of the study requires access to the students’ mobile number, which may not be possible in other settings. However, the intervention could be implemented by using any kind of platform, e.g., school managed IT platforms or through e-mails.

The overall result is important for making the environment among university students not only healthier but also more inclusive for students from different cultural backgrounds. Nonetheless, more research is needed for understanding the complex relationship between the many actors that create, maintain, and change drinking cultures in higher education. This study provides a first step toward making young people more aware of their preferences and thereby reduce their alcohol consumption.

What is already known on this topic

In most Western countries, excessive alcohol intake among university students is a cause of concern. Previous research has shown that inventions aimed at changing the social norms and behaviors that affect young people’s drinking can be both expensive and time consuming, as they typically involve targeting various actors beyond the young people, including family members, university authorities, policymakers, and the alcohol industry. In an intervention similar to ours, tested on British university students, a study by Norman et al. (2018) finds that the students that received treatment significantly reduced their frequency of units of alcohol consumed, binge drinking, and the AUDIT score compared to the control group. The study does not, however, report the impact on the number of times the students drank alcohol.

What this study adds

We demonstrated that simple nudging tools grounded in the theory of planned behavior can help young people in reducing their alcohol consumption. Our study hereby adds to the growing line of research utilizing nudging techniques, particularly within the framework of theory of planned behavior, aimed at altering young people’s drinking culture (Norman et al. 2018). We found no effect on binge drinking, the number of drinks per event and AUDIT score. The difference in our findings compared to Norman et al. (2018) can be due to the dominant drinking culture at the Danish university, which seems difficult to change, and the fact that we include university students at all levels and not only first-year students. While older students are likely to have developed appropriate protective strategies against alcohol interventions, first-year students are less embedded in the social norms and practices of the drinking culture and may be more likely to comply with the intervention (Tanner-Smith and Lipsey 2015; Moreira et al. 2009). Still, while we only find significant effects on the number of times the students drank alcohol, the average number of standard drinks on a day drinking alcohol during the past month was 5.8 for the control group, i.e., more than five drinks as is the definition of binge drinking. More research is needed to understand how web-based interventions work among young people with different drinking cultures.

Limitations

The study has limitations. First, it relies on self-reported measures. Although the intervention is based on validated measures, they are still subject to response bias and may potentially inflate the parameter estimate in the statistical analyses. In addition, the retrospective approach—asking the students about past alcohol-related events, such as those used in the study—has also been shown to be a potential source of bias (Järvinen et al. 2018). Second, as in all panel studies, our study suffers from attrition. Nonetheless, we find comparable estimates in the complete case and the intention-to-treat analysis. Third, our study involves a relatively small sample size. Therefore, we cannot provide more detailed information about the intervention and control groups by, for example, dividing the intervention group into subgroups with distinctive characteristics relative to more detailed patterns of drinking, specific fields of study, parental background, or other such subgroups.