Skip to main content
Log in

Temporal expectations mediated the repetition effect in a sequence in two ways

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The repetition priming effect generally refers to facilitated responding in instances where the same stimulus or a very similar stimulus repeats after an initial occurrence. Prior studies showed that the repetition priming effect was greater when repetitive stimuli appeared at expected times than when they appeared at less expected times. However, in addition to the expectation for repetition, the expectation for nonrepetitive stimuli may also arise in a sequence, especially after repetitive stimuli continuously appeared several times. This study was designed to further reveal how these two kinds of expectations influence the repetition effect in a sequence. Participants were asked to solve 3, 4 or 5 repetitive tasks followed by a novel task in the experimental group, a situation where the expectations for repetitive events arise in the first three serial positions but that for nonrepetitive events arise in the fourth, fifth and sixth serial positions, or were asked to continuously solve 3–5 repetitive tasks in the control group, a situation where only the expectation for repetitive events appears. The results showed that the repetition effect appeared steadily in the whole sequence for the control group, whereas the repetition effect appeared in the early serial positions but was reduced in the later serial position for the experimental group. The findings revealed the dual influences of temporal expectations on repetition effects in a sequence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data

The data and materials for experiment are available at https://osf.io/6hfxd/.

Code availability

Not applicable.

References

  • Arenas MC, Manzanedo C (2016) Novelty seeking. In: Zeigler-Hill V, Shackelford T (eds) Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences. Springer, Cham

    Google Scholar 

  • Asaridou SS, Takashima A, Dediu D, Hagoort P, McQueen JM (2016) Repetition suppression in the left inferior frontal gyrus predicts tone learning performance. Cereb Cortex 26(6):2728–2742

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Auksztulewicz R, Friston K (2016) Repetition suppression and its contextual determinants in predictive coding. Cortex 80:125–140

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bilalić M, Mcleod P, Gobet F (2008) Why good thoughts block better ones: the mechanism of the pernicious Einstellung (set) effect. Cognition 108(3):652–661

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bilalić M, McLeod P, Gobet F (2010) The mechanism of the Einstellung (set) effect: a pervasive source of cognitive bias. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 19(2):111–115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bilalić M, Mcleod P (2014) Why good thoughts block better ones. Sci Am 310(3):74–79

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chi RP, Snyder AW (2011) Facilitate insight by non-invasive brain stimulation. PLoS ONE 6(2):e16655

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Crooks NM, McNeil NM (2009) Increased practice with “set” problems hinders performance on the water jar task. In: Proceedings of the annual meeting of the cognitive science society, vol. 31, no. 31

  • DeCaro M (2016) Inducing mental set constrains procedural flexibility and conceptual understanding in mathematics. Mem Cognit 44(7):1138–1148

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis JJ, Reingold EM (2014) The Einstellung effect in anagram problem solving: evidence from eye movements. Front Psychol 5:679

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Feuerriegel Churches O, Coussens S, Keage H (2019) Temporal expectations modulate face image repetition suppression of early stimulus evoked event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia 122:76–87

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frings D (2011) The effects of group monitoring on fatigue-related Einstellung during mathematical problem solving. J Exp Psychol Appl 17(4):371–381

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Francis WS, Camacho A, Lara C (2014) Words translated in sentence contexts produce repetition priming in visual word comprehension and spoken word production. Mem Cognit 42:1143–1154

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grotheer M, Kovács G (2016) Can predictive coding explain repetition suppression? Cortex 80:113–124

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hsu YF, Hämäläinen JA, Waszak F (2014) Repetition suppression comprises both attention-independent and attention-dependent processes. Neuroimage 98:168–175

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huang F, Fan J, Luo J (2015) The neural basis of novelty and appropriateness in processing of creative chunk decomposition. Neuroimage 113:122–132

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huang F, Tang S, Hu Z (2018) Unconditional perseveration of the short-term mental set on chunk decomposition. Front Psychol 9:02568

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang F, Han L, Jiang Y, Li F, Luo J (2019) Neural adaptation and cognitive inflexibility in repeated problem-solving behaviors. Cortex 119:470–479

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jonathan G, Keren R, Nachshon M, Leonardo F (2012) “Mind the trap”: mindfulness practice reduces cognitive rigidity. PLoS ONE 7(5):e36206

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim H (2017) Brain regions that show repetition suppression and enhancement: a meta-analysis of 137 neuroimaging experiments. Hum Brain Mapp 38(4):1894–1913

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kluger AN, Siegfried Z, Ebstein RP (2002) A meta-analysis of the association between drd4 polymorphism and novelty seeking. Mol Psychiatry 7(7):712–717

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich G, Ohlsson S, Haider H, Rhenius D (1999) Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 25(6):1534–1555

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kovács G, Schweinberger SR (2016) Repetition suppression-an integrative view. Cortex 80:1–4

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lemaire P, Leclère M (2014) Strategy repetition in young and older adults: a study in arithmetic. Dev Psychol 50(2):460–468

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovett MC, Anderson JR (1996) History of success and current context in problem solving: combined influences on operator selection. Cognit Psychol 31(2):168–217

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luchins AS (1942) Mechanization in problem solving—the effect of Einstellung. Psychol Monogr 54(6):i–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luchins AS, Luchins EH (1969) Einstellung effect and group problem solving. J Soc Psychol 77(77):79–89

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin-Arevalo E, Kingstone A, Lupianez J (2013) Is “inhibition of return” due to the inhibition of the return of attention? Q J Exp Psychol 66(2):347–359

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason MF, Norton MI, Van Horn JD, Wegner DM, Grafton ST, Macrae CN (2007) Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315(5810):393–395

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mayrhauser L, Bergmann J, Crone J, Kronbichler M (2014) Neural repetition suppression: evidence for perceptual expectation in object-selective regions. Front Hum Neurosci 8:225

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Neroni MA, Vasconcelos LA, Crilly N (2017) Computer-based ‘mental set’ tasks: an alternative approach to studying design fixation. J Mech Des 139(7):071102

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Netser S, Zahar Y, Gutfreund Y (2011) Stimulus-specific adaptation: can it be a neural correlate of behavioral habituation? J Neurosci 31(49):17811–17820

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ollinger M, Jones G, Knoblich G (2008) Investigating the effect of mental set on insight problem solving. Exp Psychol 55(4):270–282

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ollinger M, Karin M, von Müller A, Carlos CS (2017) Expectation violation in political decision making: a psychological case study. Front Psychol 8:01761

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ricks TR, Turleyames KJ, Wiley J (2007) Effects of working memory capacity on mental set due to domain knowledge. Mem Cognit 35(6):1456–1462

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson IH, O’Connell RG (2010) Vigilant attention. In: Nobre AC, Coull JT (eds) Attention and time. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Rostalski SM, Amado C, Kovács G, Feuerriegel D (2020) Measures of repetition suppression in the fusiform face area are inflated by co-occurring effects of statistically learned visual associations. Cortex 131(1):123–136

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sapir A, Jackson K, Butler J, Paul MA, Abrams RA (2014) Inhibition of return affects contrast sensitivity. Q J Exp Psychol 67:1305–1316

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schillemans V, Luwel K, Ceulemans E, Onghena P, Verschaffel L (2012) The effect of single versus repeated previous strategy use on individuals’ subsequent strategy choice. Psychol Belgica 52(4):307–325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schillemans V, Luwel K, Bulté I, Onghena P, Verschaffel L (2010) The influence of previous strategy use on individuals’ subsequent strategy choice: findings from a numerosity judgement task. Psychol Belgica 49(4):191–205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz P, Searleman A (2002) Rigidity of thought and behavior: 100 years of research. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr 128(2):165–207

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Segaert K, Weber K, de Lange FP, Petersson KM, Hagoort P (2013) The suppression of repetition enhancement: a review of fMRI studies. Neuropsychologia 51(1):59–66

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheridan H, Reingold EM (2013) The mechanisms and boundary conditions of the einstellung effect in chess: evidence from eye movements. PLoS ONE 8(10):e75796

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sio U, Kotovsky K, Cagan J (2017) Interrupted: The roles of distributed effort and incubation in preventing fixation and generating problem solutions. Mem Cognit 45(4):553–565

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood J, Schooler JW (2015) The science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annu Rev Psychol 66(1):487–518

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Summerfield C, Trittschuh EH, Monti JM, Mesulam MM, Egner T (2008) Neural repetition suppression reflects fulfilled perceptual expectations. Nat Neurosci 11:1004–1006

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Szpunar, Karl K (2017) Directing the wandering mind. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 26(1):40–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang X, Pang J, Nie QY, Conci M, Luo J, Luo J (2015) Probing the cognitive mechanism of mental representational change during chunk decomposition: a parametric fMRI study. Cereb Cortex 26(7):2991–2999

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Todorovic A, van Ede F, Maris E, de Lange FP (2011) Prior expectation mediates neural adaptation to repeated sounds in the auditory cortex: an MEG study. J Neurosci 31(25):9118–9123

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vallée-Tourangeau F, Euden G, Hearn V (2011) Einstellung defused: interactivity and mental set. Q J Exp Psychol 64(10):1889–1895

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang Y, Xu Q, Liao S, Jia D (2017) Unfamiliar faces in recognition memory: spaced learning enhances subsequent recognition memory by reducing repetition priming. NeuroReport 28:872–878

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley J (1998) Expertise as mental set: the effects of domain knowledge in creative problem solving. Mem Cognit 26(4):716–730

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wu L, Knoblich G, Luo J (2013) The role of chunk tightness and chunk familiarity in problem solving: evidence from ERPs and fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 34(5):1173–1186

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wu X, He M, Zhou Y, Xiao J, Luo J (2017) Decomposing a chunk into its elements and reorganizing them as a new chunk: the two different subprocesses underlying insightful chunk decomposition. Front Psychol 8:2001

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Xue G, Mei LL, Chen CS (2011) Spaced learning enhances subsequent recognition memory by reducing neural repetition suppression. J Cognit Neurosci 23:1624–1633

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang Z, Yang K, Warren CM, Zhao G, Li P, Lei Y, Li H (2015) The influence of element type and crossed relation on the difficulty of chunk decomposition. Front Psychol 6:1025

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang Z, Luo Y, Wang C, Warren CM, Xia Q, Xing Q et al (2019) Identification and transformation difficulty in problem solving: Electrophysiological evidence from chunk decomposition. Biol Psychol 143:10–21

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This work was supported by funding programs from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant Nos. 31700956, 32060194); the China Postdoctoral Science Foundation (Grant Nos. 2018M632598 and 2018T110657); the Natural Science Foundation of Jiangxi, China (Grant No. 20181BAB214010); and the Postdoctoral Science Foundation of Jiangxi, China (Grant No. 2018KY28).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

HY: Investigation; Methodology; Formal analysis; Writing – original draft. ST: Formal analysis; Software; Writing – original draft. FH: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Project administration; Writing – original draft; Writing – review & editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Furong Huang.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this article.

Consent to participate

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Consent for publication

All of the authors listed have approved the manuscript.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants followed the principles of the 1964 Helsinki declaration and were approved by the Research Project Ethical Review Committee at Jiangxi Normal University of China.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Editors: Daniele Nardi (Ball State University), Franco Delogu (Lawrence Technological University); Reviewers: Fernando Luna (Universidad Nacional de Cordoba), Cengiz Acarturk (Jagiellonian University).

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yan, H., Tang, S. & Huang, F. Temporal expectations mediated the repetition effect in a sequence in two ways. Cogn Process 24, 463–469 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-023-01141-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-023-01141-5

Keywords

Navigation