Introduction

Tastes and smells play important roles in life and provide a vital tool for screening soluble chemicals for food evaluation, selection, and avoidance of potentially toxic substances [1]. A high comorbidity rate of olfactory and gustatory impairment was reported in the present studies [2, 3]. This rate varies widely among studies, which was partly attributed to the difficulties of distinguishing gustatory from retro-nasal smell produced in the oral cavity and nasopharynx [4, 5]. Swiecicki L et al. provided information on the prevalence of smell and taste disorders in a non-clinical population, which estimates range from 2.7 to 76.8% and 5 to 20% [6]. And an estimated 95% of taste disorders which are caused by impairment of smell rather than the gustatory loss were reported [7]. Although these two systems identify and mediate different qualities and their fibers are not found intermingled with each other at any peripheral site, some daily works such as perceiving flavor require intricate integration of taste and olfactory information [8]. Taste and odor afferent information were confirmed to converge at the orbitofrontal cortex in the center [9].

Evidence showed a high comorbidity rate of olfactory and gustatory dysfunction is based on the overlap of taste and odor representation in the brain center. Traditional theories propose that the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is the first area capable of responding to both olfactory and gustatory stimuli [10, 11]. The convergence of taste and smell information onto single neurons is realized in the caudal OFC (and immediately adjacent agranular insula) [10]. Recent studies consistently found that the gustatory cortex (GC) responds to both olfactory and gustatory stimuli. Taste perception have the ability to influence olfactory coding [12]. The insular cortex contributes to olfactory perception through overlapping taste and odor representation in it, which was based on the monosynaptic connections between the primary insular gustatory cortex and the primary piriform olfactory cortex [13]. These overlapping transduction mechanisms and connection pathways in the center proved that it is feasible for the olfactory function to affect gustatory function. However, those researches on the influences of smell on taste were mostly based on psychophysical chemical tests and functional imaging examinations. In psychophysical chemical tests, impaired olfactory function, especially the retro-nasal smell produced by volatile agents dissolved in the mouth by chewing and saliva, was usually roughly reported as changes in “taste perception” by subjects can lead to data distortion [14, 15]. The utility of functional imaging examinations was restricted by the delayed cortical response to the taste stimuli and unclear brain positioning.

Gustatory event-related potentials (gERPs) had been proven a relatively unbiased, reliable, and easy approach to objectively assess human taste function [16]. It derives from electroencephalographic (EEG) measures and recorded the early cortical response to gustatory stimulus in milliseconds. In the previous study in our group, we have used it for exploring the characteristics of EEG to gustatory stimulus in the old [17]. In this study, we compared the gERPs between participants with olfactory dysfunction and volunteers with normal olfaction. And through the correlation analysis of olfaction and gERPs, we explore the potential effect of olfactory function on early central cortical response to the gustatory stimulus.

Method and materials

Participants

Adult patients over 50 years old were recruited from the Smell and Taste Center, Beijing Anzhen Hospital, Capital Medical University. All the participants underwent complete physical examination, nasal endoscopy, sinus computed tomography (CT), head magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), and Sniffin’ Sticks tests. Demographics including age, sex, body mass index (BMI), smoking, and drinking were collected. Inclusion criteria included participants with normal gustatory function assessed by the three-drop test. Exclusion criteria included (1) patients with gustatory dysfunction with known causes (e.g., oral or dental morbidity, diabetes, neurological disorders, trauma, exposure to toxins or medications, congenital disorders, and idiopathic gustatory dysfunction); (2) participants diagnosed with cardiovascular disease, psychiatric disorder, or certain specific medical histories; (3) participants with tumor, cancer, or other chronic diseases that might influence the olfaction and cognition; and (4) participants without self-help skills. Subjects were not allowed to eat or smoke 2 h before the test. Finally, 33 participants were enrolled from the Smell and Taste Center of Beijing Anzhen Hospital, and they were divided into two groups (patients, 14 patients with the main complaint of olfactory dysfunction, and controls, 19 healthy volunteers). This study was approved by the Ethics Committee at Beijing Anzhen Hospital (Beijing, China, No. 2019YFE0116000). All participants provided written informed consent before participating in the study. The flow diagram of the study design was given in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart

Psychophysical olfactory test

Psychophysical testing of olfactory function was performed for each participant using Sniffin’ Sticks tests (Burghart, Gmbh, Wedel, Germany) which were consisted of odor threshold (OT), odor discrimination (OD), odor identification (OI), and the overall composite scores (TDI, a composite score of threshold, discrimination, and identification) [18]. Standard administration was performed according to the manufacturer’s instructions [18]. Felt-tip pens containing various odors were presented to the participants for testing. The pen’s tip was placed approximately 2 cm in front of both nostrils for bilateral stimulation. The test comprised three parts: OT, OD, and OI test. The overall results were combined and reported as TDI scores which ranged from 1 to 48, with higher scores indicating superior olfactory performance. Subjects with a TDI score greater than 30 are considered to be normosmia; otherwise, they are considered to be hyposmia. A TDI score less than or equal to 15 points was defined as anosmia [18].

Psychophysical gustatory test

Briefly, eight different concentrations of NaCl (salty), sucrose (sweet), citric acid (sour), and quinine hydrochloride (bitter) solutions were prepared for this test, with the minimum concentrations of sucrose (0.19 g/ml), NaCl (0.06 g/ml), citric acid (0.15 g/ml), and quinine hydrochloride (0.0012 g/ml) [17, 19,20,21]. Tastants drops were dripped onto the middle of the anterior two-thirds of the extended tongue by pipettes [19]. Subjects were asked to describe the taste (“salty,” “sweet,” “sour,” and “bitter”; multiple forced-choice tasks) and then rinse their mouths with tap water before the next tasks [17]. The score for each taste was ranged from 8 to 1, and 1 represented the maximum concentration, while 8 represented the minimum concentration. The score was only recorded when the taste was identified twice consecutively [17].

Electrophysiological gustatory test

Stimulation

NaCl (tastant, 0.3 M) solution was used as the gustatory stimulus and distilled water for rinsing off tastants [22]. Gustatory stimulation method has previously been described in detail in our previous study, which featured a computer-controlled gustometer (Emerging Tech Trans LLC, Hershey, Pennsylvania, USA) that delivered solutions through peristaltic pumps and Teflon tubes [17]. Through two separate channels, at a rate of 39 ml/min, liquids were kept at a similar temperature as the tongue (36 °C) and delivered into the mouth. And subjects were instructed to swallow or keep still by visual cues. A single cycle included five sessions, 2 s for relaxing, followed by 2 s for tasting, 5 s for stilling, 6 s for rinsing, and 5 s for swallowing in turn, and 60 cycles composed a test. Transistor–transistor logic pulses (+ 5 V, ≥ 10 ms) were used to trace the stimulus of tastant or distilled water, which were marked by EEG DINS of “1” and “2” on the EEG recording software for different stimuli [17].

Gustatory event-related potential recording and data analysis

Continuous EEG was recorded following the international 10–20 system using a sensor net with scalp electrodes and amplified by the Net Amps 400 (Electrical Geodesics Inc., Eugene, Oregon, USA) [17]. Four sites were recorded by surface electrodes for statistical analysis, as described by the previous study [23]: centro-parietal electrode PZ, electrode 16 (E16), electrode 21 (E21), and frontal electrode FZ. The detailed location of these electrodes was shown in Fig. 2. EEG data were processed by the MATLAB R2021a. GERPs from these four electrodes were analyzed and were proved representative of the cerebral gustative signal [16]. GERPs (n ≥ 50) were averaged by software, and then the components were detected. Three components were defined by peaks with P1, N1, and P2 being the first (positive), second (negative), and third (positive) peaks, respectively. The latency was defined as the time interval between the stimulus and the peak. The amplitude was defined as the distance between the peak and the baseline [17]. Latencies (ms) and amplitudes (µV) of each component were collected and analyzed. The detail of gERPs was shown in Fig. 3.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Location of electrodes

Fig. 3
figure 3

Grand averaged latencies and amplitudes of gustatory event-related potentials for patients and controls

Sample size calculation

This study was a case–control study. The olfactory dysfunction group was the experimental group; the normal olfactory group was the control group. The amplitudes and latency of peaks (P1, N1, P2) were the main observation indicator in the present study. According to a similar case–control study about olfactory function in two groups [24], it was estimated that the average amplitude of P1 of the control group was 4.5 ± 0.6uV and that of the experimental group was 3.6 ± 0.4uV. The difference between the two groups was 0.9uV. Set α = 0.05 (two-sided), β = 0.10, and r = 0.42, and the sample size of the experimental group and the control group calculated by PASS 15.0.5 software was 8 (control group) and 11 (experimental group), respectively.

Statistics analysis

Statistical analysis was performed with Statistical Product and Service Solutions 22.0 (SPSS 22.0, IBM Corporation, New York, NY, USA). Shapiro–Wilk test was used to verify the normal distribution. Continuous variables were presented as mean ± standard deviation (SD), and categorical variables were expressed as numerals (percentages). Two-tailed values of p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Demographic data and psychophysical gustatory or olfactory tests from patients or controls are compared by the Chi-square test. Psychophysical gustatory or olfactory test scores between patients and controls were compared by independent t test. Each component of gERPs (latency and amplitude from three peaks in three electrodes) between patients and controls was compared using an independent t test. Spearman analysis was used to explore the correlation between gERPs and data of psychophysical of in 33 subjects (r > 0 represents positive correlation, r < 0 represents negative correlation, the closer r value to 1, the stronger correlation is).

Results

Demographic data and psychophysical sensory tests

Demographic characteristics and data of psychophysical sensory function tests of the above groups were recorded in Table 1. There were no significant differences in demographic data between patients and controls. None had dental or neurological problems or any remarkable medical history. Patients with olfactory impairment had a significantly lower odor threshold score, odor discrimination score, odor identification, and threshold discrimination–identification (TDI) score. As to the gustatory performance assessed by the three-drop test, there was only a significant difference of sweet scores between the patients (5.40 ± 2.12) and the controls (7.37 ± 1.38) which was observed.

Table 1 Characteristics of participants

Gustatory event-related potentials

After averaging the gERPs recorded at four electrodes, three peaks (P1, N1, and P2) were observed for all participants. The mean value and standard deviation (SD) of latencies and amplitudes of P1, N1, and P2 of patients and controls were detailed in Table 2 and Fig. 3. To confirm that the gERPs were uniquely evoked by tastants, we examined a sequence of the potentials evoked by distilled water and found no significant peaks.

Table 2 Grand averaged latencies and amplitudes of gustatory event-related potentials for patients and controls

No significant difference was observed for P1 averaged latency and amplitude between groups. A significant difference can be observed at E16 (p = 0.033) for N1 averaged amplitude between groups. The patient group (− 2.55 ± 1.92 µV) demonstrated a larger amplitude of the N1 component compared to the control group (− 1.06 ± 1.58 µV). A significant difference can be observed at FZ for the P2 averaged amplitude between groups (p = 0.000). The patient group (5.63 ± 1.59 µV) demonstrated a larger amplitude of P2 component compared to the control group (2.32 ± 2.42 µV). The difference of P2 averaged latency between groups can be observed at the E21 (p = 0.040), and the control group (244.86 ± 25.40 ms) showed a longer latency compared to the patient group (226.17 ± 18.38 ms). All the details were recorded in Table 2.

Correlation analysis

The subjective gustatory performance of sweet showed a significant correlation with olfactory performance, which was listed in Table 3. The details of correlation analysis for gERPs and psychophysical olfactory test scores (TDI) were demonstrated in Table 4.

Table 3 Correlation analysis for sweet score and olfactory performance (TDI)
Table 4 Correlation analysis for gERPs (latency and amplitude) and olfactory performance

After the adjustment for age and gender, we observed a significant correlation between P1 averaged amplitude and odor threshold scores (T, r = 0.423, p = 0.035) at E16. The P1 averaged amplitude increased with the higher olfactory performance. For the N1 peak, we also observed a significant correlation between the averaged amplitude and odor threshold scores (T, r = 0.476, p = 0.016) at E16. For the P2 peak, at frontal electrode FZ, we observed a significant correlation between the averaged amplitude and olfactory test scores (T, r =  − 0.417, p = 0.038; D, r =  − 0.497, p = 0.011; I, r =  − 0.463, p = 0.020; and TDI, r =  − 0.478, p = 0.016). At E16, the averaged latency of P2 peak showed significant correlations with olfactory test scores (T, r = 0.421, p = 0.036; I, r = 0.564, p = 0.003; TDI, r = 0.474, p = 0.017). At E21, the P2 averaged latency also reported significant correlations with olfactory test scores (T, r = 0.444, p = 0.026; D, r = 0.436, p = 0.029; I, r = 0.602, p = 0.001; TDI, r = 0.523, p = 0.007). The P2 averaged amplitude increased with the impaired olfactory performance, while the averaged latency increased with the rising olfactory performance.

Discussion

The most obvious difference of gERPs’ components between the patients and the controls was observed at E16, E21, and the frontal electrode FZ. The averaged amplitude of the P1 component of gERPs reported a positive correlation to olfactory performance, while for P2 component, it demonstrated an opposite tendency to olfactory performance. And even the averaged latency of the P2 peak became longer with the increased odor scores.

A significant correlation between TDI scores and sweet scores was observed in this study, which was in line with gustatory complaints of patients with olfactory impairment in daily life. In previous studies, it was widely accepted that olfactory impairment patients would report a light gustatory decline, especially in the old, the patients with neurodegenerative diseases such as PD or AD, the patients with psychological diseases such as autism, and upper airway infection diseases such as COVID-19 [25,26,27,28,29,30]. Patients with olfactory dysfunction after upper respiratory tract infection reported that the degree of damage to the taste function was increased with the more severe the damage of olfactory function [31]. Behavioral studies confirmed that congruent odors could amplify taste intensity [32,33,34]. In daily life, people tend to confuse the “flavor” with the taste or the gustatory, but the flavor is the combination of taste and smell sense especially the retro-nasal olfactory sense when smells arise from inside the mouth during consumption, stimulating the epithelium upon exhalation [10, 35]. We believe that retro-nasal olfactory function should be responsible for some part of the complaint about gustatory function. Retro-nasal odors were reported to share processing circuitry commonly associated with taste, and the input was processed by a brain region responsible for taste processing, which was known as the insular gustatory cortex [35]. From previous studies, the insular cortex contributed to odor quality coding by representing the taste-like aspects of food odors. And odorized air activated the piriform cortex, OFC, and insula, among which, only the insular cortex responded to sweet taste [36]. The decreased sweet scores for olfactory-impaired patients may be attributed to the damage in overlapping regions of the insular cortex. A study explored the eating behavior of olfactory-impaired patients which also reported patients tended to use more sugar [37]. Impaired olfactory function weakened gustatory function gradually through the insular cortex, which was reflected on the sweet score.

A significant correlation between TDI scores and gERPs (including amplitudes and latencies) can be reported in this study. For the P1 and N1 components, their averaged amplitudes increased with odor threshold scores. Previous studies hold that the P1 component was the first gustatory response, namely, at around 100 ms, which was located in the transition between temporal operculum and insula, while the N1 peak was located in the middle insula, and they seem to reflect the activity of the primary gustatory area [38, 39]. And the fronto-central maximum for early amplitudes P1 (and also N1) was considered to relate specifically to gustatory function [16]. A Faurion et al. held that N1 was determined by exogenous stimulus properties (concentration or quality) [39]. In this study, averaged amplitudes of P1 and N1 decreased with the impaired olfaction, which may reflect a slight adverse effect on the primary gustatory area related to impaired olfaction. But the P2 component turned out a different tendency compared to the P1 component and the sweet score. For the P2 averaged amplitude recorded at FZ, the patient group reported a huger one than the control group. For the averaged latency, P2 was in a line with the increased olfactory performance, especially at E16. Olfactory-impaired patients reported a relatively huge averaged amplitude of P2, which is seemly opposed to normal speculates and the complaint of patients. Chie Mizoguchi et al. proposed P2 was commonly associated with the inferior insula, whose region was likely to be involved in gustatory cognitive processing, and it reflected higher gustatory functions [22]. In early studies, the posterior part of the insula has a role of more secondary gustatory response related to the subjective interception of the stimulus, and the P2 component may be dependent on endogenous processes reflecting the subjective significance of the stimuli, which could be in line with the late positive component (LPC) in olfactory event-related potential [39]. A study exploring the olfactory event-related potential of mild cognitive impairment patients found N1 amplitude was reduced, while the LPC amplitude was increased, and it proposed that a degree of perceptive compensation can occur when a sensorial function was impaired [40]. We reasonably speculated that a compensation mechanism works in the gustatory sensory system; a degree of compensation of the secondary gustatory response can occur in gustatory processing of olfactory-impaired patients, which was reflected on the P2 component.

Furthermore, A Faurion et al. reported a significant lateralized integration between gustatory and olfactory information, in the inferior part of the insula, which may be interpreted as a second-order gustatory area related to secondary processing [41]. Another PET study also reported a left insular activation in response to gustatory stimulation for right-handers [42]. In this study, we also noticed the phenomenon that the gEPRs recorded at the left showed the more obvious components of gEPRs compared to the records at the right, which supported the previous concept of lateralized integration between gustatory and olfactory information.

Conclusion

In this study, we explored not only the difference of three components of gERPs between the olfactory dysfunction population and normal olfactory function adults but also the association between gustatory and olfactory function. The P1 amplitudes reflected primary gustatory area which was decreased with the declined olfactory performance, while the P2 amplitudes related to the secondary gustatory processing or higher gustatory processing increased with better olfactory performance. These findings are conducive to explore the impairment of gustatory dysfunction in the olfactory dysfunction population, which may give more information about the interaction of gustatory and olfactory function.